Serveur d'exploration sur les dispositifs haptiques

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

The coordination of two‐effector actions: Spoon‐feeding and intermanual prehension

Identifieur interne : 004D05 ( Istex/Corpus ); précédent : 004D04; suivant : 004D06

The coordination of two‐effector actions: Spoon‐feeding and intermanual prehension

Auteurs : Andrew Churchill ; Stefan Vogt ; Brian Hopkins

Source :

RBID : ISTEX:9B8B2ABCD948E78A3EEB5AD763F87DA519B7F5A5

Abstract

The aim of this study was to assess whether kinematic characteristics of unimanual (one‐effector) prehension movements can also be found in tasks where the transport and grasp components are distributed across separate effectors. Expt 1 compared prehension with spoon‐feeding and showed that a number of kinematic landmarks, such as time to peak aperture, were similar. An unexpected difference between the tasks was that the onsets of the transport and grasp components in the two‐effector task were not always synchronized. To control for a number of differences between the tasks used in Expt 1, such as the role of haptic and visual information, Expt 2 compared unimanual with intermanual prehension, which involved passing an object from one hand to the other. The results were consistent with those found in Expt 1. In particular, time to peak aperture was similar over the different tasks, despite a lack of consistent coupling between the components at movement onset. We conclude that the one‐ and two‐effector tasks pose largely the same task constraints and are coordinated in a similar way. Intermanual prehension tasks thus provide a useful experimental tool for manipulations that are difficult or impossible to perform with unimanual reaching and grasping.

