Serveur d'exploration sur l'Université de Trèves

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

Feelings of Revenge, Retaliation Motive, and Posttraumatic Stress Reactions in Crime Victims

Identifieur interne : 001411 ( Istex/Corpus ); précédent : 001410; suivant : 001412

Feelings of Revenge, Retaliation Motive, and Posttraumatic Stress Reactions in Crime Victims

Auteurs : Ulrich Orth ; Leo Montada ; Andreas Maercker

Source :

RBID : ISTEX:C2C24723866933E407675FBF60EDFBCF8347D9C4

Abstract

Individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are often said to experience strong feelings of revenge. However, there is a need for confirmatory empirical studies. Therefore, in a study of 174 victims of violent crimes, the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions was investigated. Feelings of revenge were correlated with intrusion and hyperarousal but not with avoidance. Feelings of revenge explained incremental variance of intrusion and hyperarousal when the variance explained by victimological variables was controlled. The retaliation motive implied in feelings of revenge did not account for the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions. However, the relation was moderated by the time since victimization. Therefore, feelings of revenge must presumably be regarded as a maladaptive coping reaction to experienced injustice, but not in the first period after victimization.

Url:
DOI: 10.1177/0886260505282286

Links to Exploration step

ISTEX:C2C24723866933E407675FBF60EDFBCF8347D9C4

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI wicri:istexFullTextTei="biblStruct">
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">Feelings of Revenge, Retaliation Motive, and Posttraumatic Stress Reactions in Crime Victims</title>
<author wicri:is="90%">
<name sortKey="Orth, Ulrich" sort="Orth, Ulrich" uniqKey="Orth U" first="Ulrich" last="Orth">Ulrich Orth</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>University of Berne,</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>E-mail: ulrich.orth@psy.unibe.ch</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author wicri:is="90%">
<name sortKey="Montada, Leo" sort="Montada, Leo" uniqKey="Montada L" first="Leo" last="Montada">Leo Montada</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>University of Trier</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author wicri:is="90%">
<name sortKey="Maercker, Andreas" sort="Maercker, Andreas" uniqKey="Maercker A" first="Andreas" last="Maercker">Andreas Maercker</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>University of Trier</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">ISTEX</idno>
<idno type="RBID">ISTEX:C2C24723866933E407675FBF60EDFBCF8347D9C4</idno>
<date when="2006" year="2006">2006</date>
<idno type="doi">10.1177/0886260505282286</idno>
<idno type="url">https://api.istex.fr/document/C2C24723866933E407675FBF60EDFBCF8347D9C4/fulltext/pdf</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Istex/Corpus">001411</idno>
<idno type="wicri:explorRef" wicri:stream="Istex" wicri:step="Corpus" wicri:corpus="ISTEX">001411</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">Feelings of Revenge, Retaliation Motive, and Posttraumatic Stress Reactions in Crime Victims</title>
<author wicri:is="90%">
<name sortKey="Orth, Ulrich" sort="Orth, Ulrich" uniqKey="Orth U" first="Ulrich" last="Orth">Ulrich Orth</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>University of Berne,</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>E-mail: ulrich.orth@psy.unibe.ch</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author wicri:is="90%">
<name sortKey="Montada, Leo" sort="Montada, Leo" uniqKey="Montada L" first="Leo" last="Montada">Leo Montada</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>University of Trier</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author wicri:is="90%">
<name sortKey="Maercker, Andreas" sort="Maercker, Andreas" uniqKey="Maercker A" first="Andreas" last="Maercker">Andreas Maercker</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>University of Trier</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<monogr></monogr>
<series>
<title level="j">Journal of Interpersonal Violence</title>
<idno type="ISSN">0886-2605</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1552-6518</idno>
<imprint>
<publisher>Sage Publications</publisher>
<pubPlace>Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</pubPlace>
<date type="published" when="2006-02">2006-02</date>
<biblScope unit="volume">21</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="issue">2</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" from="229">229</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" to="243">243</biblScope>
</imprint>
<idno type="ISSN">0886-2605</idno>
</series>
<idno type="istex">C2C24723866933E407675FBF60EDFBCF8347D9C4</idno>
<idno type="DOI">10.1177/0886260505282286</idno>
<idno type="ArticleID">10.1177_0886260505282286</idno>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
<seriesStmt>
<idno type="ISSN">0886-2605</idno>
</seriesStmt>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
<langUsage>
<language ident="en">en</language>
</langUsage>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">Individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are often said to experience strong feelings of revenge. However, there is a need for confirmatory empirical studies. Therefore, in a study of 174 victims of violent crimes, the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions was investigated. Feelings of revenge were correlated with intrusion and hyperarousal but not with avoidance. Feelings of revenge explained incremental variance of intrusion and hyperarousal when the variance explained by victimological variables was controlled. The retaliation motive implied in feelings of revenge did not account for the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions. However, the relation was moderated by the time since victimization. Therefore, feelings of revenge must presumably be regarded as a maladaptive coping reaction to experienced injustice, but not in the first period after victimization.</div>
</front>
</TEI>
<istex>
<corpusName>sage</corpusName>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>Ulrich Orth</name>
<affiliations>
<json:string>University of Berne,</json:string>
<json:string>E-mail: ulrich.orth@psy.unibe.ch</json:string>
</affiliations>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>Leo Montada</name>
<affiliations>
<json:string>University of Trier</json:string>
</affiliations>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>Andreas Maercker</name>
<affiliations>
<json:string>University of Trier</json:string>
</affiliations>
</json:item>
</author>
<subject>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>crime victims</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>retaliation</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>posttraumatic stress disorder</value>
</json:item>
</subject>
<articleId>
<json:string>10.1177_0886260505282286</json:string>
</articleId>
<language>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</language>
<originalGenre>
<json:string>research-article</json:string>
</originalGenre>
<abstract>Individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are often said to experience strong feelings of revenge. However, there is a need for confirmatory empirical studies. Therefore, in a study of 174 victims of violent crimes, the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions was investigated. Feelings of revenge were correlated with intrusion and hyperarousal but not with avoidance. Feelings of revenge explained incremental variance of intrusion and hyperarousal when the variance explained by victimological variables was controlled. The retaliation motive implied in feelings of revenge did not account for the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions. However, the relation was moderated by the time since victimization. Therefore, feelings of revenge must presumably be regarded as a maladaptive coping reaction to experienced injustice, but not in the first period after victimization.</abstract>
<qualityIndicators>
<score>6.608</score>
<pdfVersion>1.3</pdfVersion>
<pdfPageSize>612 x 792 pts (letter)</pdfPageSize>
<refBibsNative>true</refBibsNative>
<abstractCharCount>953</abstractCharCount>
<pdfWordCount>5198</pdfWordCount>
<pdfCharCount>32250</pdfCharCount>
<pdfPageCount>15</pdfPageCount>
<abstractWordCount>134</abstractWordCount>
</qualityIndicators>
<title>Feelings of Revenge, Retaliation Motive, and Posttraumatic Stress Reactions in Crime Victims</title>
<refBibs>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>C. A. Anderson</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>B. J. Bushman</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>53</volume>
<pages>
<last>51</last>
<first>27</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Annual Review of Psychology</title>
</host>
<title>Human aggression</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>J. R. Averill</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>38</volume>
<pages>
<last>1160</last>
<first>1145</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>American Psychologist</title>
</host>
<title>Studies on anger and aggression: Implications for theories of emotion</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>R. M. Baron</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>D. A. Kenny</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>51</volume>
<pages>
<last>1182</last>
<first>1173</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</title>
</host>
<title>The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>L. Berkowitz</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>45</volume>
<pages>
<last>503</last>
<first>494</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>American Psychologist</title>
</host>
<title>On the formation and regulation of anger and aggression</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>G. H. Bower</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>36</volume>
<pages>
<last>148</last>
<first>129</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>American Psychologist</title>
</host>
<title>Mood and memory</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>C. R. Brewin</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>E. A. Holmes</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>23</volume>
<pages>
<last>376</last>
<first>339</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Clinical Psychology Review</title>
</host>
<title>Psychological theories of posttraumatic stress disorder</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>B. J. Bushman</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>28</volume>
<pages>
<last>731</last>
<first>724</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin</title>
</host>
<title>Does venting anger feed or extinguish the flame? Catharsis, rumination, distraction, anger, and aggressive responding</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>G. V. Caprara</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>7</volume>
<pages>
<last>769</last>
<first>763</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Personality and Individual Differences</title>
</host>
<title>Indicators of aggression: The Dissipation-Rumination Scale</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>B. L. Cardozo</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>R. Kaiser</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>C. A. Gotway</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>F. Agani</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>16</volume>
<pages>
<last>360</last>
<first>351</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Traumatic Stress</title>
</host>
<title>Mental health, social functioning, and feelings of hatred and revenge of Kosovar Albanians one year after the war in Kosovo</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>C. M. Chemtob</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>R. S. Hamada</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>H. L. Roitblat</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>M. Y. Muraoka</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>62</volume>
