Serveur d'exploration sur les dispositifs haptiques

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

A sharp image or a sharp knife: norms for the modality-exclusivity of 774 concept-property items

Identifieur interne : 000765 ( Pmc/Curation ); précédent : 000764; suivant : 000766

A sharp image or a sharp knife: norms for the modality-exclusivity of 774 concept-property items

Auteurs : Saskia Van Dantzig [Pays-Bas] ; Rosemary A. Cowell [États-Unis] ; René Zeelenberg [Pays-Bas] ; Diane Pecher [Pays-Bas]

Source :

RBID : PMC:3048290

Abstract

According to recent embodied cognition theories, mental concepts are represented by modality-specific sensory-motor systems. Much of the evidence for modality-specificity in conceptual processing comes from the property-verification task. When applying this and other tasks, it is important to select items based on their modality-exclusivity. We collected modality ratings for a set of 387 properties, each of which was paired with two different concepts, yielding a total of 774 concept-property items. For each item, participants rated the degree to which the property could be experienced through five perceptual modalities (vision, audition, touch, smell, and taste). Based on these ratings, we computed a measure of modality exclusivity, the degree to which a property is perceived exclusively through one sensory modality. In this paper, we briefly sketch the theoretical background of conceptual knowledge, discuss the use of the property-verification task in cognitive research, provide our norms and statistics, and validate the norms in a memory experiment. We conclude that our norms are important for researchers studying modality-specific effects in conceptual processing.

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.3758/s13428-010-0038-8) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.


Url:
DOI: 10.3758/s13428-010-0038-8
PubMed: 21287109
PubMed Central: 3048290

Links toward previous steps (curation, corpus...)