Url:
DOI: 10.1348/000712699161404

Links to Exploration step

ISTEX:9B8B2ABCD948E78A3EEB5AD763F87DA519B7F5A5

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI wicri:istexFullTextTei="biblStruct">
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">The coordination of two‐effector actions: Spoon‐feeding and intermanual prehension</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Churchill, Andrew" sort="Churchill, Andrew" uniqKey="Churchill A" first="Andrew" last="Churchill">Andrew Churchill</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vogt, Stefan" sort="Vogt, Stefan" uniqKey="Vogt S" first="Stefan" last="Vogt">Stefan Vogt</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hopkins, Brian" sort="Hopkins, Brian" uniqKey="Hopkins B" first="Brian" last="Hopkins">Brian Hopkins</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">ISTEX</idno>
<idno type="RBID">ISTEX:9B8B2ABCD948E78A3EEB5AD763F87DA519B7F5A5</idno>
<date when="1999" year="1999">1999</date>
<idno type="doi">10.1348/000712699161404</idno>
<idno type="url">https://api.istex.fr/document/9B8B2ABCD948E78A3EEB5AD763F87DA519B7F5A5/fulltext/pdf</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Istex/Corpus">004D05</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">The coordination of two‐effector actions: Spoon‐feeding and intermanual prehension</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Churchill, Andrew" sort="Churchill, Andrew" uniqKey="Churchill A" first="Andrew" last="Churchill">Andrew Churchill</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vogt, Stefan" sort="Vogt, Stefan" uniqKey="Vogt S" first="Stefan" last="Vogt">Stefan Vogt</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hopkins, Brian" sort="Hopkins, Brian" uniqKey="Hopkins B" first="Brian" last="Hopkins">Brian Hopkins</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<monogr></monogr>
<series>
<title level="j">British Journal of Psychology</title>
<idno type="ISSN">0007-1269</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">2044-8295</idno>
<imprint>
<publisher>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</publisher>
<pubPlace>Oxford, UK</pubPlace>
<date type="published" when="1999-05">1999-05</date>
<biblScope unit="volume">90</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="issue">2</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" from="271">271</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" to="290">290</biblScope>
</imprint>
<idno type="ISSN">0007-1269</idno>
</series>
<idno type="istex">9B8B2ABCD948E78A3EEB5AD763F87DA519B7F5A5</idno>
<idno type="DOI">10.1348/000712699161404</idno>
<idno type="ArticleID">BJOP22</idno>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
<seriesStmt>
<idno type="ISSN">0007-1269</idno>
</seriesStmt>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
<langUsage>
<language ident="en">en</language>
</langUsage>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">The aim of this study was to assess whether kinematic characteristics of unimanual (one‐effector) prehension movements can also be found in tasks where the transport and grasp components are distributed across separate effectors. Expt 1 compared prehension with spoon‐feeding and showed that a number of kinematic landmarks, such as time to peak aperture, were similar. An unexpected difference between the tasks was that the onsets of the transport and grasp components in the two‐effector task were not always synchronized. To control for a number of differences between the tasks used in Expt 1, such as the role of haptic and visual information, Expt 2 compared unimanual with intermanual prehension, which involved passing an object from one hand to the other. The results were consistent with those found in Expt 1. In particular, time to peak aperture was similar over the different tasks, despite a lack of consistent coupling between the components at movement onset. We conclude that the one‐ and two‐effector tasks pose largely the same task constraints and are coordinated in a similar way. Intermanual prehension tasks thus provide a useful experimental tool for manipulations that are difficult or impossible to perform with unimanual reaching and grasping.</div>
</front>
</TEI>
<istex>
<corpusName>wiley</corpusName>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>Andrew Churchill</name>
<affiliations>
<json:string>Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK</json:string>
</affiliations>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>Stefan Vogt</name>
<affiliations>
<json:string>Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK</json:string>
</affiliations>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>Brian Hopkins</name>
<affiliations>
<json:string>Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK</json:string>
</affiliations>
</json:item>
</author>
<articleId>
<json:string>BJOP22</json:string>
</articleId>
<language>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</language>