<pages>
<last>832</last>
<first>827</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology</title>
</host>
<title>Anger, impulsivity, and anger control in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>M. Creamer</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>P. Burgess</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>P. Pattison</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>101</volume>
<pages>
<last>459</last>
<first>452</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Abnormal Psychology</title>
</host>
<title>Reaction to trauma: A cognitive processing model</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>A. Ehlers</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>D. M. Clark</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>38</volume>
<pages>
<last>345</last>
<first>319</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Behaviour Research and Therapy</title>
</host>
<title>A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>N. C. Feeny</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>L. A. Zoellner</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>E. B. Foa</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>13</volume>
<pages>
<last>100</last>
<first>89</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Traumatic Stress</title>
</host>
<title>Anger, dissociation, and posttraumatic stress disorder among female assault victims</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>E. B. Foa</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>D. S. Riggs</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>E. D. Masie</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>M. Yarczower</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>26</volume>
<pages>
<last>499</last>
<first>487</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Behavior Therapy</title>
</host>
<title>The impact of fear activation and anger on the efficacy of exposure treatment for posttraumatic stress disorder</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>N. H. Frijda</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name> </name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name> </name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name> </name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<pages>
<last>289</last>
<first>263</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Emotions: Essays on emotion theory</title>
</host>
<title>The lex talionis: On vengeance</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>B. C. Frueh</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>K. R. Henning</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>K. L. Pellegrin</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>K. Chobot</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>53</volume>
<pages>
<last>878</last>
<first>871</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Clinical Psychology</title>
</host>
<title>Relationship between scores on anger measures and PTSD symptomatology, employment, and compensation-seeking status in combat veterans</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>A. K. Goenjian</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>L. Molina</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>A. M. Steinberg</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>L. A. Fairbanks</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>M. L. Alvarez</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>H. A. Goenjian</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name> </name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>158</volume>
<pages>
<last>794</last>
<first>788</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>American Journal of Psychiatry</title>
</host>
<title>Posttraumatic stress and depressive reactions among Nicaraguan adolescents after hurricane Mitch</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>D. B. Gold</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>D. M. Wegner</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>25</volume>
<pages>
<last>1261</last>
<first>1245</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Applied Social Psychology</title>
</host>
<title>Origins of ruminative thought: Trauma, incompleteness, nondisclosure, and suppression</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>J. L. Herman</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>5</volume>
<pages>
<last>391</last>
<first>377</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Traumatic Stress</title>
</host>
<title>Complex PTSD: A syndrome in survivors of prolonged and repeated trauma</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>R. C. Kessler</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>A. Sonnega</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>E. Bromet</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>M. Hughes</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>C. B. Nelson</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>52</volume>
<pages>
<last>1060</last>
<first>1048</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Archives of General Psychiatry</title>
</host>
<title>Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>D. G. Kilpatrick</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>B. E. Saunders</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>A. Amick-McMullan</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>C. L. Best</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>L. J. Veronen</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>H. S. Resnick</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>20</volume>
<pages>
<last>214</last>
<first>199</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Behavior Therapy</title>
</host>
<title>Victim and crime factors associated with the development of crime-related post-traumatic stress disorder</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>A. Maercker</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>M. Schützwohl</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>44</volume>
<pages>
<last>141</last>
<first>130</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Diagnostica</title>
</host>
<title>Erfassung von psychischen Belastungsfolgen: Die Impact of Event-Skala—Revidierte version (IES-R) [Assessment of psychological stress reactions: The Impact of Event Scale—Revised (IES-R)]</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>L. L. Martin</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>A. Tesser</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name> </name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<pages>
<last>47</last>
<first>1</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Ruminative thoughts</title>
</host>
<title>Some ruminative thoughts</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>M. E. McCullough</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>C. G. Bellah</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>S. D. Kilpatrick</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>J. L. Johnson</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>27</volume>
<pages>
<last>610</last>
<first>601</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin</title>
</host>
<title>Vengefulness: Relationships with forgiveness, rumination, well-being, and the Big Five</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>M. E. McCullough</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>K. C. Rachal</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>S. J. Sandage</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>E. L. Worthington</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>S. W. Brown</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>T. L. Hight</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>75</volume>
<pages>
<last>1603</last>
<first>1586</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</title>
</host>
<title>Interpersonal for giving in close relationships: II. Theoretical elaboration and measurement</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>A. C. McFarlane</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>180</volume>
<pages>
<last>445</last>
<first>439</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease</title>
</host>
<title>Avoidance and intrusion in posttraumatic stress disorder</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>L. Montada</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name> </name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name> </name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<pages>
<last>309</last>
<first>292</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>The moral self</title>
</host>
<title>Understanding oughts by assessing moral reasoning or moral emotions</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>L. Montada</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>7</volume>
<pages>
<last>28</last>
<first>5</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Social Justice Research</title>
</host>
<title>Injustice in harm and loss</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>R. W. Novaco</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>C. M. Chemtob</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>15</volume>
<pages>
<last>132</last>
<first>123</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Traumatic Stress</title>
</host>
<title>Anger and combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>U. Orth</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>27</volume>
<pages>
<last>186</last>
<first>173</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Law and Human Behavior</title>
</host>
<title>Punishment goals of crime victims</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>U. Orth</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>A. Maercker</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>L. Montada</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>32</volume>
<pages>
<last>175</last>
<first>169</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie</title>
</host>
<title>Rachegefühle und posttraumatische belastungsreaktionen bei opfern von gewalttaten [Feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions in victims of violent crimes]</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>D. S. Riggs</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>C. V. Dancu</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>B. S. Gershuny</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>D. Greenberg</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>E. B. Foa</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>5</volume>
<pages>
<last>625</last>
<first>613</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Traumatic Stress</title>
</host>
<title>Anger and post-traumatic stress disorderin female crime victims</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>C. L. Rusting</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>S. Nolen-Hoeksema</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>74</volume>
<pages>
<last>803</last>
<first>790</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</title>
</host>
<title>Regulating responses to anger: Effects of rumination and distraction on angry mood</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>K. R. Scherer</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name> </name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name> </name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<pages>
<last>663</last>
<first>637</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Handbook of cognition and emotion</title>
</host>
<title>Appraisal theory</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>M. Schützwohl</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>A. Maercker</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>188</volume>
<pages>
<last>489</last>