Links to Exploration step

PMC:3048290

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI>
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">A sharp image or a sharp knife: norms for the modality-exclusivity of 774 concept-property items</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Dantzig, Saskia" sort="Van Dantzig, Saskia" uniqKey="Van Dantzig S" first="Saskia" last="Van Dantzig">Saskia Van Dantzig</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff1">Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Pays-Bas</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Leiden University, Leiden</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff4">Cognitive Psychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Wassenaarseweg 52, Room 2B11, PO Box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Pays-Bas</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Cognitive Psychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Wassenaarseweg 52, Room 2B11, PO Box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cowell, Rosemary A" sort="Cowell, Rosemary A" uniqKey="Cowell R" first="Rosemary A." last="Cowell">Rosemary A. Cowell</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="2">
<nlm:aff id="Aff2">University of California, San Diego, CA USA</nlm:aff>
<country>États-Unis</country>
<placeName>
<region type="state">Californie</region>
</placeName>
<wicri:cityArea>University of California, San Diego</wicri:cityArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zeelenberg, Rene" sort="Zeelenberg, Rene" uniqKey="Zeelenberg R" first="René" last="Zeelenberg">René Zeelenberg</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff3">Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Pays-Bas</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pecher, Diane" sort="Pecher, Diane" uniqKey="Pecher D" first="Diane" last="Pecher">Diane Pecher</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff3">Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Pays-Bas</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">PMC</idno>
<idno type="pmid">21287109</idno>
<idno type="pmc">3048290</idno>
<idno type="url">http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3048290</idno>
<idno type="RBID">PMC:3048290</idno>
<idno type="doi">10.3758/s13428-010-0038-8</idno>
<date when="2010">2010</date>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Corpus">000765</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Curation">000765</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title xml:lang="en" level="a" type="main">A sharp image or a sharp knife: norms for the modality-exclusivity of 774 concept-property items</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Dantzig, Saskia" sort="Van Dantzig, Saskia" uniqKey="Van Dantzig S" first="Saskia" last="Van Dantzig">Saskia Van Dantzig</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff1">Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Pays-Bas</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Leiden University, Leiden</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff4">Cognitive Psychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Wassenaarseweg 52, Room 2B11, PO Box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Pays-Bas</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Cognitive Psychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Wassenaarseweg 52, Room 2B11, PO Box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cowell, Rosemary A" sort="Cowell, Rosemary A" uniqKey="Cowell R" first="Rosemary A." last="Cowell">Rosemary A. Cowell</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="2">
<nlm:aff id="Aff2">University of California, San Diego, CA USA</nlm:aff>
<country>États-Unis</country>
<placeName>
<region type="state">Californie</region>
</placeName>
<wicri:cityArea>University of California, San Diego</wicri:cityArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zeelenberg, Rene" sort="Zeelenberg, Rene" uniqKey="Zeelenberg R" first="René" last="Zeelenberg">René Zeelenberg</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff3">Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Pays-Bas</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pecher, Diane" sort="Pecher, Diane" uniqKey="Pecher D" first="Diane" last="Pecher">Diane Pecher</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff3">Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Pays-Bas</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<series>
<title level="j">Behavior Research Methods</title>
<idno type="ISSN">1554-351X</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1554-3528</idno>
<imprint>
<date when="2010">2010</date>
</imprint>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">
<p>According to recent embodied cognition theories, mental concepts are represented by modality-specific sensory-motor systems. Much of the evidence for modality-specificity in conceptual processing comes from the property-verification task. When applying this and other tasks, it is important to select items based on their modality-exclusivity. We collected modality ratings for a set of 387 properties, each of which was paired with two different concepts, yielding a total of 774 concept-property items. For each item, participants rated the degree to which the property could be experienced through five perceptual modalities (vision, audition, touch, smell, and taste). Based on these ratings, we computed a measure of modality exclusivity, the degree to which a property is perceived exclusively through one sensory modality. In this paper, we briefly sketch the theoretical background of conceptual knowledge, discuss the use of the property-verification task in cognitive research, provide our norms and statistics, and validate the norms in a memory experiment. We conclude that our norms are important for researchers studying modality-specific effects in conceptual processing.</p>
<sec>
<title>Electronic supplementary material</title>
<p>The online version of this article (doi:10.3758/s13428-010-0038-8) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.</p>
</sec>
</div>
</front>
<back>
<div1 type="bibliography">
<listBibl>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Baayen, Rh" uniqKey="Baayen R">RH Baayen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Piepenbrock, Rv" uniqKey="Piepenbrock R">Rv Piepenbrock</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Rijn, H" uniqKey="Van Rijn H">H van Rijn</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barclay, Jr" uniqKey="Barclay J">JR Barclay</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bransford, Jd" uniqKey="Bransford J">JD Bransford</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Franks, Jj" uniqKey="Franks J">JJ Franks</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mccarrel, Ns" uniqKey="Mccarrel N">NS McCarrel</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nitsch, K" uniqKey="Nitsch K">K Nitsch</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, Lw" uniqKey="Barsalou L">LW Barsalou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, Lw" uniqKey="Barsalou L">LW Barsalou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, Lw" uniqKey="Barsalou L">LW Barsalou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, Lw" uniqKey="Barsalou L">LW Barsalou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, Lw" uniqKey="Barsalou L">LW Barsalou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Caramazza, A" uniqKey="Caramazza A">A Caramazza</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shelton, Jr" uniqKey="Shelton J">JR Shelton</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cree, Gs" uniqKey="Cree G">GS Cree</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcrae, K" uniqKey="Mcrae K">K McRae</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Farah, Mj" uniqKey="Farah M">MJ Farah</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcclelland, Jl" uniqKey="Mcclelland J">JL McClelland</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Goldberg, Rf" uniqKey="Goldberg R">RF Goldberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Perfetti, Ca" uniqKey="Perfetti C">CA Perfetti</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schneider, W" uniqKey="Schneider W">W Schneider</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grush, R" uniqKey="Grush R">R Grush</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Halff, Hm" uniqKey="Halff H">HM Halff</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ortony, A" uniqKey="Ortony A">A Ortony</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Anderson, Rc" uniqKey="Anderson R">RC Anderson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Humphreys, Gw" uniqKey="Humphreys G">GW Humphreys</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Forde, Eme" uniqKey="Forde E">EME Forde</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kan, Ip" uniqKey="Kan I">IP Kan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, Lw" uniqKey="Barsalou L">LW Barsalou</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Solomon, Ko" uniqKey="Solomon K">KO Solomon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Minor, Jk" uniqKey="Minor J">JK Minor</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thompson Schill, Sl" uniqKey="Thompson Schill S">SL Thompson-Schill</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Landauer, T" uniqKey="Landauer T">T Landauer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dumais, S" uniqKey="Dumais S">S Dumais</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lynott, D" uniqKey="Lynott D">D Lynott</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Connell, L" uniqKey="Connell L">L Connell</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Marques, Jm" uniqKey="Marques J">JM Marques</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Murphy, Gl" uniqKey="Murphy G">GL Murphy</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Andrew, Jm" uniqKey="Andrew J">JM Andrew</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pecher, D" uniqKey="Pecher D">D Pecher</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dantzig, S" uniqKey="Dantzig S">S Dantzig</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schifferstein, Hnj" uniqKey="Schifferstein H">HNJ Schifferstein</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pecher, D" uniqKey="Pecher D">D Pecher</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dantzig, S" uniqKey="Dantzig S">S Dantzig</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zwaan, Ra" uniqKey="Zwaan R">RA Zwaan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zeelenberg, R" uniqKey="Zeelenberg R">R Zeelenberg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pecher, D" uniqKey="Pecher D">D Pecher</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zanolie, K" uniqKey="Zanolie K">K Zanolie</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zeelenberg, R" uniqKey="Zeelenberg R">R Zeelenberg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pecher, D" uniqKey="Pecher D">D Pecher</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zeelenberg, R" uniqKey="Zeelenberg R">R Zeelenberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, Lw" uniqKey="Barsalou L">LW Barsalou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pecher, D" uniqKey="Pecher D">D Pecher</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zeelenberg, R" uniqKey="Zeelenberg R">R Zeelenberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, Lw" uniqKey="Barsalou L">LW Barsalou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Plaut, Dc" uniqKey="Plaut D">DC Plaut</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rogers, Tt" uniqKey="Rogers T">TT Rogers</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lambon Ralph, Ma" uniqKey="Lambon Ralph M">MA Lambon Ralph</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Garrard, P" uniqKey="Garrard P">P Garrard</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bozeat, S" uniqKey="Bozeat S">S Bozeat</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcclelland, Jl" uniqKey="Mcclelland J">JL McClelland</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hodges, Jr" uniqKey="Hodges J">JR Hodges</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Simmons, Wk" uniqKey="Simmons W">WK Simmons</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ramjee, V" uniqKey="Ramjee V">V Ramjee</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Beauchamp, Ms" uniqKey="Beauchamp M">MS