Serveur d'exploration sur les dispositifs haptiques

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

Gender and line size factors modulate the deviations of the subjective visual vertical induced by head tilt

Identifieur interne : 001B85 ( Pmc/Curation ); précédent : 001B84; suivant : 001B86

Gender and line size factors modulate the deviations of the subjective visual vertical induced by head tilt

Auteurs : Marion Luyat [France] ; Myriam Noël [France] ; Vincent Thery [France] ; Edouard Gentaz [France]

Source :

RBID : PMC:3329413

Abstract

Background

The subjective visual vertical (SVV, the visual estimation of gravitational direction) is commonly considered as an indicator of the sense of orientation. The present study examined the impact of two methodological factors (the angle size of the stimulus and the participant's gender) on deviations of the SVV caused by head tilt. Forty healthy participants (20 men and 20 women) were asked to make visual vertical adjustments of a light bar with their head held vertically or roll-tilted by 30° to the left or to the right. Line angle sizes of 0.95° and 18.92° were presented.

Results

The SVV tended to move in the direction of head tilt in women but away from the direction of head tilt in men. Moreover, the head-tilt effect was also modulated by the stimulus' angle size. The large angle size led to deviations in the direction of head-tilt, whereas the small angle size had the opposite effect.

Conclusions

Our results showed that gender and line angle size have an impact on the evaluation of the SVV. These findings must be taken into account in the growing body of research that uses the SVV paradigm in disease settings. Moreover, this methodological issue may explain (at least in part) the discrepancies found in the literature on the head-tilt effect.


Url:
DOI: 10.1186/1471-2202-13-28
PubMed: 22420467
PubMed Central: 3329413

Links toward previous steps (curation, corpus...)