<first>483</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease</title>
</host>
<title>Anger in former East German political prisoners: Relationship to posttraumatic stress reactions and social support</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>R. Steil</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>A. Ehlers</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>38</volume>
<pages>
<last>558</last>
<first>537</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Behaviour Research and Therapy</title>
</host>
<title>Dysfunctional meaning of posttraumatic intrusions in chronic PTSD</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>N. Vidmar</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name> </name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name> </name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<pages>
<last>63</last>
<first>31</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Handbook of justice research in law</title>
</host>
<title>Retribution and revenge</title>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>D. S. Weiss</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>C. R. Marmar</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name> </name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name> </name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<pages>
<last>411</last>
<first>399</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Assessing psychological trauma and PTSD</title>
</host>
<title>The Impact of Event Scale—Revised</title>
</json:item>
</refBibs>
<genre>
<json:string>research-article</json:string>
</genre>
<host>
<volume>21</volume>
<publisherId>
<json:string>JIV</json:string>
</publisherId>
<pages>
<last>243</last>
<first>229</first>
</pages>
<issn>
<json:string>0886-2605</json:string>
</issn>
<issue>2</issue>
<genre>
<json:string>journal</json:string>
</genre>
<language>
<json:string>unknown</json:string>
</language>
<eissn>
<json:string>1552-6518</json:string>
</eissn>
<title>Journal of Interpersonal Violence</title>
</host>
<categories>
<wos>
<json:string>social science</json:string>
<json:string>psychology, applied</json:string>
<json:string>family studies</json:string>
<json:string>criminology & penology</json:string>
</wos>
<scienceMetrix>
<json:string>economic & social sciences</json:string>
<json:string>social sciences</json:string>
<json:string>criminology</json:string>
</scienceMetrix>
</categories>
<publicationDate>2006</publicationDate>
<copyrightDate>2006</copyrightDate>
<doi>
<json:string>10.1177/0886260505282286</json:string>
</doi>
<id>C2C24723866933E407675FBF60EDFBCF8347D9C4</id>
<score>1.5558317</score>
<fulltext>
<json:item>
<extension>pdf</extension>
<original>true</original>
<mimetype>application/pdf</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/C2C24723866933E407675FBF60EDFBCF8347D9C4/fulltext/pdf</uri>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<extension>zip</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>application/zip</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/C2C24723866933E407675FBF60EDFBCF8347D9C4/fulltext/zip</uri>
</json:item>
<istex:fulltextTEI uri="https://api.istex.fr/document/C2C24723866933E407675FBF60EDFBCF8347D9C4/fulltext/tei">
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">Feelings of Revenge, Retaliation Motive, and Posttraumatic Stress Reactions in Crime Victims</title>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<authority>ISTEX</authority>
<publisher>Sage Publications</publisher>
<pubPlace>Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</pubPlace>
<availability>
<p>SAGE</p>
</availability>
<date>2006</date>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct type="inbook">
<analytic>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">Feelings of Revenge, Retaliation Motive, and Posttraumatic Stress Reactions in Crime Victims</title>
<author xml:id="author-1">
<persName>
<forename type="first">Ulrich</forename>
<surname>Orth</surname>
</persName>
<email>ulrich.orth@psy.unibe.ch</email>
<affiliation>University of Berne,</affiliation>
</author>
<author xml:id="author-2">
<persName>
<forename type="first">Leo</forename>
<surname>Montada</surname>
</persName>
<affiliation>University of Trier</affiliation>
</author>
<author xml:id="author-3">
<persName>
<forename type="first">Andreas</forename>
<surname>Maercker</surname>
</persName>
<affiliation>University of Trier</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<monogr>
<title level="j">Journal of Interpersonal Violence</title>
<idno type="pISSN">0886-2605</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1552-6518</idno>
<imprint>
<publisher>Sage Publications</publisher>
<pubPlace>Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</pubPlace>
<date type="published" when="2006-02"></date>
<biblScope unit="volume">21</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="issue">2</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" from="229">229</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" to="243">243</biblScope>
</imprint>
</monogr>
<idno type="istex">C2C24723866933E407675FBF60EDFBCF8347D9C4</idno>
<idno type="DOI">10.1177/0886260505282286</idno>
<idno type="ArticleID">10.1177_0886260505282286</idno>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<creation>
<date>2006</date>
</creation>
<langUsage>
<language ident="en">en</language>
</langUsage>
<abstract xml:lang="en">
<p>Individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are often said to experience strong feelings of revenge. However, there is a need for confirmatory empirical studies. Therefore, in a study of 174 victims of violent crimes, the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions was investigated. Feelings of revenge were correlated with intrusion and hyperarousal but not with avoidance. Feelings of revenge explained incremental variance of intrusion and hyperarousal when the variance explained by victimological variables was controlled. The retaliation motive implied in feelings of revenge did not account for the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions. However, the relation was moderated by the time since victimization. Therefore, feelings of revenge must presumably be regarded as a maladaptive coping reaction to experienced injustice, but not in the first period after victimization.</p>
</abstract>
<textClass>
<keywords scheme="keyword">
<list>
<head>keywords</head>
<item>
<term>crime victims</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>retaliation</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>posttraumatic stress disorder</term>
</item>
</list>
</keywords>
</textClass>
</profileDesc>
<revisionDesc>
<change when="2006-02">Published</change>
</revisionDesc>
</teiHeader>
</istex:fulltextTEI>
<json:item>
<extension>txt</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>text/plain</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/C2C24723866933E407675FBF60EDFBCF8347D9C4/fulltext/txt</uri>
</json:item>
</fulltext>
<metadata>
<istex:metadataXml wicri:clean="corpus sage not found" wicri:toSee="no header">
<istex:xmlDeclaration>version="1.0" encoding="UTF-8"</istex:xmlDeclaration>
<istex:docType PUBLIC="-//NLM//DTD Journal Publishing DTD v2.3 20070202//EN" URI="journalpublishing.dtd" name="istex:docType"></istex:docType>
<istex:document>
<article article-type="research-article" dtd-version="2.3" xml:lang="EN">
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="hwp">spjiv</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="publisher-id">JIV</journal-id>
<journal-title>Journal of Interpersonal Violence</journal-title>
<issn pub-type="ppub">0886-2605</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Sage Publications</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</publisher-loc>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0886260505282286</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="publisher-id">10.1177_0886260505282286</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Articles</subject>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>Feelings of Revenge, Retaliation Motive, and Posttraumatic Stress Reactions in Crime Victims</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Orth</surname>
<given-names>Ulrich</given-names>
</name>
<aff>University of Berne,
<email xlink:type="simple">ulrich.orth@psy.unibe.ch</email>
</aff>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Montada</surname>
<given-names>Leo</given-names>
</name>
<aff>University of Trier</aff>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Maercker</surname>
<given-names>Andreas</given-names>
</name>
<aff>University of Trier</aff>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<pub-date pub-type="ppub">
<month>02</month>
<year>2006</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>21</volume>
<issue>2</issue>
<fpage>229</fpage>
<lpage>243</lpage>
<abstract>
<p>Individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are often said to experience strong feelings of revenge. However, there is a need for confirmatory empirical studies. Therefore, in a study of 174 victims of violent crimes, the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions was investigated. Feelings of revenge were correlated with intrusion and hyperarousal but not with avoidance. Feelings of revenge explained incremental variance of intrusion and hyperarousal when the variance explained by victimological variables was controlled. The retaliation motive implied in feelings of revenge did not account for the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions. However, the relation was moderated by the time since victimization. Therefore, feelings of revenge must presumably be regarded as a maladaptive coping reaction to experienced injustice, but not in the first period after victimization.</p>
</abstract>
<kwd-group>
<kwd>crime victims</kwd>
<kwd>retaliation</kwd>
<kwd>posttraumatic stress disorder</kwd>
</kwd-group>
<custom-meta-wrap>
<custom-meta xlink:type="simple">
<meta-name>sagemeta-type</meta-name>
<meta-value>Journal Article</meta-value>
</custom-meta>
<custom-meta xlink:type="simple">
<meta-name>search-text</meta-name>
<meta-value> 10.1177/0886260505282286Journal of Interpersonal V iolenceOrth et al. / Feelings of Revenge Feelings of Revenge, Retaliation Motive, and Posttraumatic Stress Reactions in Crime Victims Ulrich Orth University of Berne Leo Montada Andreas Maercker University of Trier Individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are often said to experi- ence strong feelings of revenge. However, there is a need for confirmatory empirical studies. Therefore, in a study of 174 victims of violent crimes, the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions was investigated. Feelings of revenge were correlated with intrusion and hyper- arousal but not with avoidance. Feelings of revenge explained incremental variance of intrusion and hyperarousal when the variance explained by victimo- logical variables was controlled. The retaliation motive implied in feelings of revenge did not account for the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions. However, the relation was moderated by the time since victimization. Therefore, feelings of revenge must presumably be regarded as a maladaptive coping reaction to experienced injustice, but not in the first period after victimization. Keywords: crime victims; retaliation; posttraumatic stress disorder Victimizations by rape, physical assault, and robbery are traumatic expe- riences. Epidemiological studies show that the prevalence of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is high in victims of violent crimes. Prevalence values range from 35% to 70% for rape victims, from 2% to 58% 229 Journal of Interpersonal Violence Volume 21 Number 2 February 2006 229-243 © 2006 Sage Publications 10.1177/0886260505282286 http://jiv.sagepub.com hosted at http://online.sagepub.com Authors' Note: The authors would like to thank the German victim assistance association Weisser Ring for their financial and organizational support of the data collection. Parts of this study have been published in German in Orth, Maercker, and Montada (2003). Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ulrich Orth, Department of Psychology, Univer- sityofBerne,Muesmattstrasse45,3012Berne,Switzerland;e-mail:ulrich.orth@psy.unibe.ch. for victims of physical assault, and from 18% to 28% for victims of robbery (Kessler, Sonnega, Bromet, Hughes, & Nelson, 1995; Kilpatrick et al., 1989). Furthermore, victimizations by violent crimes are experiences of injustice that provoke coping reactions (cf. Montada, 1994). Cognitive coping reac- tions that subjectively reduce injustice are, for example, minimizing the harm and excusing and justifying the perpetrator's behavior. Behavioral cop- ing reactions intended to more objectively counterbalance injustice may take the form of reporting the crime to the police, demanding the perpetrator's punishment, claiming compensation, or engaging in self-administered jus- tice and revenge. The number of crime victims who actually put revenge into practice is unknown. Likewise, littleempirical information about the prevalence of feel- ings of revenge in crime victims is available (cf. Frijda, 1994; Vidmar, 2000). In one study of victims of ethnically motivated violence, between 41% and 46% reported strong feelings of revenge (Cardozo, Kaiser, Gotway, & Agani, 2003). Feelings of revenge shall be defined here by the following cognitive com- ponents (cf. Montada, 1993; Vidmar, 2000) according to the appraisal theory of emotion (cf. Scherer, 1999): the individual perceives himself or herself to be severely harmed by another person, the individual holds the other person responsible for the harm (no excuses are recognized), the individual assesses the harm as morally wrong (no justifications are recognized), and, crucially, the individual perceives a motivation to retaliate. Feelings of Revenge and PTSD Descriptions of PTSD mention that individuals with PTSD sometimes experience strong feelings of revenge (cf. Ehlers & Clark, 2000). Excessive preoccupation with fantasies about revenge has been considered to be a con- sequence of the disordered emotion regulation in complex PTSD after pro- longed and repeated trauma (Herman, 1992). However, there is a need for empirical studies. Only two studies provide information about the relation between feelings of revenge and PTSD; how- ever, in both studies, feelings of revenge were measured by single items. In a study of victims of a natural disaster, feelings of revenge against the public authorities contributed to the prediction of PTSD with a low but significant regression coefficient (Goenjian et al., 2001). In the study of Cardozo et al. (2003) cited above, feelings of revenge were more frequent in individuals with PTSD compared to those without PTSD. 230 Journal of Interpersonal Violence Therefore, the first objective of this study is to investigate the relation between feelings of revenge and PTSD more closely and to analyze the spe- cific relations with different posttraumatic stress reactions such as post- traumatic intrusion, avoidance, and hyperarousal. The study tests the hypoth- esis that feelings of revenge in victims of violent crimes correlate with posttraumatic intrusion and hyperarousal. The study further tests whether feelings of revenge explain incremental variance when controlling the vari- ance explained by standard victimological variables such as severity of phys- ical harm and emotional support. However, feelings of revenge should corre- late to a lesser extent with posttraumatic avoidance, in particular with cognitive avoidance symptoms, because avoidance of trauma-related memo- ries should reduce the frequency and intensity of trauma-related feelings of revenge. Feelings of Revenge and Anger Although data concerning feelings of revenge are sparse, a number of empirical studies have investigated the relation of anger and PTSD. The results of these studies are described below as they may help to generate hypotheses about the relation between feelings of revenge and PTSD. However, similarities and differences between feelings of revenge and anger should be described first. On one hand, the similarities consist of the perception of a harming event, the attribution of responsibility to another person, and the perception of a lack of justification of the other person's behavior. On the other hand, feelings of revenge differ from anger in one essential aspect: Anger may motivate not only to aggressive reactions but also to nonaggressive reactions (cf. Averill, 1983). Feelings of revenge, in contrast, include the aggressive retaliation motive by definition. Therefore, the second objective of this study is to test whether the retalia- tion motive, which is the key component of feelings of revenge in contrast to anger, accounts for the relation between feelings of revenge and post- traumatic stress reactions or not. For this reason, an additional measure of the motivational component of feelings of revenge, besides the measure of feel- ings of revenge, will be used. Whether the retaliation motive accounts for the relation or not can be tested by mediation analysis (Baron & Kenny, 1986). Anger and PTSD Empirical studies of the relation between anger and PTSD show that vic- tims have higher trait anger compared to the test norms of community sam- Orth et al. / Feelings of Revenge 231 ples and that victims with PTSD show higher trait anger compared to victims without PTSD (Schützwohl & Maercker, 2000). In that study, trait anger was substantially correlated with posttraumatic intrusion and hyperarousal but not with posttraumatic avoidance. However, in another study, victims and nonvictims did not differ with respect to trait anger, nor did victims with PTSD and without PTSD (Riggs, Dancu, Gershuny, Greenberg, & Foa, 1992). The results for anger expression variables show that in most studies, both anger out and anger in correlated with severity of PTSD (Chemtob, Hamada, Roitblat, & Muraoka, 1994; Feeny, Zoellner, & Foa, 2000; Frueh, Henning, Pellegrin, & Chobot, 1997; Novaco & Chemtob, 2002; Schütz- wohl & Maercker, 2000). Interestingly, in the longitudinal study of Feeny et al. (2000), the correla- tion between anger expression and PTSD severity became higher with increasing time since victimization, rising from virtually zero in Week 2 to a medium level in Week 12. On the whole, the studies on anger and PTSD cited above have generally revealed insignificant correlations for short terms (days, weeks; cf. Feeny et al., 2000; Riggs et al., 1992) and substantial corre- lations for longer terms (months, years, decades; cf. Chemtob et al., 1994; Feeny et al., 2000; Frueh et al., 1997; Schützwohl & Maercker, 2000). Therefore, given these time-dependent results for anger and PTSD, the third objective of this study is to test the hypothesis that the time since victim- ization moderates the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic intrusion and hyperarousal, more precisely that the relation becomes stron- ger with increasing time since victimization. Method Participants The study was part of a research project on the psychological conse- quences of criminal proceedings among victims of violent crimes. Partici- pants were contacted with the help of Weisser Ring, a German victim assis- tance association. The surveyed individuals were chosen at random and were sent a questionnaire with a request to take part in the study. Participant ano- nymity was protected. The response rate was 32%. The sample was drawn from the population of victims who had received financial support from the victim assistance association (i.e., the victims' legal costs were paid) within the past 5 years. The sample consisted of 174 adult victims of violent crimes, including 84 victims of rape and 90 victims of nonsexual assault (bodily harm, robbery, deprivation of liberty). In all, 84% of the participants were women, and 16% 232 Journal of Interpersonal Violence were men. The mean age at the time of the study was 37.5 years (SD = 12.2 years, range = 17-65 years). In all, 95% of the participants were of German nationality. Mean time since victimization was 4.1 years (SD = 2.2 years). Level of school education was as follows: 41% had not finished school or had finished the obligatory 9 years, and 59% had finished high school (10 years) or academic track high school (approximately 13 years). The education level of the sample is roughly representative of the German population. Informa- tion on socioeconomic status was not collected, but level of education may be taken as an indicator of socioeconomic status. In all of the participants'cases, criminal proceedings had taken place, and 94% of the victims had reported the crime to the police themselves or had agreed to having it reported. Mean time since the end of the criminal proceedings was 3.0 years (SD = 1.9 years). Several characteristics of the population from which the sample was drawn should be documented to allow for the assessment of differences between the sample and the population (see Discussion section). In 2001, the association gave support to about 10,000 victims. Of these, 35% were vic- tims of sexual assault, 43% were victims of physical assault, robbery, or theft, and 22% were victims of other crimes. In 22% of the cases, victim age was 20 years or younger, in 42% was between 21 and 40 years, in 22% was between 41 and 60 years, and in 13% was 60 years or older. In all, 73% of the victims were women and 27% were men. Measures Posttraumatic stress reactions. The Impact of Event Scale--Revised (IES-R; Weiss & Marmar, 1997; for the German version, see Maercker & Schützwohl, 1998) was used to assess the frequency of posttraumatic stress reactions in the preceding 7 days. The intrusion subscale includes 7 items (Cronbach's = .87 in this study), the avoidance subscale includes 8 items ( = .78 in this study), and the hyperarousal subscale includes 7 items ( = .87 in this study). The value range is 0 to 35 for intrusion and hyperarousal and 0 to 40 for avoidance. In the German IES-R validation study, a linear regression equation that can be used to estimate the PTSD rate in samples, with a sensitivity of .70 to .76 and a specificity of .88 to .89, was determined (Maercker & Schützwohl, 1998). Feelings of revenge. The participants were asked to assess the strength of feelings of revenge in the preceding 4 weeks using three items ( = .92 in this study): "How often did thoughts come to mind, without your wanting them to, about doing something to the perpetrator?" "How often did you fantasize about getting back at the perpetrator for what he or she did to you?" and "Did Orth et al. / Feelings of Revenge 233 you experience feelings of revenge?" Answers were measured on a 6-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 5 (very often or very strongly). Retaliation motive. The strength of the retaliation motive was assessed using four statements with respect to the victim's punishment goals at the time of the criminal trial ( = .80 in this study). Information about the con- struction and validation of the scale has been documented (Orth, 2003). The statements were, "It was important to me that the perpetrator should be pun- ished to bring satisfaction to me, the victim;" "It was important to me that the perpetrator should be punished to make him/her suffer, as I suffered by his action;" "It was important to me that the perpetrator should be punished to even out the wrong that the offender had done;" and "It was important to me that the perpetrator should be punished to atone for the perpetrator's guilt." Answers were measured on a 6-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all impor- tant) to 5 (very important). Physical harm. Severity of physical harm was assessed with two items ( = .79). Participants assessed on one hand "physical injuries and pain at the time of the victimization," and on the other hand "enduring bodily harm (dis- abilities, pain, disfigurement)." Answers were measured on a 6-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 5 (very serious). Emotional support. Emotional support by relatives and friends was assessed with four items ( = .90): "In the time since the victimization, rela- tives or friends have conducted helpful conversations with me;" "In the time since the victimization, relatives or friends have given me consolation and hope;" "In the time since the victimization, relatives or friends have shown me that they like me;" and "In the time since the victimization, relatives or friends have shown understanding for my feelings and needs." Answers were measured on a 6-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all right) to 5 (completely right). Results Table 1 shows means, standard deviations, and simple correlations of the measures used. For the IES-R subscales, the scores represent means and standard deviations of the subscales; for the other measures, the scores repre- sent means and standard deviations of the items. The PTSD sample rate amounts to 52% according to the regression equation mentioned above (cf. Maercker & Schützwohl, 1998). 