Beauchamp</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcrae, K" uniqKey="Mcrae K">K McRae</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Martin, A" uniqKey="Martin A">A Martin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, Lw" uniqKey="Barsalou L">LW Barsalou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Solomon, Ko" uniqKey="Solomon K">KO Solomon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, Lw" uniqKey="Barsalou L">LW Barsalou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Solomon, Ko" uniqKey="Solomon K">KO Solomon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, Lw" uniqKey="Barsalou L">LW Barsalou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Spence, C" uniqKey="Spence C">C Spence</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nicholls, Mer" uniqKey="Nicholls M">MER Nicholls</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Driver, J" uniqKey="Driver J">J Driver</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tulving, E" uniqKey="Tulving E">E Tulving</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thomson, Dm" uniqKey="Thomson D">DM Thomson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dantzig, S" uniqKey="Dantzig S">S Dantzig</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pecher, D" uniqKey="Pecher D">D Pecher</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zeelenberg, R" uniqKey="Zeelenberg R">R Zeelenberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, Lw" uniqKey="Barsalou L">LW Barsalou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vermeulen, N" uniqKey="Vermeulen N">N Vermeulen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Corneille, O" uniqKey="Corneille O">O Corneille</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Niedenthal, Pm" uniqKey="Niedenthal P">PM Niedenthal</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vigliocco, G" uniqKey="Vigliocco G">G Vigliocco</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vinson, Dp" uniqKey="Vinson D">DP Vinson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lewis, W" uniqKey="Lewis W">W Lewis</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Garrett, Mf" uniqKey="Garrett M">MF Garrett</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zeelenberg, R" uniqKey="Zeelenberg R">R Zeelenberg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zeelenberg, R" uniqKey="Zeelenberg R">R Zeelenberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pecher, D" uniqKey="Pecher D">D Pecher</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shiffrin, Rm" uniqKey="Shiffrin R">RM Shiffrin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Raaijmakers, Jgw" uniqKey="Raaijmakers J">JGW Raaijmakers</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
</listBibl>
</div1>
</back>
</TEI>
<pmc article-type="research-article">
<pmc-dir>properties open_access</pmc-dir>
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="nlm-ta">Behav Res Methods</journal-id>
<journal-title-group>
<journal-title>Behavior Research Methods</journal-title>
</journal-title-group>
<issn pub-type="ppub">1554-351X</issn>
<issn pub-type="epub">1554-3528</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Springer-Verlag</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmid">21287109</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmc">3048290</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="publisher-id">38</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3758/s13428-010-0038-8</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Article</subject>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>A sharp image or a sharp knife: norms for the modality-exclusivity of 774 concept-property items</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author" corresp="yes">
<name>
<surname>van Dantzig</surname>
<given-names>Saskia</given-names>
</name>
<address>
<phone>+31-71-5276667</phone>
<email>saskia@vandantzig.nl</email>
</address>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="Aff1">1</xref>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="Aff4">4</xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cowell</surname>
<given-names>Rosemary A.</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="Aff2">2</xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Zeelenberg</surname>
<given-names>René</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="Aff3">3</xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Pecher</surname>
<given-names>Diane</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="Aff3">3</xref>
</contrib>
<aff id="Aff1">
<label>1</label>
Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands</aff>
<aff id="Aff2">
<label>2</label>
University of California, San Diego, CA USA</aff>
<aff id="Aff3">
<label>3</label>
Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands</aff>
<aff id="Aff4">
<label>4</label>
Cognitive Psychology Unit, Institute of Psychology, Wassenaarseweg 52, Room 2B11, PO Box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands</aff>
</contrib-group>
<pub-date pub-type="epub">
<day>24</day>
<month>12</month>
<year>2010</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="pmc-release">
<day>24</day>
<month>12</month>
<year>2010</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="collection">
<month>3</month>
<year>2011</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>43</volume>
<issue>1</issue>
<fpage>145</fpage>
<lpage>154</lpage>
<permissions>
<copyright-statement>© The Author(s) 2010</copyright-statement>
</permissions>
<abstract id="Abs1">
<p>According to recent embodied cognition theories, mental concepts are represented by modality-specific sensory-motor systems. Much of the evidence for modality-specificity in conceptual processing comes from the property-verification task. When applying this and other tasks, it is important to select items based on their modality-exclusivity. We collected modality ratings for a set of 387 properties, each of which was paired with two different concepts, yielding a total of 774 concept-property items. For each item, participants rated the degree to which the property could be experienced through five perceptual modalities (vision, audition, touch, smell, and taste). Based on these ratings, we computed a measure of modality exclusivity, the degree to which a property is perceived exclusively through one sensory modality. In this paper, we briefly sketch the theoretical background of conceptual knowledge, discuss the use of the property-verification task in cognitive research, provide our norms and statistics, and validate the norms in a memory experiment. We conclude that our norms are important for researchers studying modality-specific effects in conceptual processing.</p>
<sec>
<title>Electronic supplementary material</title>
<p>The online version of this article (doi:10.3758/s13428-010-0038-8) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.</p>
</sec>
</abstract>
<kwd-group>
<title>Keywords</title>
<kwd>Conceptual knowledge</kwd>
<kwd>Grounded cognition</kwd>
<kwd>Property-verification task</kwd>
<kwd>Modality exclusivity</kwd>
<kwd>Flexibility of meaning</kwd>
</kwd-group>
<custom-meta-group>
<custom-meta>
<meta-name>issue-copyright-statement</meta-name>
<meta-value>© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2011</meta-value>
</custom-meta>
</custom-meta-group>
</article-meta>
</front>
<body>
<sec id="Sec1">
<title>Introduction</title>
<p>People understand and interact with their environment by using conceptual knowledge about objects and their properties. According to various models of conceptual representation (Caramazza & Shelton,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR8">1998</xref>
; Farah & McClelland,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR10">1991</xref>
; Humphreys & Forde,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR14">2001</xref>
), concepts can be described as distributed patterns of activation across units representing semantic features. Several of these models propose that at least a subset of the features are modality-specific (e.g., Cree & McRae,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR9">2003</xref>
; Plaut,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR25">2002</xref>
; Rogers et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR26">2004</xref>
; Vigliocco, Vinson, Lewis, & Garrett,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR34">2004</xref>
). This is consistent with the
<italic>embodied cognition</italic>
view on mental representation (e.g., Barsalou,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR4">1999</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR5">2008a</xref>
; Grush,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR12">2004</xref>
), which proposes that concepts are grounded in sensory-motor processing. According to this view, object concepts are learned through recurrent experiences of perceiving and interacting with a particular object. During those experiences, the patterns of activity in sensory-motor brain areas are captured and stored in memory to form elaborated, multimodal knowledge structures, called simulators. Representation is achieved by reactivating a subset of this stored knowledge to construct a specific simulation. The exact content of a particular simulation depends on the individual’s experience with the simulated concept, as well as on situational factors such as current goals and task demands. As a result, each simulation is unique.</p>
<p>A method often used to assess conceptual knowledge about objects and their properties is the property-verification task. In this task, the word for a concept (e.g.,
<italic>banana</italic>
) is presented together with the word for a property that might be true (e.g.,
<italic>yellow</italic>
) or false (e.g.,
<italic>blue</italic>
) for the concept. Participants have to judge whether the property is true or false as quickly as possible. According to the simulation theory, the response time reflects the effort required to form an adequate simulation of the concept and property. Since simulation is analogous to sensory-motor processing, this theory predicts that the response time is affected by factors that also influence perception and action. For example, Solomon and Barsalou (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR29">2004</xref>
) found that property-verification response times depend on perceptual factors such as the size and perceptual salience of a property.</p>
<p>Given that representations are flexible and task-dependent (e.g., Barclay, Bransford, Franks, McCarrel, & Nitsch,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR2">1974</xref>
; Barsalou,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR3">1993</xref>
), a simulation may be dominated by one sensory modality, with other modalities engaged to a lesser extent. For example, when performing a property-verification task, one may form different simulations of
<italic>SAXOPHONE</italic>
,
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="Fn1">1</xref>
depending on the property to be verified. In order to verify that a
<italic>SAXOPHONE</italic>
is
<italic>shiny,</italic>
a predominantly visual simulation is created, whereas to verify that a
<italic>SAXOPHONE</italic>
sounds
<italic>loud,</italic>
a predominantly auditory simulation is created. Evidence for such flexible and task-dependent recruitment of modality-specific features in representations is provided by the
<italic>modality-switch effect</italic>
. If two representations are simulated by different modality-specific systems, then constructing them consecutively will involve a switch of attention from one modality to another. This will result in a longer reaction time and/or a lower accuracy, similar to modality-switching costs in perceptual tasks (Spence, Nicholls, & Driver,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR30">2001</xref>
). Conceptual modality-switch effects have been found in various property-verification studies (e.g., Marques,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR18">2006</xref>
; Pecher, Zeelenberg, & Barsalou,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR23">2003</xref>
; Pecher, Van Dantzig, & Schifferstein,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR20">2009</xref>
; Vermeulen, Corneille & Niedenthal,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR33">2008</xref>
). Other studies demonstrated direct interactions between conceptual processing and perceptual processing, providing even stronger evidence for the theory that the sensory-motor system is involved in conceptual representation (Van Dantzig, Pecher, Zeelenberg, & Barsalou,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR32">2008</xref>