Links to Exploration step

PMC:3329413

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI>
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">Gender and line size factors modulate the deviations of the subjective visual vertical induced by head tilt</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Luyat, Marion" sort="Luyat, Marion" uniqKey="Luyat M" first="Marion" last="Luyat">Marion Luyat</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="I1">Department of Psychology, University of Lille, Laboratory of Functional Neurosciences and Pathology EA4559, 4 rue du Barreau, Villeneuve d'Ascq, 59653, France</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">France</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Department of Psychology, University of Lille, Laboratory of Functional Neurosciences and Pathology EA4559, 4 rue du Barreau, Villeneuve d'Ascq, 59653</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Noel, Myriam" sort="Noel, Myriam" uniqKey="Noel M" first="Myriam" last="Noël">Myriam Noël</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="I1">Department of Psychology, University of Lille, Laboratory of Functional Neurosciences and Pathology EA4559, 4 rue du Barreau, Villeneuve d'Ascq, 59653, France</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">France</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Department of Psychology, University of Lille, Laboratory of Functional Neurosciences and Pathology EA4559, 4 rue du Barreau, Villeneuve d'Ascq, 59653</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thery, Vincent" sort="Thery, Vincent" uniqKey="Thery V" first="Vincent" last="Thery">Vincent Thery</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="I2">Service MPR Neurologique, Centre Hospitalier de Saint-Amand-les-Eaux, 19 rue des Anciens d'A.F.N., Saint-Amand-les-Eaux, 59230, France</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">France</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Service MPR Neurologique, Centre Hospitalier de Saint-Amand-les-Eaux, 19 rue des Anciens d'A.F.N., Saint-Amand-les-Eaux, 59230</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gentaz, Edouard" sort="Gentaz, Edouard" uniqKey="Gentaz E" first="Edouard" last="Gentaz">Edouard Gentaz</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="I3">Department of Psychology, University of Grenoble, Laboratory of Psychology and NeuroCognition UMR 5105, 1251 avenue centrale, 38040, France</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">France</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Department of Psychology, University of Grenoble, Laboratory of Psychology and NeuroCognition UMR 5105, 1251 avenue centrale, 38040</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">PMC</idno>
<idno type="pmid">22420467</idno>
<idno type="pmc">3329413</idno>
<idno type="url">http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3329413</idno>
<idno type="RBID">PMC:3329413</idno>
<idno type="doi">10.1186/1471-2202-13-28</idno>
<date when="2012">2012</date>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Corpus">001B85</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Curation">001B85</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title xml:lang="en" level="a" type="main">Gender and line size factors modulate the deviations of the subjective visual vertical induced by head tilt</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Luyat, Marion" sort="Luyat, Marion" uniqKey="Luyat M" first="Marion" last="Luyat">Marion Luyat</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="I1">Department of Psychology, University of Lille, Laboratory of Functional Neurosciences and Pathology EA4559, 4 rue du Barreau, Villeneuve d'Ascq, 59653, France</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">France</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Department of Psychology, University of Lille, Laboratory of Functional Neurosciences and Pathology EA4559, 4 rue du Barreau, Villeneuve d'Ascq, 59653</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Noel, Myriam" sort="Noel, Myriam" uniqKey="Noel M" first="Myriam" last="Noël">Myriam Noël</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="I1">Department of Psychology, University of Lille, Laboratory of Functional Neurosciences and Pathology EA4559, 4 rue du Barreau, Villeneuve d'Ascq, 59653, France</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">France</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Department of Psychology, University of Lille, Laboratory of Functional Neurosciences and Pathology EA4559, 4 rue du Barreau, Villeneuve d'Ascq, 59653</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thery, Vincent" sort="Thery, Vincent" uniqKey="Thery V" first="Vincent" last="Thery">Vincent Thery</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="I2">Service MPR Neurologique, Centre Hospitalier de Saint-Amand-les-Eaux, 19 rue des Anciens d'A.F.N., Saint-Amand-les-Eaux, 59230, France</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">France</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Service MPR Neurologique, Centre Hospitalier de Saint-Amand-les-Eaux, 19 rue des Anciens d'A.F.N., Saint-Amand-les-Eaux, 59230</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gentaz, Edouard" sort="Gentaz, Edouard" uniqKey="Gentaz E" first="Edouard" last="Gentaz">Edouard Gentaz</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="I3">Department of Psychology, University of Grenoble, Laboratory of Psychology and NeuroCognition UMR 5105, 1251 avenue centrale, 38040, France</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">France</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Department of Psychology, University of Grenoble, Laboratory of Psychology and NeuroCognition UMR 5105, 1251 avenue centrale, 38040</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<series>
<title level="j">BMC Neuroscience</title>
<idno type="eISSN">1471-2202</idno>
<imprint>
<date when="2012">2012</date>
</imprint>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">
<sec>
<title>Background</title>
<p>The subjective visual vertical (SVV, the visual estimation of gravitational direction) is commonly considered as an indicator of the sense of orientation. The present study examined the impact of two methodological factors (the angle size of the stimulus and the participant's gender) on deviations of the SVV caused by head tilt. Forty healthy participants (20 men and 20 women) were asked to make visual vertical adjustments of a light bar with their head held vertically or roll-tilted by 30° to the left or to the right. Line angle sizes of 0.95° and 18.92° were presented.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Results</title>
<p>The SVV tended to move in the direction of head tilt in women but away from the direction of head tilt in men. Moreover, the head-tilt effect was also modulated by the stimulus' angle size. The large angle size led to deviations in the direction of head-tilt, whereas the small angle size had the opposite effect.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Conclusions</title>
<p>Our results showed that gender and line angle size have an impact on the evaluation of the SVV. These findings must be taken into account in the growing body of research that uses the SVV paradigm in disease settings. Moreover, this methodological issue may explain (at least in part) the discrepancies found in the literature on the head-tilt effect.</p>
</sec>
</div>
</front>
<back>
<div1 type="bibliography">
<listBibl>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bauermeister, M" uniqKey="Bauermeister M">M Bauermeister</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Werner, H" uniqKey="Werner H">H Werner</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wapner, S" uniqKey="Wapner S">S Wapner</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Guerraz, M" uniqKey="Guerraz M">M Guerraz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Luyat, M" uniqKey="Luyat M">M Luyat</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Poquin, D" uniqKey="Poquin D">D Poquin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ohlmann, T" uniqKey="Ohlmann T">T Ohlmann</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Guardia, D" uniqKey="Guardia D">D Guardia</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cottencin, O" uniqKey="Cottencin O">O Cottencin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thomas, P" uniqKey="Thomas P">P Thomas</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dodin, V" uniqKey="Dodin V">V Dodin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Luyat, M" uniqKey="Luyat M">M Luyat</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lejeune, L" uniqKey="Lejeune L">L Lejeune</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thouvarecq, R" uniqKey="Thouvarecq R">R Thouvarecq</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Anderson, Dj" uniqKey="Anderson D">DJ Anderson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Caston, J" uniqKey="Caston J">J Caston</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jouen, F" uniqKey="Jouen F">F Jouen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Luyat, M" uniqKey="Luyat M">M Luyat</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gentaz, E" uniqKey="Gentaz E">E Gentaz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Corte, Tr" uniqKey="Corte T">TR Corte</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Guerraz, M" uniqKey="Guerraz M">M Guerraz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Luyat, M" uniqKey="Luyat M">M Luyat</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wright, Wg" uniqKey="Wright W">WG Wright</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Glasauer, S" uniqKey="Glasauer S">S Glasauer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schuler, Jr" uniqKey="Schuler J">JR Schuler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bockisch, Cj" uniqKey="Bockisch C">CJ Bockisch</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Straumann, D" uniqKey="Straumann D">D Straumann</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tarnutzer, Aa" uniqKey="Tarnutzer A">AA Tarnutzer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Funk, J" uniqKey="Funk J">J Funk</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Finke, K" uniqKey="Finke K">K Finke</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Muller, Hj" uniqKey="Muller H">HJ Muller</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Preger, R" uniqKey="Preger R">R Preger</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kerkhoff, G" uniqKey="Kerkhoff G">G Kerkhoff</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schwarz, U" uniqKey="Schwarz U">U Schwarz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Guldin, Wo" uniqKey="Guldin W">WO Guldin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grusser, O J" uniqKey="Grusser O">O-J Grüsser</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fasold, O" uniqKey="Fasold O">O Fasold</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Von Brevern, M" uniqKey="Von Brevern M">M von Brevern</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kuhberg, M" uniqKey="Kuhberg M">M Kuhberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ploner, Cj" uniqKey="Ploner C">CJ Ploner</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Villringer, A" uniqKey="Villringer A">A Villringer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lempert, T" uniqKey="Lempert T">T Lempert</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wenzel, R" uniqKey="Wenzel R">R Wenzel</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Suzuki, M" uniqKey="Suzuki M">M Suzuki</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kitano, H" uniqKey="Kitano H">H Kitano</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ito, R" uniqKey="Ito R">R Ito</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kitanishi, T" uniqKey="Kitanishi T">T Kitanishi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yazawa, Y" uniqKey="Yazawa Y">Y Yazawa</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ogawa, T" uniqKey="Ogawa T">T Ogawa</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shiino, A" uniqKey="Shiino A">A Shiino</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kitajima, K" uniqKey="Kitajima K">K Kitajima</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barra, J" uniqKey="Barra J">J Barra</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Marquer, A" uniqKey="Marquer A">A Marquer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Joassin, R" uniqKey="Joassin R">R Joassin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Reymond, C" uniqKey="Reymond C">C Reymond</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Metge, L" uniqKey="Metge L">L Metge</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chauvineau, V" uniqKey="Chauvineau V">V Chauvineau</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Perennou, D" uniqKey="Perennou D">D Perennou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bergenius, J" uniqKey="Bergenius J">J Bergenius</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tribukait, A" uniqKey="Tribukait A">A Tribukait</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brantberg, K" uniqKey="Brantberg K">K Brantberg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Anastasopoulos, D" uniqKey="Anastasopoulos D">D Anastasopoulos</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bronstein, Am" uniqKey="Bronstein A">AM Bronstein</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Karnath, Ho" uniqKey="Karnath H">HO Karnath</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dieterich, M" uniqKey="Dieterich M">M