<abstract>The aim of this study was to assess whether kinematic characteristics of unimanual (one‐effector) prehension movements can also be found in tasks where the transport and grasp components are distributed across separate effectors. Expt 1 compared prehension with spoon‐feeding and showed that a number of kinematic landmarks, such as time to peak aperture, were similar. An unexpected difference between the tasks was that the onsets of the transport and grasp components in the two‐effector task were not always synchronized. To control for a number of differences between the tasks used in Expt 1, such as the role of haptic and visual information, Expt 2 compared unimanual with intermanual prehension, which involved passing an object from one hand to the other. The results were consistent with those found in Expt 1. In particular, time to peak aperture was similar over the different tasks, despite a lack of consistent coupling between the components at movement onset. We conclude that the one‐ and two‐effector tasks pose largely the same task constraints and are coordinated in a similar way. Intermanual prehension tasks thus provide a useful experimental tool for manipulations that are difficult or impossible to perform with unimanual reaching and grasping.</abstract>
<qualityIndicators>
<score>7.376</score>
<pdfVersion>1.3</pdfVersion>
<pdfPageSize>432.283 x 627.591 pts</pdfPageSize>
<refBibsNative>false</refBibsNative>
<keywordCount>0</keywordCount>
<abstractCharCount>1271</abstractCharCount>
<pdfWordCount>7771</pdfWordCount>
<pdfCharCount>45209</pdfCharCount>
<pdfPageCount>20</pdfPageCount>
<abstractWordCount>198</abstractWordCount>
</qualityIndicators>
<title>The coordination of two‐effector actions: Spoon‐feeding and intermanual prehension</title>
<genre.original>
<json:string>article</json:string>
</genre.original>
<genre>
<json:string>article</json:string>
</genre>
<host>
<volume>90</volume>
<publisherId>
<json:string>BJOP</json:string>
</publisherId>
<pages>
<total>20</total>
<last>290</last>
<first>271</first>
</pages>
<issn>
<json:string>0007-1269</json:string>
</issn>
<issue>2</issue>
<genre>
<json:string>journal</json:string>
</genre>
<language>
<json:string>unknown</json:string>
</language>
<eissn>
<json:string>2044-8295</json:string>
</eissn>
<title>British Journal of Psychology</title>
<doi>
<json:string>10.1111/(ISSN)2044-8295</json:string>
</doi>
</host>
<publicationDate>1999</publicationDate>
<copyrightDate>1999</copyrightDate>
<doi>
<json:string>10.1348/000712699161404</json:string>
</doi>
<id>9B8B2ABCD948E78A3EEB5AD763F87DA519B7F5A5</id>
<score>1</score>
<fulltext>
<json:item>
<original>true</original>
<mimetype>application/pdf</mimetype>
<extension>pdf</extension>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/9B8B2ABCD948E78A3EEB5AD763F87DA519B7F5A5/fulltext/pdf</uri>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>application/zip</mimetype>
<extension>zip</extension>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/9B8B2ABCD948E78A3EEB5AD763F87DA519B7F5A5/fulltext/zip</uri>
</json:item>
<istex:fulltextTEI uri="https://api.istex.fr/document/9B8B2ABCD948E78A3EEB5AD763F87DA519B7F5A5/fulltext/tei">
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">The coordination of two‐effector actions: Spoon‐feeding and intermanual prehension</title>
<respStmt xml:id="ISTEX-API" resp="Références bibliographiques récupérées via GROBID" name="ISTEX-API (INIST-CNRS)"></respStmt>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<authority>ISTEX</authority>
<publisher>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</publisher>
<pubPlace>Oxford, UK</pubPlace>
<availability>
<p>WILEY</p>
</availability>
<date>1999</date>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct type="inbook">
<analytic>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">The coordination of two‐effector actions: Spoon‐feeding and intermanual prehension</title>
<author>
<persName>
<forename type="first">Andrew</forename>
<surname>Churchill</surname>
</persName>
<note type="correspondence">
<p>Correspondence: Andrew Churchill or Stefan Vogt, Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster LA1 4YF, UK (e‐mail: ).</p>
</note>
<affiliation>Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<persName>
<forename type="first">Stefan</forename>
<surname>Vogt</surname>
</persName>
<affiliation>Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<persName>
<forename type="first">Brian</forename>
<surname>Hopkins</surname>
</persName>
<affiliation>Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<monogr>
<title level="j">British Journal of Psychology</title>
<idno type="pISSN">0007-1269</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">2044-8295</idno>
<idno type="DOI">10.1111/(ISSN)2044-8295</idno>
<imprint>
<publisher>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</publisher>
<pubPlace>Oxford, UK</pubPlace>
<date type="published" when="1999-05"></date>
<biblScope unit="volume">90</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="issue">2</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" from="271">271</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" to="290">290</biblScope>
</imprint>
</monogr>
<idno type="istex">9B8B2ABCD948E78A3EEB5AD763F87DA519B7F5A5</idno>
<idno type="DOI">10.1348/000712699161404</idno>
<idno type="ArticleID">BJOP22</idno>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<creation>
<date>1999</date>
</creation>
<langUsage>
<language ident="en">en</language>
</langUsage>
<abstract xml:lang="en">