; Vermeulen et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR33">2008</xref>
). Brain imaging studies have provided corroborative evidence. Goldberg, Perfetti, and Schneider (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR11">2006</xref>
) applied functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to scan participants during a property-verification task that involved properties from four different sensory modalities (vision, sound, touch, and taste). When participants verified properties from these modalities, activity was recorded in corresponding sensory areas. Thus, verification of visual properties caused activation of visual brain areas, whereas verification of auditory properties caused activation of auditory brain areas. Similar results were found by Simmons et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR27">2007</xref>
) and Kan, Barsalou, Solomon, Minor, and Thompson-Schill (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR15">2003</xref>
). In addition, Cree and McRae (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR9">2003</xref>
), see also Plaut (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR25">2002</xref>
) showed that damage to modality-specific sensory areas may lead to semantic deficits. In sum, multiple studies provide evidence that conceptual representation takes place in the modality-specific brain areas that are also used during actual experience of instances of the represented concepts.</p>
<p>As has become clear, the property-verification task is well suited to the assessment of conceptual knowledge. When applying the task to study modality-specific processing, one should realize that property-verification items vary widely in their degree of modality-exclusivity. Whereas some properties are perceived exclusively by one specific sensory modality (e.g.,
<italic>PIANO-black</italic>
is purely visual), others may be perceived by multiple modalities (e.g.,
<italic>SHOWER-splashing</italic>
may be perceived by vision, audition, and touch). It is therefore important to carefully select items based on their modality-exclusivity. Lynott and Connell (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR17">2009</xref>
) found that controlling property-verification items for modality-exclusivity results in a markedly larger modality-switch effect (in terms of effect size). During pilot studies in our own lab, we also found that the size of the modality-switch effect depended on the modality-exclusivity of the items used (e.g., Van Dantzig et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR32">2008</xref>
).</p>
<p>In the present study, in order to select concept-property items on modality-exclusivity, we obtained modality ratings for a large set of items. For each item, participants rated the degree to which the property could be perceived through five different senses (vision, audition, touch, smell, and taste). The modality ratings form a significant extension of those collected by Lynott and Connell (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR17">2009</xref>
). In particular, while they obtained ratings for
<italic>isolated</italic>
properties (e.g.,
<italic>yellow</italic>
), we obtained ratings for concept-property
<italic>combinations</italic>
(e.g.,
<italic>BANANA-yellow</italic>
). It is important to realize that a property is never experienced in isolation, but is always connected to a particular object. In representation, independent simulators may exist for properties (Barsalou,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR6">2008b</xref>
). However, since simulation is a dynamic process, property-verification does not involve the activation of invariant representations of concepts and properties (Barsalou,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR7">2009</xref>
). Instead, the representations of the concept and property mutually constrain each other’s content. Thus, the concept
<italic>WINE</italic>
is represented differently when one has to verify the property
<italic>white</italic>
than when one has to verify the property
<italic>sweet</italic>
(Pecher, Zanolie, & Zeelenberg,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR22">2007</xref>
; Pecher, Zeelenberg, & Barsalou,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR24">2004</xref>
). Conversely, the property
<italic>white</italic>
is represented differently for the concept
<italic>WINE</italic>
than for the concept
<italic>SNOW</italic>
(Halff, Ortony, & Anderson,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR13">1976</xref>
; Solomon & Barsalou,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR28">2001</xref>
). In an experiment by Murphy and Andrew (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR19">1993</xref>
) participants provided synonyms or antonyms for adjectives that were presented in conjunction with a concept, e.g.,
<italic>fresh WATER, fresh AIR</italic>
or
<italic>fresh BREAD.</italic>
The concept strongly influenced which synonyms and antonyms were provided for a particular adjective. The degree to which a specific sensory modality is involved in perceiving a particular property may also differ across concepts. For example, the property
<italic>hot</italic>
for the concept
<italic>LIGHTBULB</italic>
is perceived almost exclusively by touch, as is well known by anyone who has ever burnt their fingers unscrewing a lamp. Fortunately, in other objects the property
<italic>hot</italic>
may also be seen (as in a red-hot
<italic>GRILL</italic>
or a steaming mug of
<italic>COFFEE</italic>
) or heard (as in a sizzling-hot
<italic>PAN</italic>
), so that we do not always have to find out the hard way. Strictly speaking, in these situations it is not the actual property
<italic>hot</italic>
that is perceived visually or auditorily, but another visual (
<italic>steaming, red</italic>
) or auditory (
<italic>sizzling</italic>
) property that merely indicates the hotness of the object. However, according to the simulation theory, property-verification involves simulating a (partial and schematic) sensory-motor experience, by reactivating patterns laid down during earlier experiences. As a result, properties (
<italic>steaming, sizzling</italic>
) that frequently co-occurred with the target property (
<italic>hot</italic>
) during those experiences may also be included in the simulation and may be used to indicate the presence of the target property. Thus, whereas the property
<italic>hot</italic>
in itself is exclusively tactile, in a property-verification task it may be associated with other-modality properties, just as in real experience. The degree to which a particular property is perceived by one or (indirectly) by multiple sensory modalities may vary across concepts.</p>
<p>To capture this variability in property meaning, we paired each property with two concepts, resulting in two different concept-property items. For example, the property
<italic>plain</italic>
was paired with the concept
<italic>FOOD</italic>
and with the concept
<italic>FABRIC.</italic>
For each concept-property item, participants rated the degree to which the property is experienced through five different sensory modalities (vision, audition, touch, smell, and taste). This resulted in a specific
<italic>modality profile</italic>
for each item, a five-dimensional vector representing the mean strength ratings on all five modalities. The modality with the highest rating was defined as the dominant modality for that item. Item pairs could be
<italic>unidominant,</italic>
with both items having the same dominant modality (e.g.,
<italic>pink FLAMINGO</italic>
and
<italic>pink COTTON CANDY</italic>
are both predominantly visual) or
<italic>bidominant,</italic>
with the two items having different dominant modalities (e.g.,
<italic>plain FABRIC</italic>
and
<italic>plain FOOD</italic>
are predominantly visual and gustatory, respectively). Additionally, we computed the angle between the two vectors of each item pair, as well as the cosine of this angle. These measures indicate how similar the modality profiles of the two items of a pair are. Finally, we computed modality-exclusivity scores for each concept-property item, indicating to what extent properties are perceived exclusively by one modality. When comparing our data with those of Lynott and Connell (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR17">2009</xref>
), some interesting patterns arose. In the remainder of this paper, we describe how the norms were collected and we provide the obtained ratings and statistics. Furthermore, the norms were validated in a memory experiment. During the study phase of this experiment, property words were presented together with one of their two normed concepts (e.g., “plain food”). Subsequently, memory for the property words was tested using a cued recognition test in which the property was presented either with the same concept word as in the study phase (“plain food”) or with the other concept of the item pair (“plain fabric”). When the concept was repeated, recognition of the property word was better than when the concept was switched. More interestingly, this effect was influenced by the similarity of the modality profiles of an item pair; the switch cost was larger for items with very different modality profiles (e.g.,
<italic>plain fabric – plain food</italic>
) than for items with highly similar modality profiles (e.g.,
<italic>grey elephant – grey pebble</italic>
). This result indicates that our norms reflect actual differences in the representation of properties and concepts.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec2">
<title>Norming study</title>
<p>
<bold>Stimuli</bold>
We selected 387 properties that could be experienced through one or more sensory modalities. Of these 387 items, 364 properties were obtained from the set of Lynott and Connell (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR17">2009</xref>
) and the remaining 23 properties had been used in earlier studies in our lab. Each of the properties was paired with two different concepts, yielding a total number of 774 concept-property items. The set of items was divided into ten different lists that consisted of 77 items (six lists) or 78 items (four lists) each. Properties and concepts were never repeated within a list. Participants received only one list, such that they never rated the same property or concept twice. The complete set of items is available in
<xref rid="MOESM1" ref-type="">Electronic supplementary material</xref>
.</p>
<p>
<bold>Participants</bold>
Four hundred thirty-one undergraduate students from the University of California, San Diego participated in the experiment in return for course credits. Given that there were 10 different lists of items, each item was rated by at least 40 participants.</p>
<p>
<bold>Procedure</bold>
The procedure was similar to that used by Lynott and Connell (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR17">2009</xref>
). The E-Prime 2.0 software package (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA) was used to present the items and collect the participants’ responses. Concept-property items were presented in random order. During each trial, a screen was presented with the text “To what extent do you experience [concept] being [property]?” (e.g., “To what extent do you experience a banana being yellow?”). This text was followed by five separate rating scales for each perceptual modality, labeled “by feeling through touch”, “by hearing”, “by seeing”, “by smelling”, and “by tasting”. To avoid any order effects, the order of the rating scales was randomly determined for each participant. Participants rated the extent to which they would experience the property through each of the five senses from 0 (not at all) to 5 (greatly), by clicking on the corresponding number with the computer mouse. Once the property had been rated on all five modalities, the participant clicked on the “Next Item” button to proceed to the subsequent item. If participants were unfamiliar with a concept or property, they were instructed to skip the item by pressing the “Next Item” button. The instruction emphasized that there were no right or wrong answers and that participants could give a high rating for as many or as few of the modalities as they wished, and a low rating for as many or as few of the modalities as they wished. Participants were encouraged to use their first intuitions about the items, and not to think too long about their answers. The experiment was self-paced and lasted approximately 20–30 min. The complete instructions are provided in