Dieterich</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brandt, T" uniqKey="Brandt T">T Brandt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dieterich, M" uniqKey="Dieterich M">M Dieterich</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Danek, A" uniqKey="Danek A">A Danek</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kerkhoff, G" uniqKey="Kerkhoff G">G Kerkhoff</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zoelch, C" uniqKey="Zoelch C">C Zoelch</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Funk, J" uniqKey="Funk J">J Funk</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Finke, K" uniqKey="Finke K">K Finke</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Muller, Hj" uniqKey="Muller H">HJ Muller</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Utz, Ks" uniqKey="Utz K">KS Utz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kerkhoff, G" uniqKey="Kerkhoff G">G Kerkhoff</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Funk, J" uniqKey="Funk J">J Funk</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Finke, K" uniqKey="Finke K">K Finke</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Muller, Hj" uniqKey="Muller H">HJ Muller</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Utz, Ks" uniqKey="Utz K">KS Utz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kerkhoff, G" uniqKey="Kerkhoff G">G Kerkhoff</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Guerraz, M" uniqKey="Guerraz M">M Guerraz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Poquin, D" uniqKey="Poquin D">D Poquin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Luyat, M" uniqKey="Luyat M">M Luyat</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ohlmann, T" uniqKey="Ohlmann T">T Ohlmann</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Luyat, M" uniqKey="Luyat M">M Luyat</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gentaz, E" uniqKey="Gentaz E">E Gentaz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Trousselard, M" uniqKey="Trousselard M">M Trousselard</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cian, C" uniqKey="Cian C">C Cian</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nougier, V" uniqKey="Nougier V">V Nougier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pla, S" uniqKey="Pla S">S Pla</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Raphel, C" uniqKey="Raphel C">C Raphel</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mast, Fw" uniqKey="Mast F">FW Mast</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wade, Nj" uniqKey="Wade N">NJ Wade</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tribukait, A" uniqKey="Tribukait A">A Tribukait</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bergenius, J" uniqKey="Bergenius J">J Bergenius</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brantberg, K" uniqKey="Brantberg K">K Brantberg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Graaf, B" uniqKey="De Graaf B">B De Graaf</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bekkering, H" uniqKey="Bekkering H">H Bekkering</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Erasmus, C" uniqKey="Erasmus C">C Erasmus</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bles, W" uniqKey="Bles W">W Bles</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kaptein, Rg" uniqKey="Kaptein R">RG Kaptein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Gisbergen, Ja" uniqKey="Van Gisbergen J">JA Van Gisbergen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Beuzekom, Ad" uniqKey="Van Beuzekom A">AD Van Beuzekom</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Gisbergen, Ja" uniqKey="Van Gisbergen J">JA Van Gisbergen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tremblay, L" uniqKey="Tremblay L">L Tremblay</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Elliott, D" uniqKey="Elliott D">D Elliott</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tremblay, L" uniqKey="Tremblay L">L Tremblay</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Elliott, D" uniqKey="Elliott D">D Elliott</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Starkes, Jl" uniqKey="Starkes J">JL Starkes</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sato, H" uniqKey="Sato H">H Sato</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sando, I" uniqKey="Sando I">I Sando</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Takahashi, H" uniqKey="Takahashi H">H Takahashi</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Asch, Se" uniqKey="Asch S">SE Asch</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Witkin, Ha" uniqKey="Witkin H">HA Witkin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Robert, M" uniqKey="Robert M">M Robert</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ohlmann, T" uniqKey="Ohlmann T">T Ohlmann</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barnett Cowan, M" uniqKey="Barnett Cowan M">M Barnett-Cowan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dyde, Rt" uniqKey="Dyde R">RT Dyde</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thompson, C" uniqKey="Thompson C">C Thompson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Harris, Lr" uniqKey="Harris L">LR Harris</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bell, Ec" uniqKey="Bell E">EC Bell</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Willson, Mc" uniqKey="Willson M">MC Willson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wilman, Ah" uniqKey="Wilman A">AH Wilman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dave, S" uniqKey="Dave S">S Dave</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Silverstone, Ph" uniqKey="Silverstone P">PH Silverstone</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Viaud Delmon, I" uniqKey="Viaud Delmon I">I Viaud-Delmon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ivanenko, Yp" uniqKey="Ivanenko Y">YP Ivanenko</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Berthoz, A" uniqKey="Berthoz A">A Berthoz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jouvent, R" uniqKey="Jouvent R">R Jouvent</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Groberg, Dh" uniqKey="Groberg D">DH Groberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dustman, Re" uniqKey="Dustman R">RE Dustman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Beck, Ec" uniqKey="Beck E">EC Beck</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wade, S" uniqKey="Wade S">S Wade</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Curthoys, I" uniqKey="Curthoys I">I Curthoys</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tarnutzer, Aa" uniqKey="Tarnutzer A">AA Tarnutzer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bockisch, Cj" uniqKey="Bockisch C">CJ Bockisch</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Straumann, D" uniqKey="Straumann D">D Straumann</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Orban, Ga" uniqKey="Orban G">GA Orban</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vogels, R" uniqKey="Vogels R">R Vogels</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Daddaoua, N" uniqKey="Daddaoua N">N Daddaoua</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dicke, Pw" uniqKey="Dicke P">PW Dicke</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thier, P" uniqKey="Thier P">P Thier</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Corbett, Je" uniqKey="Corbett J">JE Corbett</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Enns, Jt" uniqKey="Enns J">JT Enns</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Handy, Tc" uniqKey="Handy T">TC Handy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vandenberghe, R" uniqKey="Vandenberghe R">R Vandenberghe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dupont, P" uniqKey="Dupont P">P Dupont</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Bruyn, B" uniqKey="De Bruyn B">B De Bruyn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bormans, G" uniqKey="Bormans G">G Bormans</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Michiels, J" uniqKey="Michiels J">J Michiels</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mortelmans, L" uniqKey="Mortelmans L">L Mortelmans</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Orban, Ga" uniqKey="Orban G">GA Orban</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
</listBibl>
</div1>
</back>
</TEI>
<pmc article-type="research-article">
<pmc-dir>properties open_access</pmc-dir>
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="nlm-ta">BMC Neurosci</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="iso-abbrev">BMC Neurosci</journal-id>
<journal-title-group>
<journal-title>BMC Neuroscience</journal-title>
</journal-title-group>
<issn pub-type="epub">1471-2202</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>BioMed Central</publisher-name>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmid">22420467</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmc">3329413</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="publisher-id">1471-2202-13-28</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1186/1471-2202-13-28</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Research Article</subject>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>Gender and line size factors modulate the deviations of the subjective visual vertical induced by head tilt</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author" corresp="yes" id="A1">
<name>
<surname>Luyat</surname>
<given-names>Marion</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="I1">1</xref>
<email>marion.luyat@univ-lille3.fr</email>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author" id="A2">
<name>
<surname>Noël</surname>
<given-names>Myriam</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="I1">1</xref>
<email>myriam.noel@ch-roubaix.fr</email>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author" id="A3">
<name>
<surname>Thery</surname>
<given-names>Vincent</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="I2">2</xref>
<email>vthery@ch-saint-amand-les-eaux.fr</email>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author" id="A4">
<name>
<surname>Gentaz</surname>
<given-names>Edouard</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="I3">3</xref>
<email>edouard.gentaz@upmf-grenoble.fr</email>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<aff id="I1">
<label>1</label>
Department of Psychology, University of Lille, Laboratory of Functional Neurosciences and Pathology EA4559, 4 rue du Barreau, Villeneuve d'Ascq, 59653, France</aff>
<aff id="I2">
<label>2</label>
Service MPR Neurologique, Centre Hospitalier de Saint-Amand-les-Eaux, 19 rue des Anciens d'A.F.N., Saint-Amand-les-Eaux, 59230, France</aff>
<aff id="I3">
<label>3</label>
Department of Psychology, University of Grenoble, Laboratory of Psychology and NeuroCognition UMR 5105, 1251 avenue centrale, 38040, France</aff>
<pub-date pub-type="collection">
<year>2012</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="epub">
<day>15</day>
<month>3</month>
<year>2012</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>13</volume>
<fpage>28</fpage>
<lpage>28</lpage>
<history>
<date date-type="received">
<day>15</day>
<month>11</month>
<year>2011</year>
</date>
<date date-type="accepted">
<day>15</day>
<month>3</month>
<year>2012</year>
</date>
</history>
<permissions>
<copyright-statement>Copyright ©2012 Luyat et al; licensee BioMed Central Ltd.</copyright-statement>
<copyright-year>2012</copyright-year>
<copyright-holder>Luyat et al; licensee BioMed Central Ltd.</copyright-holder>
<license license-type="open-access" xlink:href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0">
<license-p>This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0">http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0</ext-link>
), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.</license-p>
</license>
</permissions>
<self-uri xlink:href="http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2202/13/28"></self-uri>
<abstract>
<sec>
<title>Background</title>
<p>The subjective visual vertical (SVV, the visual estimation of gravitational direction) is commonly considered as an indicator of the sense of orientation. The present study examined the impact of two methodological factors (the angle size of the stimulus and the participant's gender) on deviations of the SVV caused by head tilt. Forty healthy participants (20 men and 20 women) were asked to make visual vertical adjustments of a light bar with their head held vertically or roll-tilted by 30° to the left or to the right. Line angle sizes of 0.95° and 18.92° were presented.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Results</title>
<p>The SVV tended to move in the direction of head tilt in women but away from the direction of head tilt in men. Moreover, the head-tilt effect was also modulated by the stimulus' angle size. The large angle size led to deviations in the direction of head-tilt, whereas the small angle size had the opposite effect.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Conclusions</title>
<p>Our results showed that gender and line angle size have an impact on the evaluation of the SVV. These findings must be taken into account in the growing body of research that uses the SVV paradigm in disease settings. Moreover, this methodological issue may explain (at least in part) the discrepancies found in the literature on the head-tilt effect.</p>
</sec>
</abstract>
</article-meta>
</front>
<body>
<sec>
<title>Background</title>