<p>The aim of this study was to assess whether kinematic characteristics of unimanual (one‐effector) prehension movements can also be found in tasks where the transport and grasp components are distributed across separate effectors. Expt 1 compared prehension with spoon‐feeding and showed that a number of kinematic landmarks, such as time to peak aperture, were similar. An unexpected difference between the tasks was that the onsets of the transport and grasp components in the two‐effector task were not always synchronized. To control for a number of differences between the tasks used in Expt 1, such as the role of haptic and visual information, Expt 2 compared unimanual with intermanual prehension, which involved passing an object from one hand to the other. The results were consistent with those found in Expt 1. In particular, time to peak aperture was similar over the different tasks, despite a lack of consistent coupling between the components at movement onset. We conclude that the one‐ and two‐effector tasks pose largely the same task constraints and are coordinated in a similar way. Intermanual prehension tasks thus provide a useful experimental tool for manipulations that are difficult or impossible to perform with unimanual reaching and grasping.</p>
</abstract>
</profileDesc>
<revisionDesc>
<change when="1999-05">Published</change>
<change xml:id="refBibs-istex" who="#ISTEX-API" when="2016-4-23">References added</change>
</revisionDesc>
</teiHeader>
</istex:fulltextTEI>
<json:item>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>text/plain</mimetype>
<extension>txt</extension>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/9B8B2ABCD948E78A3EEB5AD763F87DA519B7F5A5/fulltext/txt</uri>
</json:item>
</fulltext>
<metadata>
<istex:metadataXml wicri:clean="Wiley component found">
<istex:xmlDeclaration>version="1.0" encoding="UTF-8" standalone="yes"</istex:xmlDeclaration>
<istex:document>
<component version="2.0" type="serialArticle" xml:lang="en">
<header>
<publicationMeta level="product">
<publisherInfo>
<publisherName>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</publisherName>
<publisherLoc>Oxford, UK</publisherLoc>
</publisherInfo>
<doi origin="wiley" registered="yes">10.1111/(ISSN)2044-8295</doi>
<issn type="print">0007-1269</issn>
<issn type="electronic">2044-8295</issn>
<idGroup>
<id type="product" value="BJOP"></id>
<id type="publisherDivision" value="ST"></id>
</idGroup>
<titleGroup>
<title type="main" sort="BRITISH JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY">British Journal of Psychology</title>
</titleGroup>
</publicationMeta>
<publicationMeta level="part" position="05102">
<doi origin="wiley">10.1111/bjop.1999.90.issue-2</doi>
<numberingGroup>
<numbering type="journalVolume" number="90">90</numbering>
<numbering type="journalIssue" number="2">2</numbering>
</numberingGroup>
<coverDate startDate="1999-05">May 1999</coverDate>
</publicationMeta>
<publicationMeta level="unit" type="article" position="8" status="forIssue">
<doi origin="wiley">10.1348/000712699161404</doi>
<idGroup>
<id type="unit" value="BJOP22"></id>
</idGroup>
<countGroup>
<count type="pageTotal" number="20"></count>
</countGroup>
<titleGroup>
<title type="tocHeading1">Original Article</title>
</titleGroup>
<copyright>1999 The British Psychological Society</copyright>
<eventGroup>
<event type="xmlConverted" agent="Converter:BPG_TO_WML3G version:2.3.27 mode:FullText" date="2010-12-08"></event>
<event type="firstOnline" date="2010-12-24"></event>
<event type="publishedOnlineFinalForm" date="2010-12-24"></event>
<event type="xmlConverted" agent="Converter:WILEY_ML3G_TO_WILEY_ML3GV2 version:3.8.8" date="2014-02-27"></event>
<event type="xmlConverted" agent="Converter:WML3G_To_WML3G version:4.1.7 mode:FullText,remove_FC" date="2014-10-15"></event>
</eventGroup>
<numberingGroup>
<numbering type="pageFirst" number="271">271</numbering>
<numbering type="pageLast" number="290">290</numbering>
</numberingGroup>
<correspondenceTo>Andrew Churchill or Stefan Vogt, Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster LA1 4YF, UK (e‐mail:
<email>a.churchill@lancaster.ac.uk or s.vogt@lancaster.ac.uk</email>
).</correspondenceTo>
<linkGroup>
<link type="toTypesetVersion" href="file:BJOP.BJOP22.pdf"></link>
</linkGroup>
</publicationMeta>
<contentMeta>
<countGroup>
<count type="referenceTotal" number="0"></count>
<count type="linksCrossRef" number="4"></count>
</countGroup>
<titleGroup>
<title type="main">The coordination of two‐effector actions: Spoon‐feeding and intermanual prehension</title>
</titleGroup>
<creators>
<creator creatorRole="author" xml:id="cr1" affiliationRef="#a1" corresponding="yes">
<personName>
<givenNames>Andrew</givenNames>
<familyName>Churchill</familyName>
</personName>
</creator>
<creator creatorRole="author" xml:id="cr2" affiliationRef="#a1">
<personName>
<givenNames>Stefan</givenNames>
<familyName>Vogt</familyName>
</personName>
</creator>
<creator creatorRole="author" xml:id="cr3" affiliationRef="#a1">
<personName>
<givenNames>Brian</givenNames>
<familyName>Hopkins</familyName>
</personName>
</creator>
</creators>
<affiliationGroup>
<affiliation xml:id="a1" countryCode="GB">
<unparsedAffiliation>Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK</unparsedAffiliation>
</affiliation>
</affiliationGroup>
<abstractGroup>
<abstract type="main" xml:lang="en">
<p>The aim of this study was to assess whether kinematic characteristics of unimanual (one‐effector) prehension movements can also be found in tasks where the transport and grasp components are distributed across separate effectors. Expt 1 compared prehension with spoon‐feeding and showed that a number of kinematic landmarks, such as time to peak aperture, were similar. An unexpected difference between the tasks was that the onsets of the transport and grasp components in the two‐effector task were not always synchronized. To control for a number of differences between the tasks used in Expt 1, such as the role of haptic and visual information, Expt 2 compared unimanual with intermanual prehension, which involved passing an object from one hand to the other. The results were consistent with those found in Expt 1. In particular, time to peak aperture was similar over the different tasks, despite a lack of consistent coupling between the components at movement onset. We conclude that the one‐ and two‐effector tasks pose largely the same task constraints and are coordinated in a similar way. Intermanual prehension tasks thus provide a useful experimental tool for manipulations that are difficult or impossible to perform with unimanual reaching and grasping.</p>