<xref rid="Sec8" ref-type="sec">Appendix</xref>
.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec3">
<title>Results and discussion</title>
<p>The data from fourteen participants were excluded from the analysis because they were non-native English speakers. One other participant was excluded because he/she failed to comply with the instructions, leaving a total of 416 participants.</p>
<p>The ratings provided by the participants were used to compute mean ratings for each concept-property item. The complete set of items and their ratings can be found in
<xref rid="MOESM1" ref-type="">Electronic supplementary material</xref>
.</p>
<p>We first analyzed each item separately. For each item, the modality with the highest mean rating was defined as the dominant modality for that item. Table 
<xref rid="Tab1" ref-type="table">1</xref>
shows the distribution of items and ratings for each dominant modality. Table 
<xref rid="Tab2" ref-type="table">2</xref>
shows the correlations between the ratings on each of the five modalities.
<table-wrap id="Tab1">
<label>Table 1</label>
<caption>
<p>Number of properties per dominant modality, with modality exclusivity scores and mean strength ratings (0–5) on the five perceptual modalities</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th rowspan="2">Dominant modality</th>
<th rowspan="2">
<italic>n</italic>
</th>
<th rowspan="2">Modality Exclusivity</th>
<th colspan="5">Strength</th>
</tr>
<tr>
<th>Visual</th>
<th>Auditory</th>
<th>Haptic</th>
<th>Gustatory</th>
<th>Olfactory</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td>Visual</td>
<td>351</td>
<td>55%</td>
<td>4.6</td>
<td>0.8</td>
<td>2.2</td>
<td>0.5</td>
<td>0.6</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Auditory</td>
<td>150</td>
<td>55%</td>
<td>2.7</td>
<td>4.7</td>
<td>1.1</td>
<td>0.1</td>
<td>0.3</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Haptic</td>
<td>126</td>
<td>44%</td>
<td>3.3</td>
<td>0.8</td>
<td>4.4</td>
<td>0.6</td>
<td>0.6</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Gustatory</td>
<td>91</td>
<td>37%</td>
<td>2.5</td>
<td>0.3</td>
<td>1.7</td>
<td>4.6</td>
<td>2.7</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Olfactory</td>
<td>56</td>
<td>46%</td>
<td>2.0</td>
<td>0.5</td>
<td>1.0</td>
<td>1.7</td>
<td>4.4</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Total</td>
<td>774</td>
<td>51%</td>
<td>1.5</td>
<td>1.0</td>
<td>2.2</td>
<td>1.1</td>
<td>3.6</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</table-wrap>
<table-wrap id="Tab2">
<label>Table 2</label>
<caption>
<p>Correlation matrix for mean strength ratings per item on the five perceptual modalities</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th rowspan="2">Modality</th>
<th colspan="5">Modality</th>
</tr>
<tr>
<th>Auditory</th>
<th>Gustatory</th>
<th>Haptic</th>
<th>Olfactory</th>
<th>Visual</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td>Auditory</td>
<td char="." align="char">1</td>
<td></td>
<td></td>
<td></td>
<td></td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Gustatory</td>
<td char="." align="char">–0.318**</td>
<td char="." align="char">1</td>
<td></td>
<td></td>
<td></td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Haptic</td>
<td char="." align="char">–0.267**</td>
<td char="." align="char">–0.050</td>
<td char="." align="char">1</td>
<td></td>
<td></td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Olfactory</td>
<td char="." align="char">–0.311**</td>
<td char="." align="char">0.615**</td>
<td char="." align="char">–0.177**</td>
<td char="." align="char">1</td>
<td></td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Visual</td>
<td char="." align="char">–0.238**</td>
<td char="." align="char">–0.294**</td>
<td char="." align="char">0.245**</td>
<td char="." align="char">–0.361**</td>
<td>1</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
<table-wrap-foot>
<p>
<italic>Note</italic>
. **
<italic>p</italic>
< .001 (two-tailed)</p>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
</p>
<p>
<bold>Modality exclusivity</bold>
Modality exclusivity is the extent to which a property is perceived exclusively by one modality. Following Lynott and Connell (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR17">2009</xref>
), an item’s modality exclusivity was derived from the mean strength ratings on the five modality scales. Modality exclusivity was computed as the range of scores across modalities divided by the sum of the scores. The resulting modality exclusivity scores range from 0 to 1 (translated to 0% to 100%). A score of 0% is obtained when equal scores are given for each modality, indicating that the property is completely multimodal. A score of 100% is obtained when the property is completely experienced by one single modality. Modality-exclusivity for individual properties ranged from 11 to 98%, with a mean of 50.6% (SD  =  16.2%). Figure 
<xref rid="Fig1" ref-type="fig">1</xref>
displays the distribution of exclusivity scores for the items grouped by dominant modality. Exclusivity scores differed across dominant modalities,
<italic>F</italic>
(4, 769) = 34.60,
<italic>p</italic>
  <  .001. Posthoc tests with Bonferroni correction revealed that the exclusivity scores of the visual and auditory items (55%) were higher than the scores on the other items (all
<italic>p</italic>
values < .001), but did not differ from each other. The olfactory items (46%) and haptic items (44%) did not differ from each other significantly, but both were higher than the gustatory items (37%) (both
<italic>p</italic>
values < .05).
<fig id="Fig1">
<label>Fig. 1</label>
<caption>
<p>Distribution of exclusivity scores, grouped by dominant modality</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="13428_2010_38_Fig1_HTML" id="MO1"></graphic>
</fig>
Properties are never experienced in isolation, but are always connected to a particular object. These objects influence the way in which properties are perceived and represented. As a result, a property may receive different modality ratings depending on the concept that it is combined with. This variance can be investigated in our dataset, because each property occurs twice, in combination with two different concepts. The two concept-property items sharing the same property formed an item pair, which could be analyzed in several ways.Item pairs could be unidominant, with both items of the pair having the same dominant modality (e.g.,
<italic>pink FLAMINGO</italic>
and pink
<italic>COTTON CANDY</italic>
are both predominantly visual) or bidominant, with the two items having different modalities (e.g.,
<italic>plain FABRIC</italic>
and
<italic>plain FOOD</italic>
are predominantly visual and gustatory, respectively). Our set contained 255 unidominant pairs (66%) and 132 bidominant pairs (34%). Table 
<xref rid="Tab3" ref-type="table">3</xref>
shows how often each combination of dominant modalities occurs and provides examples of each modality combination.
<table-wrap id="Tab3">
<label>Table 3</label>
<caption>
<p>Frequencies and examples of unidominant and bidominant item pairs by modality combination</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th>Type</th>
<th>Modality combination</th>
<th>
<italic>n</italic>
</th>
<th>Example</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td rowspan="5">Unidominant</td>
<td>Auditory-Auditory</td>
<td char="." align="char">57</td>
<td>Howling WIND - howling WOLF</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Gustatory-Gustatory</td>
<td char="." align="char">16</td>
<td>Sour LEMON - sour VINEGAR</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Haptic-Haptic</td>
<td char="." align="char">34</td>
<td>Damp FOG - damp TOWEL</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Olfactory-Olfactory</td>
<td char="." align="char">15</td>
<td>Eggy BREATH - eggy HOT SPRING</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Visual-Visual</td>
<td char="." align="char">133</td>
<td>Red FIRETRUCK - red STRAWBERRY</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="9">Bidominant</td>
<td>Auditory-Gustatory</td>
<td char="." align="char">3</td>
<td>Honeyed VOICE - honeyed COOKIES</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Auditory-Haptic</td>
<td char="." align="char">8</td>
<td>Thumping DRYER - thumping HEADACHE</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Auditory-Visual</td>
<td char="." align="char">25</td>
<td>Boiling KETTLE - boiling SOUP</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Gustatory-Haptic</td>
<td char="." align="char">19</td>
<td>Bitter COFFEE - bitter COLD</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Gustatory-Olfactory</td>
<td char="." align="char">16</td>
<td>Coconutty PINA COLADA - coconutty SUN CREAM</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Gustatory-Visual</td>
<td char="." align="char">30</td>
<td>Dry BISCUIT - dry RIVERBED</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Haptic-Olfactory</td>
<td char="." align="char">1</td>
<td>Light FEATHER - light COLOGNE</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Haptic-Visual</td>
<td char="." align="char">30</td>
<td>Hairy COCONUT - hairy MAMMOTH</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Olfactory-Visual</td>
<td char="." align="char">9</td>
<td>Burning TOAST - burning CANDLE</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</table-wrap>
Interestingly, even in the unidominant item pairs, our data show that properties are experienced differently across concepts. For example, the property
<italic>round</italic>
received high visual ratings for the concepts
<italic>COIN</italic>
and
<italic>PLANET,</italic>
indicating that this property is experienced predominantly visually in both concepts
<italic>.</italic>
The property was also rated as strongly haptic for the concept
<italic>COIN</italic>
, but not for the concept
<italic>PLANET</italic>
(see Fig. 
<xref rid="Fig2" ref-type="fig">2</xref>
). After all, people can (and often do) feel that a coin is round, but they never feel the round shape of a planet. Such subtle differences in the way in which properties are experienced are reflected in our ratings.
<fig id="Fig2">
<label>Fig. 2</label>
<caption>
<p>Modality strengths of the property
<italic>round</italic>
for the concepts
<italic>COIN</italic>
and
<italic>PLANET</italic>
</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="13428_2010_38_Fig2_HTML" id="MO2"></graphic>
</fig>
</p>
<p>
<bold>Similarity of item pairs</bold>
Each concept-property item has a specific
<italic>modality profile,</italic>
a five-dimensional vector representing the mean strength ratings on the five modalities. In order to determine how similar the two modality profiles of a particular item pair are, we computed the angle between their two vectors. If two items have highly similar modality profiles (e.g.,
<italic>spiky HEDGEHOG – spiky SEA ANEMONE</italic>
), the angle between the vectors will be small. If the profiles are very different (e.g.,
<italic>honeyed COOKIES – honeyed VOICE</italic>
), the angle between the vectors will be large. Overall, unidominant item pairs had smaller angles (
<italic>M</italic>
  =  15.8 degrees, SD  =  10.6) than bidominant pairs (
<italic>M</italic>
  =  40.6 degrees, SD  =  21.0),
<italic>t</italic>
(385)  =  15.47,
<italic>p </italic>
 <  .001. Note, however, that the two measures do not map completely. The two items of a unidominant pair, while having the same dominant modality, could have very different scores on the remaining modalities, rendering them very dissimilar. Conversely, the two items of a bidominant pair could have very similar modality profiles, while their dominant modalities differ by a very small margin. Because the similarity measure is based on the full modality profile, it is more sensitive to subtle differences between the items of a pair than when one only takes into account the dominant modalities. The angle and the cosine of the angle of each item pair are reported in the data file (see
<xref rid="MOESM1" ref-type="">Electronic supplementary material</xref>
).The majority of properties used in our dataset (364 out of 387 properties, 94%) were also normed by Lynott and Connell (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR17">2009</xref>