234 Journal of Interpersonal Violence Testing the Relation Hypothesis Feelings of revenge have significant correlations at a medium level with intrusion and hyperarousal but not with avoidance. Thus, the simple correla- tions corroborate the relation hypothesis. However, it is crucial to show that the relation remains significant when other predictors of posttraumatic stress reactions are simultaneously taken into account. Therefore, hierarchical regression analyses were conducted. Table 2 summarizes the results of the hierarchical regression analysis pre- dicting intrusion. The results show that feelings of revenge explain incre- mental variance (Step 2, R 2 = .04, p = .002) when controlling for the vari- ance explained by physical harm, emotional support, age, and time since victimization (Step 1, R 2 = .30, p = .000). The results of the hierarchical regression analysis predicting hyperarousal are structurally similar (Step 1, R 2 = .20, p = .000; Step 2, R 2 = .04, p = .004). In contrast, the variance explained in the regression analysis predicting avoidance is substantially lower (Step 1, R 2 = .05, p = .063; Step 2, R 2 = .01, p = .136). Orth et al. / Feelings of Revenge 235 Table 1 Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations of the Study Variables Correlations Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1. Intrusion (IES-R) 20.00 9.40 .-- 2. Avoidance (IES-R) 20.65 9.89 .45** .-- 3. Hyperarousal (IES-R) 20.13 10.29 .80** .57** .-- 4. Feelings of revenge 1.69 1.82 .28** .14 .24** .-- 5. Retaliation motive 3.55 1.37 .00 .09 .06 .27** .-- 6. Physical harm 2.40 1.79 .37** .19* .27** .17* .14 .-- 7. Emotional support 3.71 1.36 ­.32** ­.09 ­.24** ­.12 .16* .05 .-- 8. Age 37.51 12.20 .22** .12 .24** ­.16* .07 .18* .03 .-- 9. Time since victimization 4.11 2.18 ­.16* ­.01 ­.14 ­.04 ­.03 ­.02 .05 .02 .-- Note: 168 n 173. IES-R is the Impact of Event Scale--Revised. *p < .05. **p < .01. Testing the Mediation Hypothesis In Step 3 of the hierarchical regression analysis, the retaliation motive is included to test whether it accounts for the relation (i.e., mediates the rela- tion) between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions. To establish mediation, three conditions must be fulfilled (cf. Baron & Kenny, 1986). First, the predictor (feelings of revenge) must correlate with the medi- ator (retaliation motive). In this study, the correlation between the feelings of revenge and the retaliation motive is r = .27, p = .000 (Table 1). Second, the predictor (feelings of revenge) must correlate with the criterion (posttrau- matic stress reactions). In this study, as reported above, feelings of revenge significantly correlate with posttraumatic intrusion and hyperarousal at a medium level (Table 1). Third, the mediator (retaliation motive) must affect the criterion (posttraumatic stress reactions) when controlling for the predic- tor in a multiple regression analysis. The regression coefficient of the retalia- tion motive should have a significant positive value, and the regression coef- ficient of feelings of revenge should significantly decrease, if the mediation hypothesis is right. However, the regression coefficient of the retaliation motive is nonsignif- icant (for all IES-R subscales), and the regression coefficient of feelings of revenge virtually does not change after inclusion of the retaliation motive (for all IES-R subscales). Thus, the retaliation motive does not mediate the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions. Step 236 Journal of Interpersonal Violence Table 2 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Intrusion Regression Coefficients for Intrusion Variable Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 4 Step 1 Physical harm .35** .31** .31** .32** Emotional support ­.34** ­.31** ­.29** ­.28** Age .17* .21** .22** .20** Time since victimization ­.14* ­.13* ­.14* ­.34** Step 2 Feelings of revenge .-- .22** .24** ­.18 Step 3 Retaliation motive .-- .-- ­.08 ­.09 Step 4 Feelings of revenge × Time since victimization .-- .-- .-- .51** R2 .30** .04** .01 .04** Note: n = 163 *p < .05. **p < .01. 3 results in R 2 = .01, p = .232, for intrusion; R 2 = .00, p = .615, for avoid- ance; and R 2 = .00, p = .891, for hyperarousal. Testing the Moderation Hypothesis In Step 4 of the hierarchical regression analysis, the statistical interaction of feelings of revenge and time since victimization is included to test the moderator effect of time since victimization on the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions. To establish moderation, one condition must be fulfilled (cf. Baron & Kenny, 1986): The interaction of the predictor (feelings of revenge) and the moderator (time since victimization) must have a significant effect on the criterion (posttraumatic stress reac- tions). Step 4 results in R2 = .04, p = .001, for intrusion; R 2 = .01, p = .134, for avoidance; and R 2 = .02, p = .038, for hyperarousal. Thus, time since victimization moderates the relation of feelings of revenge with intrusion and hyperarousal but not avoidance. Figure 1 illus- trates the moderator effect by linear regression of time since victimization predicting intrusion, separately computed for victims with low feelings of revenge (first quartile = 0.00), median feelings of revenge (second quartile = 1.00), and strong feelings of revenge (third quartile = 3.00). The upper border of the time axis is set to 8 years as only 5% of the participants score above that and as the significance of the data from these individuals should not be over- Orth et al. / Feelings of Revenge 237 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 0 8 Time since victimization (in years) Intrusion(IES-R) low feelings of revenge median feelings of revenge strong feelings of revenge Figure 1 Linear Regression of Time Since Victimization Predicting Intrusion (Impact of Event Scale--Revised, IES-R) in Victims With Low (1st Quartile = 0.00), Median (2nd Quartile = 1.00), and Strong (3rd Quartile = 3.00) Feelings of Revenge valued. In the first period after the victimization feelings of revenge do not allow for prediction of posttraumatic intrusion, whereas in later periods, strong feelings of revenge, compared to low feelings of revenge, are associ- ated with significantly more frequent intrusion. Discussion The results suggest that feelings of revenge are significantly linked to posttraumatic stress reactions in crime victims and are moderated in a mean- ingful way by the length of time since the victimization. Feelings of revenge explain substantial proportions of variance in posttraumatic intrusion and hyperarousal, particularly if their effect in interaction with time since victim- ization is taken into account. Given that the onset and maintenance of posttraumatic stress reactions is influenced by multiple psychological fac- tors (cf. Brewin & Holmes, 2003), the incremental variance explained by feelings of revenge is considerable. Feelings of Revenge Are Related to Posttraumatic Stress Reactions The first objective of this study was to investigate the relation between feelings of revenge and PTSD. The results show that feelings of revenge in victims of violent crimes correlate at a medium level with posttraumatic intrusion and hyperarousal but not with posttraumatic avoidance. Feelings of revenge explain significant incremental variance in intrusion and hyper- arousal when the variance explained by standard victimological variables such as severity of physical harm, emotional support, victim age, and time since victimization is controlled. This result is consistent with data reported in a study of victims of a natu- ral disaster (Goenjian et al., 2001) and with studies of reactions to less severe transgressions in social relationships using the intrusion subscale of the IES (McCullough, Bellah, Kilpatrick, & Johnson, 2001; McCullough et al., 1998). However, in the study by McCullough et al. (2001), feelings of revenge were also correlated with the avoidance subscale of the IES. The Retaliation Motive Does Not Account for the Relation The second objective of the study was to test whether the retaliation motive accounts for the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions. However, the strength of the retaliation motive, assessed by an additional measure, did not correlate with posttraumatic stress reactions 238 Journal of Interpersonal Violence and did not mediate the relation between feelings of revenge and posttrau- matic stress reactions. Thus, this result suggests that it is not the retaliation motive implied in feelings of revenge that accounts for the relation but that other factors are responsible. These factors might also explain the relation between anger and posttraumatic stress reactions, given that the key difference between feelings of revenge and anger, the retaliation motive, is not responsible for the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions. A promising candidate for an explanation of the relation might be the ruminative character of feelings of revenge. Theory suggests that feelings of revenge are often ruminative, as one origin of rumination consists in discrep- ancies between the present state and central goals that are not easily attain- able or that the individual does not know how to attain (Gold & Wegner, 1995; Martin & Tesser, 1996). Presumably, this frequently applies for the goal of revenge among victims of violent crimes. Thus, rumination on victimization-related themes might be the crucial link between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions. Rusting and Nolen-Hoeksema (1998) have shown that rumination about the causes and consequences of angry moods maintains or increases the angry mood, and they explain the link by associative network theories of memory (cf. Bower, 1981). Likewise, research on catharsis theory has shown that rumina- tion about anger-eliciting events increases subsequent anger and aggressive behavior (Bushman, 2002), consistent with the cognitive-neoassociationistic theory of aggression (Berkowitz, 1990). Furthermore, individual differences in rumination on offenses suffered that can explain the maintenance of feel- ings of revenge among crime victims have been documented (Caprara, 1986). But rumination about victimization-related themes is presumably also linked to posttraumatic intrusions, likewise based on associative net- works (cf. Ehlers & Clark, 2000; for empirical results, see Steil & Ehlers, 2000). The investigation of these mediating links might be a starting point for further studies. Time Since Victimization Moderates the Relation With respect to the third objective of the study, the results show that the relation of feelings of revenge with intrusion and hyperarousal is moderated by time since victimization and that the relation becomes substantially stron- ger with increasing time since victimization. The statistical interaction of feelings of revenge and time since victimization explains further substantial variance proportions in posttraumatic intrusion. The data are consistent with the assumption that in the first period after a victimization, feelings of Orth et al. / Feelings of Revenge 239 revenge are not a maladaptive emotional coping reaction to