<p>On Earth, humans need to have a sense of verticality. In sensorimotor terms, our upright, bipedal, postural stance is mediated by vestibular, somesthetic and visual inputs that serve as indicators of any deviation from the vertical. On the cognitive level, our vertical perception defines a gravitational reference frame, which subserves the coding of the location and orientation of objects in the environment independently of the observer's own orientation. Consequently, the subjective vertical (SV, i.e. the subjective estimation of gravitational direction) is commonly considered as an indicator of the sense of orientation. The SV is measured by asking the observer to align a light bar with the direction of gravity (i.e. the subjective visual vertical, SVV). Other modalities (such as the haptic modality) have been also used (e.g. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B1">1</xref>
-
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B9">9</xref>
]).</p>
<p>The SVV arises from the complex integration of inputs from vestibular, visual, proprioceptive and tactile receptors. It has been clearly established that subcortical structures are involved in the vestibular contribution to oculomotor control (vestibulo-oculomotor reflexes) and postural control (vestibulo-collic and vestibulo-spinal reflexes) (for a review, see [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B10">10</xref>
]). However, it is not known precisely how and where in the cortex the vestibular information on spatial cognition (and on the sense of verticality, in particular) is processed. Otoliths and semicircular canals give rise to vestibular inputs, which run from the eighth nerve to the vestibular nuclei at the pontine level. Studies in monkeys [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B11">11</xref>
] have shown that after thalamic projection, the signals reach (directly or indirectly) several areas of the vestibular cortex (areas 2v, 3a and 7, in particular), the parieto-insular vestibular cortex (PIVC) and the ventral intraparietal area. Functional MRI studies [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B12">12</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B13">13</xref>
] have revealed that similar areas are involved in humans (particularly the PIVC and temporal areas), with right-hemisphere dominance. According to Barra et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B14">14</xref>
], the representation of verticality may depend on neural circuits that include thalamoparietal projections (for somesthetic graviception) and thalamo-insular projections (for vestibular graviception).</p>
<p>Lesions or impairment at any of the steps in vestibular information processing can induce pathological deviations of the SVV or the subjective visual horizontal. Indeed, this paradigm has frequently been used to detect acute, unilateral vestibular defects in disease settings [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B15">15</xref>
]. However, thalamic infarction [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B16">16</xref>
] and cortical damage (especially in parietal areas and, more specifically, damage to the PIVC) can also induce deviation of the SVV (for a review, see [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B17">17</xref>
]). For example, in the study by Brandt et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B18">18</xref>
], contraversive tilts of the SVV were found in 33 out of 52 patients with brain damage in the PIVC area; ocular torsion was ruled out as a possible cause of the deviation. Most studies on stroke patients have reported an altered sense of verticality and a subjective vertical tilted towards the contralesional side - especially in patients suffering from hemineglect syndrome [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B19">19</xref>
-
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B21">21</xref>
]. Nowadays, a growing number of researchers use the SVV paradigm to investigate other diseases, such as paraplegia [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B14">14</xref>
] and psychiatric conditions [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B3">3</xref>
].</p>
<p>However, even healthy observers will suffer from non-negligible, biased accuracy if visual cues are not available and the head axis is no longer relevant (when roll-tilted, for example). Many literature reports show that (i) head tilts of up to 60° can give rise to contraversive displacement of the SVV (the Müller effect, also known as the E-effect) and (ii) greater tilt angles induce systematic deviations in the head tilt direction (the Aubert effect, also known as the A-effect). Nevertheless, a review of the literature on head-tilt effects as a function of the amplitude of head tilt reveals strong disagreements with respect to the specific values and conditions that yield A- or E-effects. For example, several researchers have found systematic A-effects with moderate head tilts of about 30° [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B2">2</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B22">22</xref>
-
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">25</xref>
], whereas others always observed an E-effect under similar conditions [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B26">26</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B27">27</xref>
]. Furthermore, some researchers report high, between-subject variability rather than an average, systematic effect of head tilt [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B28">28</xref>
-
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B30">30</xref>
]. Table
<xref ref-type="table" rid="T1">1</xref>
provides a summary of these various experiments and their main results.</p>
<table-wrap id="T1" position="float">
<label>Table 1</label>
<caption>
<p>Results obtained in different studies on the Subjective Visual Vertical (SVV) or Subjective Visual Horizontal (SVH).</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th align="left">Authors</th>
<th align="left">Sample</th>
<th align="left">Physical size</th>
<th align="left">Distance</th>
<th align="left">Angular size</th>
<th align="left">Orientation</th>
<th align="left">Tilt</th>
<th align="left">Results</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="left">Wade (1969)</td>
<td align="left">10 subjects, sex no specified (ns)</td>
<td align="left">91.5 cm et 15.5 cm (2.1 cm wide)</td>
<td align="left">180 cm</td>
<td align="left">28.52° et 4.93°</td>
<td align="left">SVV</td>
<td align="left">± 30° (Head Tilt)</td>
<td align="left">E-effect on average
<break></break>
Decrease of the E-effect with large bar</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td colspan="8">
<hr></hr>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">De Graaf et al. (1992)</td>
<td align="left">7 men, 5 women (age between 19-32 yr)</td>
<td align="left">50 cm</td>
<td align="left">125 cm</td>
<td align="left">22.6°</td>
<td align="left">SVH</td>
<td align="left">0°, ± 5°, ± 10°, ± 15°, ± 20°, ± 25° (Body Tilt)</td>
<td align="left">On average, no deviation from upright.
<break></break>
5 subjects had E-effect, 3 others an A-effect, the remaining 4 subjects were not easily determined: 3 of them tended to show an E-effect and 1 tended to show an A- effect</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td colspan="8">
<hr></hr>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Tribukait et al. (1996)</td>
<td align="left">39 men and 36 women (37 ans), 3 were excluded but their sex was not specified</td>
<td align="left">ns</td>
<td align="left">ns</td>
<td align="left">6.5°</td>
<td align="left">SVH</td>
<td align="left">0°, ± 10°, ± 20°, ± 30 (Body Tilt)</td>
<td align="left">High between-subjects variability but E-effect on average</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td colspan="8">
<hr></hr>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Guerraz et al. (1998)</td>
<td align="left">35 women and 34 men (age between 18 and 22 yr)</td>
<td align="left">21 cm</td>
<td align="left">ns</td>
<td align="left">20°</td>
<td align="left">SVV</td>
<td align="left">± 28° (Head Tilt)</td>
<td align="left">A-effect</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td colspan="8">
<hr></hr>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Van Beuzekom & Van Gisbergen (2000)</td>
<td align="left">5 men and 1 woman (age between 20 and 54 yr)</td>
<td align="left">ns</td>
<td align="left">100 cm</td>
<td align="left">17°</td>
<td align="left">SVV and SVH</td>
<td align="left">Between -180° and +180° with a 10° interval (Body Tilt)</td>
<td align="left">High between-subjects variability, for small angle of tilt, some subjects show E-effects, others, A-effects</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td colspan="8">
<hr></hr>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Guerraz et al. (2000)</td>
<td align="left">N = 20 sex no specified (age between 18 and 22 yr)</td>
<td align="left">21 cm</td>
<td align="left">60 cm</td>
<td align="left">19.85°</td>
<td align="left">SVV</td>
<td align="left">0° ± 7, ± 14, ± 21, ± 28, ± 35° (Head tilt)</td>
<td align="left">A-effect on average from 7° to 35° of head-tilt</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td colspan="8">
<hr></hr>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Mast (2000)</td>
<td align="left">1 man and 3 women (age between 26 and 32 yr)</td>
<td align="left">ns</td>
<td align="left">120 cm</td>
<td align="left"></td>
<td align="left">SVV</td>
<td align="left">Angles between 0° and 180° at each 15° intervals
<break></break>
0°, 15°, 30, 45°...180° (Body Tilt)</td>
<td align="left">A small (no precise value) A-effect for tilt angle up to 135°</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td colspan="8">
<hr></hr>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Luyat & Gentaz (2002)</td>
<td align="left">7 women and 2 men (mean age = 22.4 yr)</td>
<td align="left">27 cm</td>
<td align="left">68 cm</td>
<td align="left">22.46°</td>
<td align="left">SVV</td>
<td align="left">± 45°</td>
<td align="left">A-effect</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td colspan="8">
<hr></hr>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trousselard et al. (2003)</td>
<td align="left">5 women and 11 men (mean age: 28 yr)</td>
<td align="left">32 cm long (1.5 cm wide)</td>
<td align="left">55 cm</td>
<td align="left">32.44°</td>
<td align="left">SVV</td>
<td align="left">0°, ± 15°, ± 30°, ± 45°, ± 60°, ± 75°, ± 90°, ± 105° (Body Tilt)</td>
<td align="left">A-effect</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td colspan="8">
<hr></hr>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Kaptein & Van Gisbergen (2004)</td>
<td align="left">6 men (age between 23-59 yr)</td>
<td align="left">ns</td>
<td align="left">90 cm</td>
<td align="left">20°</td>
<td align="left">SVV</td>
<td align="left">Angles between 0° and 360° at each 30° intervals
<break></break>
0°, 30, 60°, 90°...360° (Body Tilt)</td>
<td align="left">Limited time of adjustment (30 sec)
<break></break>
For angle ≤ 60°: 2/6: E-effect, 4 gave veridical response or small A-effect (no precise value)</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
<table-wrap-foot>
<p>ns = no-specified</p>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
<p>In fact, if we consider moderate tilts and normal subjects, two main factors appear to vary strongly from one study to another (see Table
<xref ref-type="table" rid="T1">1</xref>
): the angle size and the participants' gender (when this information is mentioned). In experiments showing a systematic A-effect [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B2">2</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B22">22</xref>
-
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">25</xref>
], very large stimuli were used (from 19.85° to 32.44°). Although the effect of the stimulus angle size was tested some time ago by Wade [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B26">26</xref>
], it merits renewed investigation. In fact, Wade found that a large angle size could diminish the E-effect but did not find an A-effect - even with a large angle size (28.52°). Unfortunately, data concerning other aspects of study design (such as whether participants of both genders or just one gender were included) were not specified.</p>
<p>Several spatial tasks show gender effects, although the latter are not completely understood. Anatomical explanations have been proposed [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B31">31</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B32">32</xref>
]. The otolithic organs (i.e. the utricle and saccule) and the superior semicircular canals appear to be larger in men than in women [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B33">33</xref>
]; this may explain (at least in part) gender-related differences in vestibular information processing (see [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B31">31</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B32">32</xref>
]). Since the pioneering work by Asch and Witkin [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B34">34</xref>
], it has been well documented that women are usually more affected by visual, contextual cues (such as those used as in the rod-and-frame test and the water-level task, for example [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B35">35</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B36">36</xref>
]). Gender effects are also frequently mentioned in spatial attention tasks, with poorer performance levels by women (although a correlation with the functional differences revealed by fMRI has not been found [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B37">37</xref>
]). In navigation tasks, significant differences between men and women have also been evidenced [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B38">38</xref>