). By comparing our modality exclusivity scores for this subset with the scores of Lynott and Connell (furthermore referred to as L&C exclusivity scores), some interesting patterns become apparent. First, our modality exclusivity scores are slightly higher (
<italic>M</italic>
  =  0.51, SD  =  0.14) than the L&C exclusivity scores (
<italic>M</italic>
  =  0.47, SD  =  0.17),
<italic>t</italic>
(363) = 7.50,
<italic>p</italic>
  <  .001. This difference might be because properties were paired with specific concepts in the current study, whereas they were presented in isolation in the study of Lynott and Connell. The latter may have caused participants to represent different concepts for the same property. For example, when asked to rate the property
<italic>rotten</italic>
, one participant might form a representation of an apple with brown spots and a mushy texture that indicate its rottenness. Thus, this participant will rate
<italic>rotten</italic>
as predominantly visual and haptic. Another participant might instead form a representation of a rotten egg, distinguished by its sulfuric smell. Thus, this participant will rate
<italic>rotten</italic>
as predominantly olfactory. It is also possible that a participant may represent multiple concepts for a particular property. This participant may choose to provide modality ratings that average across the represented concepts. The variance in represented concepts, either between participants or within the same participant, will result in lower modality exclusivity scores. Alternatively, participants may form a generic, multimodal representation of the property
<italic>rotten,</italic>
based on earlier experience but abstracted from the specific objects with which it was experienced. Such generic representations will result in lower modality exclusivity scores than the more specific representation that is formed when the property is presented together with a particular object.In any case, we would expect the difference between our exclusivity scores and the L&C scores to be more pronounced in the bidominant item pairs than in the unidominant item pairs. This was tested by splitting the data into unidominant and bidominant items and performing the comparison for both groups separately. In the unidominant items, our exclusivity scores (
<italic>M </italic>
 =  .53, SD  =  .14) are similar to the L&C exclusivity scores (
<italic>M</italic>
  =  .52, SD  =  .16),
<italic>t</italic>
(236) = 1.71,
<italic>p</italic>
  >  .05. In the bidominant items, on the other hand, our exclusivity scores are significantly higher (
<italic>M </italic>
 =  .47, SD  =  .12) than the L&C scores (
<italic>M</italic>
  =  .35, SD  =  .10),
<italic>t</italic>
(126) = 9.85,
<italic>p</italic>
  <  .001. Figure 
<xref rid="Fig3" ref-type="fig">3</xref>
plots the proportion of unidominant and bidominant item pairs as a function of L&C exclusivity. The proportion of unidominant item pairs increases as exclusivity scores become higher. This pattern indicates that in the study of Lynott and Connell, participants might have represented different concepts for the same property, which increased the variance and lowered the exclusivity score especially for those items in which the different concepts lead to very dissimilar response vectors.
<fig id="Fig3">
<label>Fig. 3</label>
<caption>
<p>Proportion of unidominant and bidominant item pairs as a function of L&C exclusivity score</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="13428_2010_38_Fig3_HTML" id="MO3"></graphic>
</fig>
</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec4">
<title>Memory experiment</title>
<p>Our norms show that when properties are paired with different concepts, the resulting modality profiles can vary widely in similarity. Next, we wanted to investigate whether the similarity in modality profiles in our norms reflects similarity in the mental representations of the properties. For example, the norms indicate that the representation of
<italic>mild</italic>
might be very different in the context of
<italic>CURRY</italic>
than in the context of
<italic>WINTER</italic>
, whereas the representation of
<italic>grey</italic>
might not differ so much between
<italic>ELEPHANT</italic>
and
<italic>PEBBLE</italic>
. Previous studies showed that both implicit (Zeelenberg, Pecher, Shiffrin, & Raaijmakers,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR36">2003</xref>
) and explicit (Barclay et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR2">1974</xref>
; Tulving & Thomson,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR31">1973</xref>
; Zeelenberg,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR35">2005</xref>
) memory for concept nouns are sensitive to the overlap in contextual cues at study and test. Furthermore, such contextual effects might be due to differences in modality specific features of the representations at study and test (Pecher et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR24">2004</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR22">2007</xref>
; Pecher, Van Dantzig, Zwaan, & Zeelenberg,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR21">2009</xref>
). In the present study, we investigated how memory of property words was influenced by context (i.e., a concept word). During the study phase, property words were presented together with one of their two concepts (e.g., “grey elephant”). Subsequently, memory for the property words was tested using a cued recognition test, in which the property was presented together with either the same concept word as in the study phase (“grey elephant”), or with the other concept of the item pair (“grey pebble”). We selected item pairs that had either highly similar modality profiles (e.g.,
<italic>grey ELEPHANT – grey PEBBLE</italic>
) or very different modality profiles (e.g.,
<italic>mild CURRY – mild WINTER</italic>
). We expected a main effect of concept switch; recognition of the property would be worse if the concept was switched than if the concept was repeated. Moreover, we expected the switch effect to be influenced by the similarity of the modality profiles; it should be larger for items with different modality profiles than for items with similar modality profiles. After all, for items with different modality profiles switching the concept would result in very different representations of the property during study and test, whereas this difference would be much less pronounced for items with similar modality profiles.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec5" sec-type="methods">
<title>Method</title>
<p>
<bold>Participants</bold>
Twenty-five undergraduate students from the University of California, San Diego, participated in the experiment in return for course credits. None had participated in the norming study. One participant did not perform the memory task as instructed and was removed from the analysis.</p>
<p>
<bold>Stimuli</bold>
Two sets of 40 properties each were selected. Each property was paired with the two concepts that had been presented in the norming study. The
<italic>similar modality profile</italic>
properties had very high cosines (small angles) between the modality vectors for the two concepts, indicating that they had very similar ratings on the sensory modalities. The
<italic>different modality profile</italic>
properties had very low cosines (large angles) between the modality vectors for the two concepts, indicating that they had very different ratings on the sensory modalities. The stimuli in the two sets were matched on length of the property word, word frequency of the property and both concept words, and the Latent Semantic Analysis (LSA) cosine between the two concept words (Landauer & Dumais,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR16">1997</xref>
). Word frequency norms were derived from the Celex database (Baayen, Piepenbrock, & van Rijn,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR1">1993</xref>
). The characteristics of the two sets of stimuli are provided in Table 
<xref rid="Tab4" ref-type="table">4</xref>
.
<table-wrap id="Tab4">
<label>Table 4</label>
<caption>
<p>Characteristics of the items of the similar modality profile items and different modality profile items (standard deviations in
<italic>parentheses</italic>
) used in the memory experiment</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th>Characteristic</th>
<th>Similar modality profile items</th>
<th>Different modality profile items</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td>Property word length</td>
<td>6.7 (1.9)</td>
<td>6.0 (1.8)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Log frequency of property word</td>
<td>0.61 (0.58)</td>
<td>0.79 (0.77)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Log frequency of concept words</td>
<td>1.06 (0.72)</td>
<td>1.24 (0.74)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Cosine of modality profiles</td>
<td>1.00 (0.0)</td>
<td>0.53 (0.2)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Angle of modality profiles (in degrees)</td>
<td>4.9 (2.0)</td>
<td>57.0 (13.6)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>LSA cosine (concept 1 to concept 2)</td>
<td>0.05 (0.11)</td>
<td>0.03 (0.12)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>LSA cosine (property-concept item 1 to property-concept item 2)</td>
<td>0.32 (0.29)</td>
<td>0.31 (0.29)</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</table-wrap>
We created four lists to counterbalance the items. Each participant saw only one list; six participants were assigned to each list. Across these lists, each property was used twice in the repeated concept condition and twice in the switch concept condition, and for each of the two conditions a concept was used once as study concept and once as test concept. Examples of the conditions and counterbalancing are provided in Table 
<xref rid="Tab5" ref-type="table">5</xref>
. The resulting lists contained 20 properties in each of the four conditions: similar profile-repeat concept, similar profile-switch concept, different profile-repeat concept, and different profile-switch concept. For the memory test, an additional set of 80 different property-concept pairs was created to serve as new items (distractors).
<table-wrap id="Tab5">
<label>Table 5</label>
<caption>
<p>Examples of the design and counterbalancing in the memory experiment</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th>Condition</th>
<th>Property</th>
<th>Study concept</th>
<th>Test concept</th>
<th>Version</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td rowspan="2">Different modality profile – repeat concept</td>
<td rowspan="2">Mild</td>
<td>Curry</td>
<td>Curry</td>
<td>1</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Winter</td>
<td>Winter</td>
<td>2</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="2">Different modality profile – switch concept</td>
<td rowspan="2">Mild</td>
<td>Winter</td>
<td>Curry</td>
<td>3</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Curry</td>
<td>Winter</td>
<td>4</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="2">Similar modality profile – repeat concept</td>
<td rowspan="2">Grey</td>
<td>Elephant</td>
<td>Elephant</td>
<td>1</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Pebble</td>
<td>Pebble</td>
<td>2</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="2">Similar modality profile – switch concept</td>
<td rowspan="2">Grey</td>
<td>Elephant</td>
<td>Pebble</td>
<td>3</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Pebble</td>
<td>Elephant</td>
<td>4</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</table-wrap>
</p>
<p>
<bold>Procedure</bold>
Participants were instructed to study the properties (presented in uppercase) for a later memory test. They were told that they should also read the concepts (presented in lowercase) because they might be provided at test to help them remember the properties. Each property-concept pair was presented for 3,500 ms in the center of the computer screen. The property was always the left hand item of the pair and the concept the right hand item (e.g., MILD curry). Between items, a 500-ms blank screen was presented. The 80 items were presented in a different random order for each participant. The test phase started immediately after the study phase. Participants were instructed that they would see property-concept pairs, and their task was to indicate whether they had studied the property in the previous phase. They were told that the concept might help their memory, but that they should indicate “old” to all studied properties, whether the concept was studied or not. An item was presented at the center of the screen and remained there until the participant indicated “old” by pressing the m key or “new” by pressing the z key. All studied items and distractors were presented in different random order for each participant.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec6">