the experience of injustice and that it is only with increasing time that feelings of revenge become more and more maladaptive. Again, the potentially ruminative character of feelings of revenge might offer an explanation. Continuing rumination about victimization-related themes should increasingly strengthen the associations between thoughts and emotions involved because they are simultaneously activated. At the beginning, the individual might occasionally switch over from intrusions of the traumatic experiences to rumination on revenge and vice versa. Then, by and by, switching over might become more frequent. As a consequence, feel- ings of revenge and posttraumatic intrusion would increasingly covary with each other. Limitations of the Study and Implications for Future Research Sample characteristics account for some of the limitations. First, the response rate was only 32%, and the nonresponders might differ in some unknown way from the sample with respect to the key variables of the study. Though it is generally difficult to obtain high response rates in surveys with crime victims, the low response rate restricts the generality of the findings. Nonresponders might suffer from stronger feelings of revenge or from stron- ger posttraumatic stress reactions and consequently might not have wanted to participate in the study because it would have meant having to reflect on potentially aversive themes. However, deviating means in a sample do not necessarily affect the relations among variables. Moreover, the sample did not substantially differ from the population from which the sample was drawn with respect to demographic variables, as can be seen from the data in the method section. The high proportion of women in this study is a charac- teristic of the population, and the assault type proportions and the partici- pants'ages in this study correspond sufficiently with the available population data as well. A second significant factor in sample selection is represented by the fact that nearly all participants reported the crime to the police themselves or agreed to having it reported. Thus, the victims surveyed in this study presum- ably have stronger feelings of revenge than do victims who decide not to report the crime to the police. Moreover, criminal proceedings had taken place for all participants, and dissatisfaction with the court decision could increase the maladaptivity of feelings of revenge. These factors also restrict the generality of the findings. Therefore, in future studies, the comparison of victims who had and had not reported the crime to the police would be of interest. Moreover, the relation of feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions could also be studied in samples with victims of noncriminal victimization such as motor vehicle accidents. 240 Journal of Interpersonal Violence A further limitation is the cross-sectional study design, which restricts the validity of conclusions about the time-dependent change in maladaptivity of feelings of revenge. The present study only corroborates the plausibility of this time course. Therefore, future studies should use longitudinal study designs. Moreover, because of the cross-sectional study design, conclusions about the causality of the relation between feelings of revenge and posttrau- matic stress reactions are not valid. The investigation of the causal direction of the relationship and mediating factors that may explain the relationship might benefit from drawing both on theories of cognitive processing after traumatic experiences (cf. Creamer, Burgess, & Pattison, 1992; McFarlane, 1992) and on theories of affective human aggression (cf. Anderson & Bushman, 2002). Future studies should also include measures of both feelings of revenge and anger to analyze the degree of convergence in the relations between these emotions and posttraumatic stress reactions. The role of feelings of revenge in the treatment of PTSD should be investigated, as has been done with respect to anger and PTSD (cf. Foa, Riggs, Masie, & Yarczower, 1995). If feelings of revenge have a causal effect on the maintenance of posttraumatic stress reactions, specific interventions that help the individual to cope more effectively with the injustice suffered should be included in the treatment of PTSD. References Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 27-51. Averill, J. R. (1983). Studies on anger and aggression: Implications for theories of emotion. American Psychologist, 38, 1145-1160. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psy- chological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182. Berkowitz, L. (1990). On the formation and regulation of anger and aggression. American Psy- chologist, 45, 494-503. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129-148. Brewin, C. R., & Holmes, E. A. (2003). Psychological theories of posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 23, 339-376. Bushman, B. J. (2002). Does venting anger feed or extinguish the flame? Catharsis, rumination, distraction, anger, and aggressive responding. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 724-731. Caprara, G. V. (1986). Indicators of aggression: The Dissipation-Rumination Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 763-769. Cardozo, B. L., Kaiser, R., Gotway, C. A., & Agani, F. (2003). Mental health, social functioning, and feelings of hatred and revenge of Kosovar Albanians one year after the war in Kosovo. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 16, 351-360. Orth et al. / Feelings of Revenge 241 Chemtob, C. M., Hamada, R. S., Roitblat, H. L., & Muraoka, M. Y. (1994). Anger, impulsivity, and anger control in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 827-832. Creamer, M., Burgess, P., & Pattison, P. (1992). Reaction to trauma: A cognitive processing model. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 452-459. Ehlers, A., & Clark, D. M. (2000).A cognitive model of posttraumaticstress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 319-345. Feeny, N. C., Zoellner, L. A., & Foa, E. B. (2000). Anger, dissociation, and posttraumatic stress disorder among female assault victims. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 13, 89-100. Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S., Masie, E. D., & Yarczower, M. (1995). The impact of fear activation and anger on the efficacy of exposure treatment for posttraumatic stress disorder. Behavior Ther- apy, 26, 487-499. Frijda, N. H. (1994). The lex talionis: On vengeance. In S. H. M. van Goozen, N. E. Van de Poll, & J. A. Sergeant (Eds.), Emotions: Essays on emotion theory (pp. 263-289). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Frueh, B. C., Henning,K. R., Pellegrin, K. L., & Chobot,K. (1997).Relationship between scores on anger measures and PTSD symptomatology, employment, and compensation-seeking status in combat veterans. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 53, 871-878. Goenjian, A. K., Molina, L., Steinberg, A. M., Fairbanks, L. A., Alvarez, M. L., Goenjian, H. A., et al. (2001). Posttraumatic stress and depressive reactions among Nicaraguan adolescents after hurricane Mitch. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 788-794. Gold, D. B., & Wegner, D. M. (1995). Origins of ruminative thought: Trauma, incompleteness, nondisclosure, and suppression. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 1245-1261. Herman, J. L. (1992). Complex PTSD: A syndrome in survivors of prolonged and repeated trauma. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 5, 377-391. Kessler, R. C., Sonnega, A., Bromet, E., Hughes, M., & Nelson, C. B. (1995). Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 1048-1060. Kilpatrick, D. G., Saunders, B. E., Amick-McMullan, A., Best, C. L., Veronen, L. J., & Resnick, H. S. (1989). Victim and crime factors associated with the development of crime-related post-traumatic stress disorder. Behavior Therapy, 20, 199-214. Maercker, A., & Schützwohl, M. (1998). Erfassung von psychischen Belastungsfolgen: Die Impact of Event-Skala--Revidierte version (IES-R) [Assessment of psychological stress reactions: The Impact of Event Scale--Revised (IES-R)]. Diagnostica, 44, 130-141. Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1996). Some ruminative thoughts. In R. S. Wyer (Ed.), Ruminative thoughts (pp. 1-47). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. McCullough, M. E., Bellah, C. G., Kilpatrick, S. D., & Johnson, J. L. (2001). Vengefulness: Relationships with forgiveness, rumination, well-being, and the Big Five. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 601-610. McCullough, M. E., Rachal, K. C., Sandage, S. J., Worthington, E. L., Brown, S. W., & Hight, T. L. (1998).Interpersonalforgivingin close relationships:II. Theoreticalelaborationand mea- surement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1586-1603. McFarlane, A. C. (1992). Avoidance and intrusion in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 180, 439-445. Montada, L. (1993). Understanding oughts by assessing moral reasoning or moral emotions. In G. G. Noam& T. E. Wren (Eds.),The moral self (pp.292-309).Cambridge,MA: MITPress. Montada, L. (1994). Injustice in harm and loss. Social Justice Research, 7, 5-28. Novaco, R. W., & Chemtob, C. M. (2002). Anger and combat-related posttraumatic stress disor- der. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 15, 123-132. 242 Journal of Interpersonal Violence Orth, U. (2003). Punishment goals of crime victims. Law and Human Behavior, 27, 173-186. Orth, U., Maercker, A., & Montada, L. (2003). Rachegefühle und posttraumatische belastungsreaktionen bei opfern von gewalttaten [Feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions in victims of violent crimes]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 32, 169-175. Riggs, D. S., Dancu, C. V., Gershuny, B. S., Greenberg, D., & Foa, E. B. (1992). Anger and post- traumaticstress disorderin femalecrimevictims.Journalof TraumaticStress, 5, 613-625. Rusting, C. L., & Nolen-Hoeksema,S. (1998).Regulatingresponses to anger: Effects of rumina- tion and distraction on angry mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 790- 803. Scherer, K. R. (1999). Appraisal theory. In T. Dalgleish & M. J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cog- nition and emotion (pp. 637-663). Chichester, UK: Wiley. Schützwohl, M., & Maercker, A. (2000). Anger in former East German political prisoners: Rela- tionship to posttraumatic stress reactions and social support. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 188, 483-489. Steil, R., & Ehlers, A. (2000). Dysfunctional meaning of posttraumatic intrusions in chronic PTSD. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 537-558. Vidmar, N. (2000). Retribution and revenge. In J. Sanders & V. L. Hamilton (Eds.), Handbook of justice research in law (pp. 31-63). New York: Kluwer. Weiss, D. S., & Marmar, C. R. (1997). The Impact of Event Scale--Revised. In J. P. Wilson & T. M. Keane (Eds.), Assessing psychological trauma and PTSD (pp. 399-411). New York: Guilford. Ulrich Orth, Ph.D., is research fellow in the Department of Psychology, University of Berne, Switzerland. His research focuses on traumatic stress, emotion, and justice. Leo Montada, Dr. rer. soc., is professor of psychology at the University of Trier, Germany, a member of the European Academy of Science, and founding president of the International Soci- ety for Justice Research. AndreasMaercker,Ph.D.,M.D.,is professorofpsychologyandchairat theUniversityofTrier, Germany, and is directing a center for research on posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). His research focuses on interpersonal factors in the etiology and treatment of PTSD and on posttraumatic syndromes in old age. Orth et al. / Feelings of Revenge 243 </meta-value>
</custom-meta>
</custom-meta-wrap>
</article-meta>
</front>
<back>
<ref-list>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Anderson, C. A.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Bushman, B. J.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Human aggression</article-title>
.