]. However, gender differences in spatial tasks do not appear to be limited to paradigms involving visual contextual cues, since Tremblay et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B31">31</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B32">32</xref>
] found gender differences in judgment of the morphological horizon in different body orientations. A few studies have sought to identify a gender difference for the SVV during head-tilt (and in the absence of visual context) [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B36">36</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B39">39</xref>
] but failed to do so.</p>
<p>In order to explain some of discrepancies concerning head-tilt effects for the SVV, we decided to investigate the possible impact of methodological factors (the line's angle size and the participant's gender) with a moderate (30°) head tilt. This question is important in view of the growing body of research using the SVV paradigm in various disease settings. Forty healthy participants (20 men and 20 women) were asked to make visual vertical adjustments of a light bar with their head positioned vertically or roll-tilted by 30° to the left or the right. Line angle sizes of 0.95° and 18.92° were presented.</p>
</sec>
<sec sec-type="methods">
<title>Method</title>
<sec>
<title>Participants</title>
<p>The 40 study participants (20 women (mean ± SD age: 23.20 ± 3.22) and 20 men (mean ± SD age: 24.10 ± 3.04)) were all psychology or neuroscience students. A T-test failed to show any difference between the gender groups' respective mean ages (t
<sub>38 </sub>
= -0.90;
<italic>p </italic>
= 0.37). The participants' vision was normal or corrected-to-normal with lenses. According to self-reports, none of the subjects had hearing or vestibular disorders, diseases with ocular effects or motion sickness. The subjects were all volunteers and informed consent was obtained after a full explanation of the experimental procedure. In France, our behavioral, non-invasive, non-medical study did not require approval by an independent ethics committee. However, we did follow the recommendations of the French Psychology Society
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.sfpsy.org/spip.php?rubrique27">http://www.sfpsy.org/spip.php?rubrique27</ext-link>
.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Materials</title>
<p>The equipment used to present the stimulus comprised of a 60-cm-long tunnel in a metal framework (see Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F1">1</xref>
), which was covered by a black cloth during the experiment to block any visual, contextual cues. At the bottom of this device was a rotating metal disc (diameter: 44 cm) fitted with electroluminescent diodes. A rigid black plate with an adjustable aperture (corresponding to the line size) was placed in front of the diodes to provide either a small angle size for the emitted light (angle: 0.95°; length: 1 cm; width: 1 mm) or a large angle size (18.92°; length: 20 cm; width: 1 mm). The disc could be rotated by up to 240° around its central axis. The display was viewed binocularly. The back of the disc was graduated in degrees and the display's sensitivity threshold was ± 0.5°.</p>
<fig id="F1" position="float">
<label>Figure 1</label>
<caption>
<p>
<bold>The experimental apparatus</bold>
. During the experiment, the whole apparatus was covered by a black cloth (to prevent contextual visual cues).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="1471-2202-13-28-1"></graphic>
</fig>
<p>At the aperture of the visual apparatus, the participant's head was held in place at four points (the temples, the top of the head and the chin placed on a padded rest), which allowed the head to be held upright or tilted by 30° to the left or to the right. The participant was seated in front of the visual apparatus at an appropriate height, with his/her head placed in the head rest device.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Experimental conditions and procedures</title>
<p>The participants had to adjust the rod to the gravitational vertical as accurately as possible. No time limit was imposed. A gravitational definition of the vertical was given to the participants (the direction of a plumb line; a concrete example was shown). The two chosen light bar angles (0.95° and 18.92°) were presented in separate sessions, with a 5 min interval. For each angle size condition, the participant adjusted the line in three head posture conditions: (i) the head held vertically (the baseline condition), (ii) the head roll-tilted to the left (- 30°, by convention) and (iii) the head roll tilted to the right (+30°). The baseline condition was performed first and the order of presentation of the left-tilt and right-tilt conditions was randomized. In each of these six experimental conditions (two angles × three head positions), there were four adjustments to the vertical. The initial rod position was 20° away from the physical vertical and the direction of displacement (clockwise and counter-clockwise) was counterbalanced over the four trials.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Results</title>
<p>The results are depicted in Table
<xref ref-type="table" rid="T2">2</xref>
.</p>
<table-wrap id="T2" position="float">
<label>Table 2</label>
<caption>
<p>The visual subjective vertical in the three head conditions as a function of angular size and gender.</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th></th>
<th align="center" colspan="2">Head to the Vertical</th>
<th align="center" colspan="2">Head Tilted to the Left</th>
<th align="center" colspan="2">Head Tilted to the Right</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td></td>
<td align="center" colspan="6">
<bold>Angular size of the luminous bar</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td colspan="7">
<hr></hr>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center">Gender</td>
<td align="center">0.95°</td>
<td align="center">18.92°</td>
<td align="center">0.95°</td>
<td align="center">18.92°</td>
<td align="center">0.95°</td>
<td align="center">18.92°</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td colspan="7">
<hr></hr>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center">Women</td>
<td align="center">-0.394 (0.304)</td>
<td align="center">-0.619 (0.245)</td>
<td align="center">-1.022 (0.731)</td>
<td align="center">-2.500 (0.605)</td>
<td align="center">-0.666 (0.640)</td>
<td align="center">0.503 (0.545)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td colspan="7">
<hr></hr>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td></td>
<td align="center">Min = -3.375</td>
<td align="center">Min = -2.25</td>
<td align="center">Min = -9.5</td>
<td align="center">Min = -8.25</td>
<td align="center">Min = -10.5</td>
<td align="center">Min = -3.75</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td colspan="7">
<hr></hr>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td></td>
<td align="center">Max = 3.4375</td>
<td align="center">Max = 2.125</td>
<td align="center">Max = 2.875</td>
<td align="center">Max = 1.875</td>
<td align="center">Max = 1.875</td>
<td align="center">Max = 5.5</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td colspan="7">
<hr></hr>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center">Men</td>
<td align="center">-1 (0.304)</td>
<td align="center">-1.222 (0.245)</td>
<td align="center">0.744 (0.731)</td>
<td align="center">-0.972 (0.605)</td>
<td align="center">-2.131 (0.640)</td>
<td align="center">-1.334 (0.544)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td colspan="7">
<hr></hr>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td></td>
<td align="center">Min = -3.437</td>
<td align="center">Min = -2.25</td>
<td align="center">Min = -9.5</td>
<td align="center">Min = -8.25</td>
<td align="center">Min = -10.5</td>
<td align="center">Min = -3.75</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td colspan="7">
<hr></hr>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td></td>
<td align="center">Max = 0.875</td>
<td align="center">Max = 0.375</td>
<td align="center">Max = 7.75</td>
<td align="center">Max = 3.375</td>
<td align="center">Max = 1.5</td>
<td align="center">Max = 4.5</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
<table-wrap-foot>
<p>Standard errors of the mean are noted in parentheses (measures expressed in degrees)</p>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
<p>For each trial, the deviation (in degrees) of the line's final position from the gravitational vertical was first noted. By convention, deviations to the left (i.e. with the rod turned counter-clockwise with respect to the gravitational vertical, as viewed by the subject) were counted as negative and deviations to the right (clockwise) were counted as positive. The mean of the algebraic deviations (in degrees) over the four trials in each postural condition was computed. The threshold for statistical significance (i.e. for rejection of the null hypothesis) was set to p < 0.05 for all tests.</p>
<p>The standard assumptions for performing an analysis of variance (ANOVA) were validated, i.e. the normality of the distributions (checked with a Kolmogorov-Smirnov test) and the homogeneity of the variance matrix between groups (checked with Box's M test;
<italic>p </italic>
> 0.40). We performed a two (
<italic>gender</italic>
) × three (
<italic>head position</italic>
) × two (
<italic>angle size</italic>
) factor ANOVA with repeated measures on the latter two factors. This analysis did not reveal a significant effect of
<italic>gender </italic>
(F
<sub>(1,38) </sub>
= 0.344;
<italic>p </italic>
= 0.56) or
<italic>head position</italic>
(F
<sub>(2,76) </sub>
= 0.038;
<italic>p </italic>
= 0.963). In contrast, the interaction between
<italic>head position </italic>
and
<italic>gender </italic>
was statistically significant (F
<sub>(2,76) </sub>
= 5.960;
<italic>p </italic>
= 0.0003 (see Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F2">2</xref>
)). However, an orthogonal contrast analysis revealed a significant difference between women and men in the "right-tilt" condition (F
<sub>(1,38) = </sub>
5.866;
<italic>p </italic>
= 0.02). In the other head positions, the effects showed a trend but failed to achieve statistical significance (F
<sub>(1,38) </sub>
= 3.069,
<italic>p </italic>
= 0.08, for the "head vertical" condition; F
<sub>(1,38) </sub>
= 3.721,
<italic>p </italic>
= 0.06 for "left-tilt").</p>
<fig id="F2" position="float">
<label>Figure 2</label>
<caption>
<p>
<bold>The subjective visual vertical (as the mean of algebraic deviations (MAD), in degrees) in women and in men as a function of the head position</bold>
. The vertical bars represent the 95% confidence interval.</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="1471-2202-13-28-2"></graphic>
</fig>
<p>Even though the mean size of the deviations was moderate (with high between-subject variability; see Table
<xref ref-type="table" rid="T2">2</xref>
), the effect of the head-tilt differed according to
<italic>gender</italic>
. In women, a deviation in the direction of head tilt (i.e. an A-effect) was observed (vs. the baseline, "head vertical" condition), whereas the opposite was found in men (i.e. an E-effect).</p>
<p>The ANOVA also revealed an interaction between
<italic>head position </italic>
and
<italic>angle size </italic>
(F
<sub>(2,76) </sub>
= 10.549;
<italic>p </italic>
< .00001 (see Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F3">3</xref>
)). An orthogonal contrast analysis showed that the effect of angle size was significant for the head-tilted conditions (t
<sub>(38) </sub>
= 3.87,
<italic>p </italic>
< 0.001, for "left-tilt", t
<sub>(38) </sub>
= 2.00,
<italic>p </italic>
= 0.025, for "right-tilt") but not for the "head vertical" condition (t
<sub>(38) </sub>
= 1.24;
<italic>p </italic>
= 0.11).</p>
<fig id="F3" position="float">
<label>Figure 3</label>
<caption>
<p>The subjective visual vertical (as the mean of algebraic deviations (MAD), in degrees) as a function of the tilt direction.</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="1471-2202-13-28-3"></graphic>
</fig>
<p>When adjustments were made with a small angle size (0.95°), they tended to deviate away from the direction of the head tilt. In contrast, a large angle size (18.92°) led to deviations in the direction of the head tilt. None of the other single effects or interactions was significant (
<italic>p </italic>
> 0.19 for the effect of angle size and Fs < 1 for all the other factors and interactions).</p>
<p>In order to clarify the additive effects of gender and angle size on the SVV, we further analyzed the "head-tilt indexes". To that end, the mean obtained in the baseline condition (i.e. the head aligned with the vertical) was subtracted from the mean computed for the left- or right-tilted head condition, in order to isolate a "pure" head-tilt effect. Moreover, each mean was expressed with respect to the head axis (30°), in order to rule out the normal, reverse deviation caused by head-tilt to the left or to the right, respectively. Thus, an accurate gravitational judgment would be equal to 30°. In contrast, a value below 30° would reflect an A-effect and a value above 30° would reflect an E-effect.</p>
<p>The standard assumptions for performing ANOVA were observed, i.e. a normal data distribution (checked with a Kolmogorov-Smirnov test) and a homogeneous variance-covariance matrix (checked with a Box's M test). We performed a two (
<italic>gender</italic>
) × two (
<italic>head position</italic>
) × two (
<italic>angle size</italic>
) factor ANOVA with repeated measures on the two latter factors. This analysis revealed a significant effect of angle size (F
<sub>(1,38) </sub>
= 12.654;
<italic>p </italic>