<title>Results</title>
<p>The average hit rates per condition are shown in Table 
<xref rid="Tab6" ref-type="table">6</xref>
. We analyzed the individual hit rates in a 2 (modality profile) by 2 (concept switch) ANOVA. The results showed a main effect of switch,
<italic>F</italic>
(1, 23) = 60.61,
<italic>p </italic>
 <  .001,
<italic>η</italic>
<sup>
<italic>2</italic>
</sup>
  =  .73, indicating that memory for the properties was better when the concept was repeated than when it was switched. There was no main effect of modality profile,
<italic>F</italic>
  <  1, indicating that memory for the two sets of properties was not different. Most interesting, there was a significant interaction between modality profile and concept repetition,
<italic>F</italic>
(1, 23) = 5.01,
<italic>p </italic>
 <  .05,
<italic>η</italic>
<sup>
<italic>2 </italic>
</sup>
 =  .18. As we predicted, the memory decrease due to concept switch was larger for different modality profile properties than for similar modality profile properties.
<table-wrap id="Tab6">
<label>Table 6</label>
<caption>
<p>Hit rates (standard errors in parentheses) in the memory experiment</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th>Condition</th>
<th>Similar modality profile</th>
<th>Different modality profile</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td>Repeat concept</td>
<td>.71 (.03)</td>
<td>.76 (.04)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Switch concept</td>
<td>.52 (.06)</td>
<td>.50 (.05)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Switch effect</td>
<td>.19 (.03)</td>
<td>.27 (.04)</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
<table-wrap-foot>
<p>
<italic>Note</italic>
. The false alarm rate was .18</p>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec7">
<title>Conclusions</title>
<p>We collected modality-exclusivity norms for 387 properties, each paired with two different concepts. The results show that modality profiles and modality exclusivity vary widely for a substantial proportion of properties. In a following experiment, we showed that such variability affects recognition memory for the properties. These results indicate that the norms reflect something essential about the representation of properties. When applying the property verification task or a different verbal task to study modality-specific effects, such as the modality-switch effect, it is important to select items carefully based on their modality-exclusivity. This may reduce the variability attributed to error, yielding cleaner data and larger effect sizes, as demonstrated by the study of Lynott and Connell (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR17">2009</xref>
). Because our norms incorporate the variance of properties across concepts, they are more specific than the norms collected by Lynott and Connell. In order to select items for an experiment, the two data sets can be used in conjunction. We recommend our norms for selecting items for a property-verification task or other verbal tasks using specific concept-property combinations (such as the memory task described in this paper). The norms of Lynott and Connell might be preferred for tasks that address more general meanings of properties.</p>
</sec>
<sec sec-type="supplementary-material">
<sec id="AppESM1">
<supplementary-material id="d28e1686" content-type="local-data">
<p>Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material. </p>
<media xlink:href="13428_2010_38_MOESM1_ESM.xls" id="MOESM1" mimetype="application" mime-subtype="vnd.ms-excel">
<label>ESM 1</label>
<caption>
<p>(241 KB)</p>
</caption>
</media>
</supplementary-material>
</sec>
</sec>
</body>
<back>
<ack>
<title>Acknowledgements</title>
<p>We are grateful to Jennifer Duong, Ana Flores, and Dan Killoran for assistance with the data collection.</p>
<p>
<bold>Open Access</bold>
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.</p>
</ack>
<ref-list id="Bib1">
<title>References</title>
<ref id="CR1">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Baayen</surname>
<given-names>RH</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Piepenbrock</surname>
<given-names>Rv</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>van Rijn</surname>
<given-names>H</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>The CELEX lexical database [CDROM]</source>
<year>1993</year>
<publisher-loc>Philadelphia</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>University of Pennsylvania, Linguistic Data Consortium</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR2">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Barclay</surname>
<given-names>JR</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bransford</surname>
<given-names>JD</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Franks</surname>
<given-names>JJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McCarrel</surname>
<given-names>NS</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nitsch</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Comprehension and semantic flexibility</article-title>
<source>Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior</source>
<year>1974</year>
<volume>13</volume>
<fpage>471</fpage>
<lpage>481</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0022-5371(74)80024-1</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR3">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>LW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Collins</surname>
<given-names>AC</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gathercole</surname>
<given-names>SE</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Conway</surname>
<given-names>MA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Morris</surname>
<given-names>PEM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Flexibility, structure, and linguistic vagary in concepts: Manifestations of a compositional system of perceptual symbols</article-title>
<source>Theories of memory</source>
<year>1993</year>
<publisher-loc>Hillsdale, NJ</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Erlbaum</publisher-name>
<fpage>29</fpage>
<lpage>101</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR4">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>LW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Perceptual symbol systems</article-title>
<source>The Behavioral and Brain Sciences</source>
<year>1999</year>
<volume>22</volume>
<fpage>577</fpage>
<lpage>660</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11301525</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR5">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>LW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Grounded cognition</article-title>
<source>Annual Review of Psychology</source>
<year>2008</year>
<volume>59</volume>
<fpage>617</fpage>
<lpage>645</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17705682</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR6">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>LW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Semin</surname>
<given-names>GR</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>ER</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Grounding symbolic operations in the brain’s modal systems</article-title>
<source>Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches</source>
<year>2008</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
<fpage>9</fpage>
<lpage>42</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR7">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>LW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction</article-title>
<source>Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences</source>
<year>2009</year>
<volume>364</volume>
<fpage>1281</fpage>
<lpage>1289</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1098/rstb.2008.0319</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19528009</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR8">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Caramazza</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Shelton</surname>
<given-names>JR</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Domain-specific knowledge systems in the brain: The animate-inanimate distinction</article-title>
<source>Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience</source>
<year>1998</year>
<volume>10</volume>
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>34</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1162/089892998563752</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">9526080</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR9">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cree</surname>
<given-names>GS</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McRae</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Analyzing the factors underlying the structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunk, cherry, chisel, cheese, and cello (and many other such concrete nouns)</article-title>
<source>Journal of Experimental Psychology: General</source>
<year>2003</year>
<volume>132</volume>
<fpage>163</fpage>
<lpage>201</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.163</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12825636</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR10">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Farah</surname>
<given-names>MJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McClelland</surname>
<given-names>JL</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>A computational model of semantic memory impairment: Modality specificity and emergent category specificity</article-title>
<source>Journal of Experimental Psychology: General</source>
<year>1991</year>
<volume>120</volume>
<fpage>339</fpage>
<lpage>357</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0096-3445.120.4.339</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">1837294</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR11">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Goldberg</surname>
<given-names>RF</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Perfetti</surname>
<given-names>CA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schneider</surname>
<given-names>W</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Perceptual knowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions</article-title>
<source>The Journal of Neuroscience</source>
<year>2006</year>
<volume>26</volume>
<fpage>4917</fpage>
<lpage>4921</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5389-05.2006</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16672666</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR12">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Grush</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception</article-title>
<source>The Behavioral and Brain Sciences</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>27</volume>
<fpage>377</fpage>
<lpage>442</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15736871</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR13">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Halff</surname>
<given-names>HM</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ortony</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Anderson</surname>
<given-names>RC</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>A context-sensitive representation of word meanings</article-title>
<source>Memory & Cognition</source>
<year>1976</year>
<volume>4</volume>
<fpage>378</fpage>
<lpage>383</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3758/BF03213193</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR14">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Humphreys</surname>
<given-names>GW</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Forde</surname>
<given-names>EME</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Hierarchies, similarity, and interactivity in object recognition: “category-specific” neuropsychological deficits</article-title>
<source>The Behavioral and Brain Sciences</source>
<year>2001</year>
<volume>24</volume>
<fpage>453</fpage>
<lpage>509</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11682799</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR15">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kan</surname>