</abstract>
</abstractGroup>
</contentMeta>
</header>
</component>
</istex:document>
</istex:metadataXml>
<mods version="3.6">
<titleInfo lang="en">
<title>The coordination of two‐effector actions: Spoon‐feeding and intermanual prehension</title>
</titleInfo>
<titleInfo type="alternative" contentType="CDATA" lang="en">
<title>The coordination of two‐effector actions: Spoon‐feeding and intermanual prehension</title>
</titleInfo>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Andrew</namePart>
<namePart type="family">Churchill</namePart>
<affiliation>Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK</affiliation>
<description>Correspondence: Andrew Churchill or Stefan Vogt, Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster LA1 4YF, UK (e‐mail: ).</description>
<role>
<roleTerm type="text">author</roleTerm>
</role>
</name>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Stefan</namePart>
<namePart type="family">Vogt</namePart>
<affiliation>Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK</affiliation>
<role>
<roleTerm type="text">author</roleTerm>
</role>
</name>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Brian</namePart>
<namePart type="family">Hopkins</namePart>
<affiliation>Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, UK</affiliation>
<role>
<roleTerm type="text">author</roleTerm>
</role>
</name>
<typeOfResource>text</typeOfResource>
<genre type="article" displayLabel="article"></genre>
<originInfo>
<publisher>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</publisher>
<place>
<placeTerm type="text">Oxford, UK</placeTerm>
</place>
<dateIssued encoding="w3cdtf">1999-05</dateIssued>
<copyrightDate encoding="w3cdtf">1999</copyrightDate>
</originInfo>
<language>
<languageTerm type="code" authority="rfc3066">en</languageTerm>
<languageTerm type="code" authority="iso639-2b">eng</languageTerm>
</language>
<physicalDescription>
<internetMediaType>text/html</internetMediaType>
</physicalDescription>
<abstract lang="en">The aim of this study was to assess whether kinematic characteristics of unimanual (one‐effector) prehension movements can also be found in tasks where the transport and grasp components are distributed across separate effectors. Expt 1 compared prehension with spoon‐feeding and showed that a number of kinematic landmarks, such as time to peak aperture, were similar. An unexpected difference between the tasks was that the onsets of the transport and grasp components in the two‐effector task were not always synchronized. To control for a number of differences between the tasks used in Expt 1, such as the role of haptic and visual information, Expt 2 compared unimanual with intermanual prehension, which involved passing an object from one hand to the other. The results were consistent with those found in Expt 1. In particular, time to peak aperture was similar over the different tasks, despite a lack of consistent coupling between the components at movement onset. We conclude that the one‐ and two‐effector tasks pose largely the same task constraints and are coordinated in a similar way. Intermanual prehension tasks thus provide a useful experimental tool for manipulations that are difficult or impossible to perform with unimanual reaching and grasping.</abstract>
<relatedItem type="host">
<titleInfo>
<title>British Journal of Psychology</title>
</titleInfo>
<genre type="journal">journal</genre>
<identifier type="ISSN">0007-1269</identifier>
<identifier type="eISSN">2044-8295</identifier>
<identifier type="DOI">10.1111/(ISSN)2044-8295</identifier>
<identifier type="PublisherID">BJOP</identifier>
<part>
<date>1999</date>
<detail type="volume">
<caption>vol.</caption>
<number>90</number>
</detail>
<detail type="issue">
<caption>no.</caption>
<number>2</number>
</detail>
<extent unit="pages">
<start>271</start>
<end>290</end>
<total>20</total>
</extent>
</part>
</relatedItem>
<identifier type="istex">9B8B2ABCD948E78A3EEB5AD763F87DA519B7F5A5</identifier>
<identifier type="DOI">10.1348/000712699161404</identifier>
<identifier type="ArticleID">BJOP22</identifier>
<accessCondition type="use and reproduction" contentType="copyright">1999 The British Psychological Society</accessCondition>
<recordInfo>
<recordContentSource>WILEY</recordContentSource>
<recordOrigin>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</recordOrigin>
</recordInfo>
</mods>
</metadata>
<serie></serie>
</istex>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Ticri/CIDE/explor/HapticV1/Data/Istex/Corpus
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 004D05 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Istex/Corpus/biblio.hfd -nk 004D05 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Ticri/CIDE
   |area=    HapticV1
   |flux=    Istex
   |étape=   Corpus
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     ISTEX:9B8B2ABCD948E78A3EEB5AD763F87DA519B7F5A5
   |texte=   The coordination of two‐effector actions: Spoon‐feeding and intermanual prehension
}}

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.23.
Data generation: Mon Jun 13 01:09:46 2016. Site generation: Wed Mar 6 09:54:07 2024