= 0.001). When adjustments were made with the small angle size (0.95°), the errors tended to be in the opposite direction to that of the head tilt (M = 30.630°; an E-effect). In contrast, a large angle size (18.92°) led to a slight deviation in the direction of the head (M = 29.340°; an A-effect). The effect of gender was also significant (F
<sub>(1,38) </sub>
= 6.307;
<italic>p </italic>
= 0.016). Men generally showed an E-effect (M = 30.809°), whereas women generally showed an A-effect (M = 29.160°). The two factors
<italic>gender </italic>
and
<italic>angle size </italic>
had additive effects, since the interaction between them was not statistically significant (F < 1; see Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F4">4</xref>
).</p>
<fig id="F4" position="float">
<label>Figure 4</label>
<caption>
<p>
<bold>Deviations from the head axis (in degrees), as a function of gender and line angle size</bold>
. The vertical bars represent the 95% confidence interval.</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="1471-2202-13-28-4"></graphic>
</fig>
<p>There was no significant effect of
<italic>head tilt </italic>
(F < 1). The SVV with the head tilted to the left (M = 29.871°) did not differ from that obtained with the head tilted to the right (M = 30.098°). The interaction between
<italic>gender </italic>
and
<italic>head tilt </italic>
failed to achieve statistical significance (F
<sub>(1,38) </sub>
= 2.667; MSE = 5.4;
<italic>p </italic>
= 0.11). All other interactions were non-significant (F < 1).</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Discussion</title>
<p>Our study results showed moderate SVV effects with a 30° head tilt. However, our experiment notably revealed that two methodological factors (the participant's gender and the angle size of the stimulus) had a statistically significant effect on the SVV in general and on the head-tilt effect in particular. Our second data analysis revealed that gender and angle size had additive effects on the deviation of the estimated SVV. The women tended to deviate their SVV in the direction of the head-tilt, whereas the opposite effect was found in men. Moreover, the head-tilt effect was modulated by the stimulus angle size. A large stimulus angle size led to deviations in the direction of the head-tilt, whereas a small angle size gave the opposite effect.</p>
<p>We suggest that these methodological factors could be partly involved in some of the differences found in the literature concerning the effect of head-tilt on the SVV. For instance, an experiment in which the SVV is tested with a large stimulus angle size in a predominantly women group (e.g. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B23">23</xref>
]) would be more likely to give rise to an A-effect (i.e. under-compensation of head-tilt; deviation of the SVV towards the head). In contrast, an experiment conducted with presentation of small angle size in a group of men would be more likely to an E-effect (i.e. overcompensation of head-tilt; deviation away from the head) on average. In the two other cases (women with a small angle size and men with a large stimulus angle size), the mean SVV may be close to the true physical vertical and would not display systematic deviation.</p>
<p>The difference between men and women observed here contrasts with the lack of an effect in previous studies [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B36">36</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B39">39</xref>
]. However, our results fit well with Tremblay's assertion [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B32">32</xref>
] that gender differences in the perception of spatial orientation are linked to anatomical differences in the vestibular apparatus. As mentioned above, Sato et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B33">33</xref>
] found that the otoliths and superior semicircular canals are larger in men than in women. According to Tremblay [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B32">32</xref>
], this physiological difference may mean that women are poorly sensitive to vestibular information. The higher weighting given to vestibular signals could explain the men's propensity for an E-effect via overcompensation of head tilt (see Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F4">4</xref>
).</p>
<p>Our results on the effect of angle size agree rather well with the previous work by Wade [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B26">26</xref>
], i.e. an increase in the E-effect with a small-sized stimulus angle. Although the gender of the ten participants was not stated in Wade's paper, an assessment of solely male participants would explain the lack of an A-effect in the 28.52° angle size condition.</p>
<p>Although the overall effects remain difficult to explain, some degree of speculation is justified. The effect of the angle size factor could be studied by rigorous investigation of eye movements and restriction of the allowed adjustment time (to avoid scanning movements, for example). Indeed, head tilt induces a vestibulo-ocular reflex (ocular countertorsion) that might lead to a visual E-effect (i.e. overestimation of head tilt) if the system does not take into account the slight countertorsion of the retinal meridians (usually about 10% of the head-tilt). Some researchers have considered that countertorsion is the predominant factor in SVV deviations [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B40">40</xref>
] and SVV variability [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B41">41</xref>
], although other researchers failed to find a link between eye torsion and the SVV [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">25</xref>
].</p>
<p>Moreover, angle size differences could trigger additional eye movements. Although the 0.95° stimulus may be foveated, saccades and volitional eye movements are certainly required to fully view an 18.9°-sized line. Even though it is now clear that several regions of the human cortex contain orientation-selective neurons (V1, V2, V3, V3A and V4 in particular; for a review, see [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B42">42</xref>
]), it is still not known where in the brain vestibular signals are integrated with visual signals (in order to convert the representation of visual stimulus from a retinal frame of reference into a world-centered frame of reference [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B43">43</xref>
]). In humans, Corbett et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B44">44</xref>
] used event-related potential experiments to show that in the presence of a tilted frame, the SVV involved later, post-perceptual information processing. Moreover, Vandenberghe et al.'s PET study [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B45">45</xref>
] showed that the brain areas involved in orientation discrimination are activated differently by a main stimulus and by the addition of a peripheral stimulus. The two stimuli used in our present research, a centrally viewed bar (0.95°) and a more peripherally viewed bar (18.95°), may have activated different cortical areas and thus generated different behavioral results (i.e. different SVV deviations).</p>
<p>Our study had several limitations. Firstly, the deviations of the SVV found with a 30° head tilt were quite moderate. One can legitimately postulate that factors not taken into account here (such as handedness) may have contributed to the great between-subject variability and thus the small overall deviation induced by head tilt. Thus, it would be useful to control for this factor and increase the number of participants. The second limitation relates to the fact that only a moderate head tilt (30°) was studied here; it would be interesting to investigate several larger tilt angles. Thirdly, it is difficult to interpret our data from a physiological point of view. However, as suggested by one of the reviewers of this article, it would be useful to check the validity of our findings by estimating both the subjective visual and haptic verticals in the same participants. This would enable us to determine whether the effects of gender and angle size observed here are linked to the central processing of visual information or, in contrast, otolithic information (see [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B8">8</xref>
]).</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Conclusion</title>
<p>In summary, our study revealed the impact of two factors on evaluation of the SVV: the angle size of the stimulus and the participant's gender. Given the growing body of research that uses the SVV paradigm as an indicator of spatial orientation (particularly in disease settings), this methodological issue is important and may explain (at least in part) the discrepancies found in the literature on the head-tilt effect.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Authors' contributions</title>
<p>ML conceived the design of the study, helped in the statistical analysis and wrote the manuscript. MN carried out the experiment, performed the statistical analysis and helped to draft the manuscript. VT carried out the experiment, performed the statistical analysis and helped to review the manuscript. EG helped to design the study and helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.</p>
</sec>
</body>
<back>
<sec>
<title>Acknowledgements</title>
<p>This work was funded by grants from the National Centre for Scientific Research (CNRS). We thank Claire Leconte-Lambert, Romaric Marcilly and all the study participants for their help with this research. We thank the anonymous reviewers of this article for their enlightening comments. We also thank Dr David Fraser (Biotech Communication, Damery, France) for his helpful advice on the English language in this paper.</p>
</sec>
<ref-list>
<ref id="B1">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Bauermeister</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Werner</surname>
<given-names>H</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wapner</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>The effect of body tilt on tactual-kinesthetic perception of verticality</article-title>
<source>Am J Psychol</source>
<year>1964</year>
<volume>77</volume>
<fpage>451</fpage>
<lpage>456</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/1421016</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14198668</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B2">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Guerraz</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Luyat</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Poquin</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ohlmann</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>The role of neck afferents in subjective orientation in the visual and tactile sensory modalities</article-title>
<source>Acta Otolaryngol</source>
<year>2000</year>
<volume>120</volume>
<issue>6</issue>
<fpage>735</fpage>
<lpage>738</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/000164800750000261</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11099150</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B3">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Guardia</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cottencin</surname>
<given-names>O</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thomas</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dodin</surname>
<given-names>V</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Luyat</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Spatial orientation constancy is impaired in anorexia nervosa</article-title>
<source>Psychiatry Res</source>
<year>2012</year>
<volume>195</volume>
<issue>1-2</issue>
<fpage>56</fpage>
<lpage>59</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.psychres.2011.08.003</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21872340</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B4">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Lejeune</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thouvarecq</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Anderson</surname>
<given-names>DJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Caston</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jouen</surname>
<given-names>F</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Kinaesthetic and visual perceptions of orientations</article-title>
<source>Perception</source>
<year>2009</year>
<volume>38</volume>
<issue>7</issue>
<fpage>988</fpage>
<lpage>1001</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1068/p6132</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19764301</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B5">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Luyat</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gentaz</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Corte</surname>
<given-names>TR</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Guerraz</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Reference frames and haptic perception of orientation: body and head tilt effects on the oblique effect</article-title>
<source>Percept Psychophys</source>
<year>2001</year>
<volume>63</volume>
<issue>3</issue>
<fpage>541</fpage>
<lpage>554</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3758/BF03194419</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11414140</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B6">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Luyat</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Verticale subjective versus verticale posturale: une note sur l'étude de la perception de la verticale (Subjective vertical vs postural vertical: a note on the perception of verticality</article-title>