<source>Annual Review of Psychology</source>
,
<volume>53</volume>
,
<fpage>27</fpage>
-
<lpage>51</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Averill, J. R.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1983</year>
).
<article-title>Studies on anger and aggression: Implications for theories of emotion</article-title>
.
<source>American Psychologist</source>
,
<volume>38</volume>
,
<fpage>1145</fpage>
-
<lpage>1160</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Baron, R. M.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Kenny, D. A.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1986</year>
).
<article-title>The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations</article-title>
.
<source>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</source>
,
<volume>51</volume>
,
<fpage>1173</fpage>
-
<lpage>1182</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Berkowitz, L.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<article-title>On the formation and regulation of anger and aggression</article-title>
.
<source>American Psychologist</source>
,
<volume>45</volume>
,
<fpage>494</fpage>
-
<lpage>503</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Bower, G. H.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1981</year>
).
<article-title>Mood and memory</article-title>
.
<source>American Psychologist</source>
,
<volume>36</volume>
,
<fpage>129</fpage>
-
<lpage>148</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Brewin, C. R.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Holmes, E. A.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Psychological theories of posttraumatic stress disorder</article-title>
.
<source>Clinical Psychology Review</source>
,
<volume>23</volume>
,
<fpage>339</fpage>
-
<lpage>376</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Bushman, B. J.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Does venting anger feed or extinguish the flame? Catharsis, rumination, distraction, anger, and aggressive responding</article-title>
.
<source>Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin</source>
,
<volume>28</volume>
,
<fpage>724</fpage>
-
<lpage>731</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Caprara, G. V.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1986</year>
).
<article-title>Indicators of aggression: The Dissipation-Rumination Scale</article-title>
.
<source>Personality and Individual Differences</source>
,
<volume>7</volume>
,
<fpage>763</fpage>
-
<lpage>769</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Cardozo, B. L.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Kaiser, R.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Gotway, C. A.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Agani, F.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Mental health, social functioning, and feelings of hatred and revenge of Kosovar Albanians one year after the war in Kosovo</article-title>
.
<source>Journal of Traumatic Stress</source>
,
<volume>16</volume>
,
<fpage>351</fpage>
-
<lpage>360</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Chemtob, C. M.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Hamada, R. S.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Roitblat, H. L.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Muraoka, M. Y.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>Anger, impulsivity, and anger control in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder</article-title>
.
<source>Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology</source>
,
<volume>62</volume>
,
<fpage>827</fpage>
-
<lpage>832</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Creamer, M.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Burgess, P.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Pattison, P.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1992</year>
).
<article-title>Reaction to trauma: A cognitive processing model</article-title>
.
<source>Journal of Abnormal Psychology</source>
,
<volume>101</volume>
,
<fpage>452</fpage>
-
<lpage>459</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Ehlers, A.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Clark, D. M.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder</article-title>
.
<source>Behaviour Research and Therapy</source>
,
<volume>38</volume>
,
<fpage>319</fpage>
-
<lpage>345</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Feeny, N. C.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Zoellner, L. A.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Foa, E. B.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Anger, dissociation, and posttraumatic stress disorder among female assault victims</article-title>
.
<source>Journal of Traumatic Stress</source>
,
<volume>13</volume>
,
<fpage>89</fpage>
-
<lpage>100</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Foa, E. B.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Riggs, D. S.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Masie, E. D.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Yarczower, M.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>The impact of fear activation and anger on the efficacy of exposure treatment for posttraumatic stress disorder</article-title>
.
<source>Behavior Therapy</source>
,
<volume>26</volume>
,
<fpage>487</fpage>
-
<lpage>499</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Frijda, N. H.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>The lex talionis: On vengeance</article-title>
. In
<name name-style="western">
<surname>S. H. M. van Goozen</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>N. E. Van de Poll</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>J. A. Sergeant</surname>
</name>
(Eds.),
<source>Emotions: Essays on emotion theory</source>
(pp.
<fpage>263</fpage>
-
<lpage>289</lpage>
).
<publisher-loc>Hillsdale, NJ</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Lawrence Erlbaum</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Frueh, B. C.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Henning, K. R.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Pellegrin, K. L.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Chobot, K.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1997</year>
).
<article-title>Relationship between scores on anger measures and PTSD symptomatology, employment, and compensation-seeking status in combat veterans</article-title>
.
<source>Journal of Clinical Psychology</source>
,
<volume>53</volume>
,
<fpage>871</fpage>
-
<lpage>878</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Goenjian, A. K.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Molina, L.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Steinberg, A. M.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Fairbanks, L. A.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Alvarez, M. L.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Goenjian, H. A.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>et al.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Posttraumatic stress and depressive reactions among Nicaraguan adolescents after hurricane Mitch</article-title>
.
<source>American Journal of Psychiatry</source>
,
<volume>158</volume>
,
<fpage>788</fpage>
-
<lpage>794</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Gold, D. B.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Wegner, D. M.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>Origins of ruminative thought: Trauma, incompleteness, nondisclosure, and suppression</article-title>
.
<source>Journal of Applied Social Psychology</source>
,
<volume>25</volume>
,
<fpage>1245</fpage>
-
<lpage>1261</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Herman, J. L.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1992</year>
).
<article-title>Complex PTSD: A syndrome in survivors of prolonged and repeated trauma</article-title>
.
<source>Journal of Traumatic Stress</source>
,
<volume>5</volume>
,
<fpage>377</fpage>
-
<lpage>391</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Kessler, R. C.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Sonnega, A.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Bromet, E.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Hughes, M.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Nelson, C. B.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey</article-title>
.
<source>Archives of General Psychiatry</source>
,
<volume>52</volume>
,
<fpage>1048</fpage>
-
<lpage>1060</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Kilpatrick, D. G.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Saunders, B. E.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Amick-McMullan, A.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Best, C. L.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Veronen, L. J.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Resnick, H. S.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1989</year>
).
<article-title>Victim and crime factors associated with the development of crime-related post-traumatic stress disorder</article-title>
.
<source>Behavior Therapy</source>
,
<volume>20</volume>
,
<fpage>199</fpage>
-
<lpage>214</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Maercker, A.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Schützwohl, M.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1998</year>
).
<article-title>Erfassung von psychischen Belastungsfolgen: Die Impact of Event-Skala—Revidierte version (IES-R) [Assessment of psychological stress reactions: The Impact of Event Scale—Revised (IES-R)]</article-title>
.
<source>Diagnostica</source>
,
<volume>44</volume>
,
<fpage>130</fpage>
-
<lpage>141</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Martin, L. L.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Tesser, A.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1996</year>
).
<article-title>Some ruminative thoughts</article-title>
. In
<name name-style="western">
<surname>R. S. Wyer</surname>
</name>
(Ed.),
<source>Ruminative thoughts</source>
(pp.
<fpage>1</fpage>
-
<lpage>47</lpage>
).