<source>L'Année Psychologique</source>
<year>1997</year>
<volume>97</volume>
<fpage>433</fpage>
<lpage>447</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22423300</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B7">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Wright</surname>
<given-names>WG</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Glasauer</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Haptic subjective vertical shows context dependence: task and vision play a role during dynamic tilt stimulation</article-title>
<source>Ann N Y Acad Sci</source>
<year>2003</year>
<volume>1004</volume>
<fpage>531</fpage>
<lpage>535</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1196/annals.1303.069</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14662511</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B8">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Schuler</surname>
<given-names>JR</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bockisch</surname>
<given-names>CJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Straumann</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tarnutzer</surname>
<given-names>AA</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Precision and accuracy of the subjective haptic vertical in the roll plane</article-title>
<source>BMC Neurosci</source>
<year>2010</year>
<volume>11</volume>
<fpage>83</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1186/1471-2202-11-83</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20630097</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B9">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Funk</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Finke</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Muller</surname>
<given-names>HJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Preger</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kerkhoff</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Systematic biases in the tactile perception of the subjective vertical in patients with unilateral neglect and the influence of upright vs. supine posture</article-title>
<source>Neuropsychologia</source>
<year>2010</year>
<volume>48</volume>
<issue>1</issue>
<fpage>298</fpage>
<lpage>308</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.018</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19782092</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B10">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Schwarz</surname>
<given-names>U</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Neuro-ophthalmology: a brief Vademecum</article-title>
<source>Eur J Radiol</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>49</volume>
<issue>1</issue>
<fpage>31</fpage>
<lpage>63</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14975493</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B11">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Guldin</surname>
<given-names>WO</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Grüsser</surname>
<given-names>O-J</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Is there a vestibular cortex?</article-title>
<source>Trends Neurosci</source>
<year>1998</year>
<volume>21</volume>
<issue>6</issue>
<fpage>254</fpage>
<lpage>259</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01211-3</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">9641538</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B12">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Fasold</surname>
<given-names>O</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>von Brevern</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kuhberg</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ploner</surname>
<given-names>CJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Villringer</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lempert</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wenzel</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Human vestibular cortex as identified with caloric stimulation in functional magnetic resonance imaging</article-title>
<source>NeuroImage</source>
<year>2002</year>
<volume>17</volume>
<issue>3</issue>
<fpage>1384</fpage>
<lpage>1393</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1006/nimg.2002.1241</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12414278</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B13">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Suzuki</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kitano</surname>
<given-names>H</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ito</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kitanishi</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Yazawa</surname>
<given-names>Y</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ogawa</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Shiino</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kitajima</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Cortical and subcortical vestibular response to caloric stimulation detected by functional magnetic resonance imaging</article-title>
<source>Cognitive Brain Res</source>
<year>2001</year>
<volume>12</volume>
<issue>3</issue>
<fpage>441</fpage>
<lpage>449</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00080-5</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B14">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Barra</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Marquer</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Joassin</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Reymond</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Metge</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Chauvineau</surname>
<given-names>V</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Perennou</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Humans use internal models to construct and update a sense of verticality</article-title>
<source>Brain</source>
<year>2010</year>
<volume>133</volume>
<issue>Pt 12</issue>
<fpage>3552</fpage>
<lpage>3563</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21097492</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B15">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Bergenius</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tribukait</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brantberg</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>The subjective horizontal at different angles of roll-tilt in patients with unilateral vestibular impairment</article-title>
<source>Brain Res Bull</source>
<year>1996</year>
<volume>40</volume>
<issue>5-6</issue>
<fpage>385</fpage>
<lpage>390</lpage>
<comment>discussion 390-381</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0361-9230(96)00131-1</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8886363</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B16">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Anastasopoulos</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bronstein</surname>
<given-names>AM</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>A case of thalamic syndrome: somatosensory influences on visual orientation</article-title>
<source>J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry</source>
<year>1999</year>
<volume>67</volume>
<issue>3</issue>
<fpage>390</fpage>
<lpage>394</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1136/jnnp.67.3.390</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10449566</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B17">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Karnath</surname>
<given-names>HO</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dieterich</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Spatial neglect-a vestibular disorder?</article-title>
<source>Brain</source>
<year>2006</year>
<volume>129</volume>
<issue>Pt 2</issue>
<fpage>293</fpage>
<lpage>305</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16371409</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B18">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Brandt</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dieterich</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Danek</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Vestibular cortex lesions affect the perception of verticality</article-title>
<source>Ann Neurol</source>
<year>1994</year>
<volume>35</volume>
<fpage>403</fpage>
<lpage>412</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1002/ana.410350406</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8154866</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B19">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Kerkhoff</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zoelch</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Disorders of visual orientation in the frontal plane in patients with visual neglect following right or left parietal lesions</article-title>
<source>Exp Brain Res</source>
<year>1998</year>
<volume>122</volume>
<issue>1</issue>
<fpage>108</fpage>
<lpage>120</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s002210050497</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">9772118</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B20">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Funk</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Finke</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Muller</surname>
<given-names>HJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Utz</surname>
<given-names>KS</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kerkhoff</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Effects of lateral head inclination on multimodal spatial orientation judgments in neglect: evidence for impaired spatial orientation constancy</article-title>
<source>Neuropsychologia</source>
<year>2010</year>
<volume>48</volume>
<issue>6</issue>
<fpage>1616</fpage>
<lpage>1627</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.01.029</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20138897</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B21">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Funk</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Finke</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Muller</surname>
<given-names>HJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Utz</surname>
<given-names>KS</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kerkhoff</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Visual context modulates the subjective vertical in neglect: evidence for an increased rod-and-frame-effect</article-title>
<source>Neuroscience</source>
<year>2011</year>
<volume>173</volume>
<fpage>124</fpage>
<lpage>134</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21073929</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B22">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Guerraz</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Poquin</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Luyat</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ohlmann</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Head orientation involvement in assessment of the subjective vertical during whole body tilt</article-title>
<source>Percept Mot Skills</source>
<year>1998</year>
<volume>87</volume>
<issue>2</issue>
<fpage>643</fpage>
<lpage>648</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2466/pms.1998.87.2.643</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">9842617</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B23">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Luyat</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gentaz</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Body tilt effect on the reproduction of orientations: studies on the visual oblique effect and subjective orientations</article-title>
<source>J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform</source>
<year>2002</year>
<volume>28</volume>
<issue>4</issue>
<fpage>1002</fpage>
<lpage>1011</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12190248</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B24">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Trousselard</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cian</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nougier</surname>
<given-names>V</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pla</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Raphel</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Contribution of somesthetic cues to the perception of body orientation and subjective visual vertical</article-title>
<source>Percept Psychophys</source>
<year>2003</year>
<volume>65</volume>
<issue>8</issue>
<fpage>1179</fpage>
<lpage>1187</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3758/BF03194843</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14710953</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B25">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Mast</surname>
<given-names>FW</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Does the world rock when the eyes roll? Allocentric orientation representation, ocular counterroll, and the subjective vertical</article-title>
<source>Swiss J Psychol</source>
<year>2000</year>
<volume>59</volume>
<fpage>89</fpage>
<lpage>101</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1024//1421-0185.59.2.89</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B26">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Wade</surname>
<given-names>NJ</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>The effect of stimulus line variations on visual orientation with head upright and tilted</article-title>
<source>Aust J Psychol</source>
<year>1969</year>
<volume>21</volume>
<issue>2</issue>
<fpage>177</fpage>
<lpage>185</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/00049536908257782</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B27">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Tribukait</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bergenius</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brantberg</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>The subjective visual horizontal for different body tilts in the roll plane: characterization of normal subjects</article-title>
<source>Brain Res Bull</source>
<year>1996</year>
<volume>40</volume>
<fpage>(5</fpage>
<lpage>6):375-381</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B28">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>De Graaf</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bekkering</surname>
<given-names>H</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Erasmus</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bles</surname>
<given-names>W</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Influence of visual, vestibular, cervical, and somatosensory tilt information on ocular rotation and perception of the horizontal</article-title>
<source>J Vest Res</source>
<year>1992</year>
<volume>2</volume>
<fpage>15</fpage>
<lpage>30</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B29">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Kaptein</surname>
<given-names>RG</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Van Gisbergen</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Interpretation of a discontinuity in the sense of verticality at large body tilt</article-title>
<source>J Neurophysiol</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>91</volume>
<issue>5</issue>
<fpage>2205</fpage>
<lpage>2214</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1152/jn.00804.2003</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14668294</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B30">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Van Beuzekom</surname>
<given-names>AD</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Van Gisbergen</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Properties of the internal representation of gravity inferred from spatial-direction and body-tilt estimates</article-title>
<source>J Neurophysiol</source>
<year>2000</year>
<volume>84</volume>
<issue>1</issue>
<fpage>11</fpage>
<lpage>27</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10899179</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B31">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Tremblay</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Elliott</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Sex differences in judging self-orientation: the morphological horizon and body pitch</article-title>
<source>BMC Neurosci</source>
<year>2007</year>
<volume>8</volume>
<fpage>6</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1186/1471-2202-8-6</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17207289</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B32">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Tremblay</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Elliott</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Starkes</surname>
<given-names>JL</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Gender differences in perception of self-orientation: software or hardware?</article-title>
<source>Perception</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>33</volume>
<issue>3</issue>
<fpage>329</fpage>
<lpage>337</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1068/p5209</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15176617</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B33">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Sato</surname>
<given-names>H</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sando</surname>
<given-names>I</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Takahashi</surname>
<given-names>H</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Computer-aided three-dimensional measurement of the human vestibular apparatus</article-title>
<source>Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg</source>
<year>1992</year>
<volume>107</volume>
<issue>3</issue>
<fpage>405</fpage>
<lpage>409</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">1408226</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B34">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Asch</surname>
<given-names>SE</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Witkin</surname>
<given-names>HA</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Studies in space orientation: II. Perception of the upright with displaced visual field and body tilted</article-title>
<source>J Exp Psychol</source>
<year>1948</year>
<volume>38</volume>
<fpage>455</fpage>
<lpage>477</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18874603</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B35">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Robert</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ohlmann</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Water-level representation by men and women as a function of rod-and-frame proficiency and visual and postural information</article-title>
<source>Perception</source>
<year>1994</year>
<volume>23</volume>
<fpage>1321</fpage>
<lpage>1333</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1068/p231321</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">7761243</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B36">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Barnett-Cowan</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dyde</surname>
<given-names>RT</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thompson</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Harris</surname>
<given-names>LR</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Multisensory determinants of orientation perception: task-specific sex differences</article-title>
<source>Eur J Neurosci</source>
<year>2010</year>
<volume>31</volume>
<issue>10</issue>
<fpage>1899</fpage>
<lpage>1907</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07199.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20584195</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B37">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Bell</surname>
<given-names>EC</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Willson</surname>
<given-names>MC</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wilman</surname>
<given-names>AH</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dave</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Silverstone</surname>
<given-names>PH</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Males and females differ in brain activation during cognitive tasks</article-title>
<source>NeuroImage</source>
<year>2006</year>
<volume>30</volume>
<issue>2</issue>
<fpage>529</fpage>
<lpage>538</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.049</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16260156</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B38">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Viaud-Delmon</surname>
<given-names>I</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ivanenko</surname>
<given-names>YP</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Berthoz</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jouvent</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Sex, lies and virtual reality</article-title>
<source>Nat Neurosci</source>
<year>1998</year>
<volume>1</volume>
<issue>1</issue>
<fpage>15</fpage>
<lpage>16</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1038/215</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10195102</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B39">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Groberg</surname>
<given-names>DH</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dustman</surname>
<given-names>RE</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Beck</surname>
<given-names>EC</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>The effect of body and head tilt in the perception of vertical: Comparison of body and head tilt with left and right handed, male and female subjects</article-title>
<source>Neuropsychologia</source>
<year>1969</year>
<volume>7</volume>
<fpage>89</fpage>
<lpage>100</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0028-3932(69)90048-7</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B40">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Wade</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Curthoys</surname>
<given-names>I</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>The effect of ocular torsional position on perception of the roll-tilt of visual stimuli</article-title>
<source>Vis Res</source>
<year>1997</year>
<volume>37</volume>
<issue>8</issue>
<fpage>1071</fpage>
<lpage>1078</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00252-0</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">9196725</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B41">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Tarnutzer</surname>
<given-names>AA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bockisch</surname>
<given-names>CJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Straumann</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Head roll dependent variability of subjective visual vertical and ocular counterroll</article-title>
<source>Exp Brain Res</source>
<year>2009</year>
<volume>195</volume>
<issue>4</issue>
<fpage>621</fpage>
<lpage>626</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s00221-009-1823-4</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19415246</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B42">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Orban</surname>
<given-names>GA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Vogels</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>The neuronal machinery involved in successive orientation discrimination</article-title>
<source>Prog Neurobiol</source>
<year>1998</year>
<volume>55</volume>
<issue>2</issue>
<fpage>117</fpage>
<lpage>147</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00010-0</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">9618746</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B43">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Daddaoua</surname>
<given-names>N</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dicke</surname>
<given-names>PW</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thier</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>The subjective visual vertical in a nonhuman primate</article-title>
<source>J Vis</source>
<year>2008</year>
<volume>8</volume>
<issue>3</issue>
<fpage>19 11</fpage>
<lpage>18</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18484825</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B44">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Corbett</surname>
<given-names>JE</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Enns</surname>
<given-names>JT</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Handy</surname>
<given-names>TC</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>Electrophysiological evidence for a post-perceptual influence of global visual context on perceived orientation</article-title>
<source>Brain Res</source>
<year>2009</year>
<volume>1292</volume>
<fpage>82</fpage>
<lpage>92</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19632209</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B45">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Vandenberghe</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dupont</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>De Bruyn</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bormans</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Michiels</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mortelmans</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Orban</surname>
<given-names>GA</given-names>
</name>
<article-title>The influence of stimulus location on the brain activation pattern in detection and orientation discrimination. A PET study of visual attention</article-title>
<source>Brain</source>
<year>1996</year>
<volume>119</volume>
<issue>Pt 4</issue>
<fpage>1263</fpage>
<lpage>1276</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8813289</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</pmc>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Ticri/CIDE/explor/HapticV1/Data/Pmc/Curation
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 001B85 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Curation/biblio.hfd -nk 001B85 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Ticri/CIDE
   |area=    HapticV1
   |flux=    Pmc
   |étape=   Curation
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     PMC:3329413
   |texte=   Gender and line size factors modulate the deviations of the subjective visual vertical induced by head tilt
}}

Pour générer des pages wiki

HfdIndexSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Curation/RBID.i   -Sk "pubmed:22420467" \
       | HfdSelect -Kh $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Curation/biblio.hfd   \
       | NlmPubMed2Wicri -a HapticV1 

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.23.
Data generation: Mon Jun 13 01:09:46 2016. Site generation: Wed Mar 6 09:54:07 2024