<publisher-loc>Mahwah, NJ</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Lawrence Erlbaum</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>McCullough, M. E.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Bellah, C. G.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Kilpatrick, S. D.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Johnson, J. L.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Vengefulness: Relationships with forgiveness, rumination, well-being, and the Big Five</article-title>
.
<source>Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin</source>
,
<volume>27</volume>
,
<fpage>601</fpage>
-
<lpage>610</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>McCullough, M. E.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Rachal, K. C.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Sandage, S. J.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Worthington, E. L.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Brown, S. W.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Hight, T. L.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1998</year>
).
<article-title>Interpersonal for giving in close relationships: II. Theoretical elaboration and measurement</article-title>
.
<source>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</source>
,
<volume>75</volume>
,
<fpage>1586</fpage>
-
<lpage>1603</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>McFarlane, A. C.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1992</year>
).
<article-title>Avoidance and intrusion in posttraumatic stress disorder</article-title>
.
<source>Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease</source>
,
<volume>180</volume>
,
<fpage>439</fpage>
-
<lpage>445</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Montada, L.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1993</year>
).
<article-title>Understanding oughts by assessing moral reasoning or moral emotions</article-title>
. In
<name name-style="western">
<surname>G. G. Noam</surname>
</name>
&
<name name-style="western">
<surname>T. E. Wren</surname>
</name>
(Eds.),
<source>The moral self</source>
(pp.
<fpage>292</fpage>
-
<lpage>309</lpage>
).
<publisher-loc>Cambridge, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>MIT Press</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Montada, L.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>Injustice in harm and loss</article-title>
.
<source>Social Justice Research</source>
,
<volume>7</volume>
,
<fpage>5</fpage>
-
<lpage>28</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Novaco, R. W.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Chemtob, C. M.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Anger and combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder</article-title>
.
<source>Journal of Traumatic Stress</source>
,
<volume>15</volume>
,
<fpage>123</fpage>
-
<lpage>132</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Orth, U.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Punishment goals of crime victims</article-title>
.
<source>Law and Human Behavior</source>
,
<volume>27</volume>
,
<fpage>173</fpage>
-
<lpage>186</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Orth, U.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Maercker, A.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Montada, L.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Rachegefühle und posttraumatische belastungsreaktionen bei opfern von gewalttaten [Feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions in victims of violent crimes]</article-title>
.
<source>Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie</source>
,
<volume>32</volume>
,
<fpage>169</fpage>
-
<lpage>175</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Riggs, D. S.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Dancu, C. V.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Gershuny, B. S.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Greenberg, D.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Foa, E. B.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1992</year>
).
<article-title>Anger and post-traumatic stress disorderin female crime victims</article-title>
.
<source>Journal of Traumatic Stress</source>
,
<volume>5</volume>
,
<fpage>613</fpage>
-
<lpage>625</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Rusting, C. L.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Nolen-Hoeksema, S.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1998</year>
).
<article-title>Regulating responses to anger: Effects of rumination and distraction on angry mood</article-title>
.
<source>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</source>
,
<volume>74</volume>
,
<fpage>790</fpage>
-
<lpage>803</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Scherer, K. R.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<article-title>Appraisal theory</article-title>
. In
<name name-style="western">
<surname>T. Dalgleish</surname>
</name>
&
<name name-style="western">
<surname>M. J. Power</surname>
</name>
(Eds.),
<source>Handbook of cognition and emotion</source>
(pp.
<fpage>637</fpage>
-
<lpage>663</lpage>
).
<publisher-loc>Chichester, UK</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Wiley</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Schützwohl, M.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Maercker, A.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Anger in former East German political prisoners: Relationship to posttraumatic stress reactions and social support</article-title>
.
<source>Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease</source>
,
<volume>188</volume>
,
<fpage>483</fpage>
-
<lpage>489</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Steil, R.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Ehlers, A.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Dysfunctional meaning of posttraumatic intrusions in chronic PTSD</article-title>
.
<source>Behaviour Research and Therapy</source>
,
<volume>38</volume>
,
<fpage>537</fpage>
-
<lpage>558</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Vidmar, N.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Retribution and revenge</article-title>
. In
<name name-style="western">
<surname>J. Sanders</surname>
</name>
&
<name name-style="western">
<surname>V. L. Hamilton</surname>
</name>
(Eds.),
<source>Handbook of justice research in law</source>
(pp.
<fpage>31</fpage>
-
<lpage>63</lpage>
).
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Kluwer</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Weiss, D. S.</surname>
</name>
, &
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Marmar, C. R.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1997</year>
).
<article-title>The Impact of Event Scale—Revised</article-title>
. In
<name name-style="western">
<surname>J. P. Wilson</surname>
</name>
&,!
<name name-style="western">
<surname>T. M. Keane</surname>
</name>
(Eds.),
<source>Assessing psychological trauma and PTSD</source>
(pp.
<fpage>399</fpage>
-
<lpage>411</lpage>
).
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Guilford</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</article>
</istex:document>
</istex:metadataXml>
<mods version="3.6">
<titleInfo lang="en">
<title>Feelings of Revenge, Retaliation Motive, and Posttraumatic Stress Reactions in Crime Victims</title>
</titleInfo>
<titleInfo type="alternative" lang="en" contentType="CDATA">
<title>Feelings of Revenge, Retaliation Motive, and Posttraumatic Stress Reactions in Crime Victims</title>
</titleInfo>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Ulrich</namePart>
<namePart type="family">Orth</namePart>
<affiliation>University of Berne,</affiliation>
<affiliation>E-mail: ulrich.orth@psy.unibe.ch</affiliation>
</name>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Leo</namePart>
<namePart type="family">Montada</namePart>
<affiliation>University of Trier</affiliation>
</name>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Andreas</namePart>
<namePart type="family">Maercker</namePart>
<affiliation>University of Trier</affiliation>
</name>
<typeOfResource>text</typeOfResource>
<genre type="research-article" displayLabel="research-article"></genre>
<originInfo>
<publisher>Sage Publications</publisher>
<place>
<placeTerm type="text">Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</placeTerm>
</place>
<dateIssued encoding="w3cdtf">2006-02</dateIssued>
<copyrightDate encoding="w3cdtf">2006</copyrightDate>
</originInfo>
<language>
<languageTerm type="code" authority="iso639-2b">eng</languageTerm>
<languageTerm type="code" authority="rfc3066">en</languageTerm>
</language>
<physicalDescription>
<internetMediaType>text/html</internetMediaType>
</physicalDescription>
<abstract lang="en">Individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are often said to experience strong feelings of revenge. However, there is a need for confirmatory empirical studies. Therefore, in a study of 174 victims of violent crimes, the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions was investigated. Feelings of revenge were correlated with intrusion and hyperarousal but not with avoidance. Feelings of revenge explained incremental variance of intrusion and hyperarousal when the variance explained by victimological variables was controlled. The retaliation motive implied in feelings of revenge did not account for the relation between feelings of revenge and posttraumatic stress reactions. However, the relation was moderated by the time since victimization. Therefore, feelings of revenge must presumably be regarded as a maladaptive coping reaction to experienced injustice, but not in the first period after victimization.</abstract>
<subject>
<genre>keywords</genre>
<topic>crime victims</topic>
<topic>retaliation</topic>
<topic>posttraumatic stress disorder</topic>
</subject>
<relatedItem type="host">
<titleInfo>
<title>Journal of Interpersonal Violence</title>
</titleInfo>
<genre type="journal">journal</genre>
<identifier type="ISSN">0886-2605</identifier>
<identifier type="eISSN">1552-6518</identifier>
<identifier type="PublisherID">JIV</identifier>
<identifier type="PublisherID-hwp">spjiv</identifier>
<part>
<date>2006</date>
<detail type="volume">
<caption>vol.</caption>
<number>21</number>
</detail>
<detail type="issue">
<caption>no.</caption>
<number>2</number>
</detail>
<extent unit="pages">
<start>229</start>
<end>243</end>
</extent>
</part>
</relatedItem>
<identifier type="istex">C2C24723866933E407675FBF60EDFBCF8347D9C4</identifier>
<identifier type="DOI">10.1177/0886260505282286</identifier>
<identifier type="ArticleID">10.1177_0886260505282286</identifier>
<recordInfo>
<recordContentSource>SAGE</recordContentSource>
</recordInfo>
</mods>
</metadata>
<serie></serie>
</istex>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Wicri/Rhénanie/explor/UnivTrevesV1/Data/Istex/Corpus
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 001411 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Istex/Corpus/biblio.hfd -nk 001411 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Wicri/Rhénanie
   |area=    UnivTrevesV1
   |flux=    Istex
   |étape=   Corpus
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     ISTEX:C2C24723866933E407675FBF60EDFBCF8347D9C4
   |texte=   Feelings of Revenge, Retaliation Motive, and Posttraumatic Stress Reactions in Crime Victims
}}

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.31.
Data generation: Sat Jul 22 16:29:01 2017. Site generation: Wed Feb 28 14:55:37 2024