<given-names>IP</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>LW</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Solomon</surname>
<given-names>KO</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Minor</surname>
<given-names>JK</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thompson-Schill</surname>
<given-names>SL</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Role of mental imagery in a property verification task: fMRI evidence for perceptual representations of conceptual knowledge</article-title>
<source>Cognitive Neuropsychology</source>
<year>2003</year>
<volume>20</volume>
<fpage>525</fpage>
<lpage>540</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/02643290244000257</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20957583</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR16">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Landauer</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dumais</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>A solution to Plato’s problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction and representation of knowledge</article-title>
<source>Psychological Review</source>
<year>1997</year>
<volume>104</volume>
<fpage>211</fpage>
<lpage>240</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-295X.104.2.211</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR17">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lynott</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Connell</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Modality exclusivity norms for 423 object properties</article-title>
<source>Behavior Research Methods</source>
<year>2009</year>
<volume>41</volume>
<fpage>558</fpage>
<lpage>564</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3758/BRM.41.2.558</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19363198</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR18">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Marques</surname>
<given-names>JM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Specialization and semantic organization: Evidence for multiple-semantics linked to sensory modalities</article-title>
<source>Memory & Cognition</source>
<year>2006</year>
<volume>34</volume>
<fpage>60</fpage>
<lpage>67</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3758/BF03193386</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR19">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Murphy</surname>
<given-names>GL</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Andrew</surname>
<given-names>JM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The conceptual basis of antonymy and synonymy in adjectives</article-title>
<source>Journal of Memory and Language</source>
<year>1993</year>
<volume>32</volume>
<fpage>301</fpage>
<lpage>319</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1006/jmla.1993.1016</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR20">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Pecher</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dantzig</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schifferstein</surname>
<given-names>HNJ</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Concepts are not represented by imagery</article-title>
<source>Psychonomic Bulletin & Review</source>
<year>2009</year>
<volume>16</volume>
<fpage>914</fpage>
<lpage>919</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3758/PBR.16.5.914</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19815798</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR21">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Pecher</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dantzig</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zwaan</surname>
<given-names>RA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zeelenberg</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Language comprehenders retain implied shape and orientation of objects</article-title>
<source>The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology</source>
<year>2009</year>
<volume>62</volume>
<fpage>1108</fpage>
<lpage>1114</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/17470210802633255</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19142833</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR22">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Pecher</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zanolie</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zeelenberg</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Verifying visual properties in sentence verification facilitates picture recognition memory</article-title>
<source>Experimental Psychology</source>
<year>2007</year>
<volume>54</volume>
<fpage>173</fpage>
<lpage>179</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17725157</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR23">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Pecher</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zeelenberg</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>LW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Verifying different-modality properties for concepts produces switching costs</article-title>
<source>Psychological Science</source>
<year>2003</year>
<volume>14</volume>
<fpage>119</fpage>
<lpage>124</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01429</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12661672</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR24">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Pecher</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zeelenberg</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>LW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Sensorimotor simulations underlie conceptual representations: Modality-specific effects of prior activation</article-title>
<source>Psychonomic Bulletin & Review</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>11</volume>
<fpage>164</fpage>
<lpage>167</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3758/BF03206477</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15117003</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR25">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Plaut</surname>
<given-names>DC</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Graded modality specific specialisation in semantics: A computational account of optic aphasia</article-title>
<source>Cognitive Neuropsychology</source>
<year>2002</year>
<volume>19</volume>
<fpage>603</fpage>
<lpage>639</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/02643290244000112</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20957556</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR26">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rogers</surname>
<given-names>TT</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lambon Ralph</surname>
<given-names>MA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Garrard</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bozeat</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McClelland</surname>
<given-names>JL</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hodges</surname>
<given-names>JR</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
<article-title>Structure and deterioration of semantic memory: A neuropsychological and computational investigation</article-title>
<source>Psychological Review</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>111</volume>
<fpage>205</fpage>
<lpage>235</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.205</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14756594</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR27">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Simmons</surname>
<given-names>WK</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ramjee</surname>
<given-names>V</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Beauchamp</surname>
<given-names>MS</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McRae</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Martin</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>LW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>A common neural substrate for perceiving and knowing about color</article-title>
<source>Neuropsychologia</source>
<year>2007</year>
<volume>45</volume>
<fpage>2802</fpage>
<lpage>2810</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.002</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17575989</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR28">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Solomon</surname>
<given-names>KO</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>LW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Representing properties locally</article-title>
<source>Cognitive Psychology</source>
<year>2001</year>
<volume>43</volume>
<fpage>129</fpage>
<lpage>169</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1006/cogp.2001.0754</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11527433</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR29">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Solomon</surname>
<given-names>KO</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>LW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Perceptual simulation in property verification</article-title>
<source>Memory & Cognition</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>32</volume>
<fpage>244</fpage>
<lpage>259</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3758/BF03196856</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR30">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Spence</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nicholls</surname>
<given-names>MER</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Driver</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The cost of expecting events in the wrong sensory modality</article-title>
<source>Perception & Psychophysics</source>
<year>2001</year>
<volume>63</volume>
<fpage>330</fpage>
<lpage>336</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3758/BF03194473</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11281107</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR31">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tulving</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thomson</surname>
<given-names>DM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory</article-title>
<source>Psychological Review</source>
<year>1973</year>
<volume>80</volume>
<fpage>359</fpage>
<lpage>380</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/h0020071</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR32">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Dantzig</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pecher</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zeelenberg</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>LW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Perceptual processing affects conceptual processing</article-title>
<source>Cognitive Science</source>
<year>2008</year>
<volume>32</volume>
<fpage>579</fpage>
<lpage>590</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/03640210802035365</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21635347</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR33">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Vermeulen</surname>
<given-names>N</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Corneille</surname>
<given-names>O</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Niedenthal</surname>
<given-names>PM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Sensory load incurs conceptual processing costs</article-title>
<source>Cognition</source>
<year>2008</year>
<volume>109</volume>
<fpage>287</fpage>
<lpage>294</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.cognition.2008.09.004</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18996513</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR34">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Vigliocco</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Vinson</surname>
<given-names>DP</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lewis</surname>
<given-names>W</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Garrett</surname>
<given-names>MF</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Representing the meanings of object and action words: The featural and unitary semantic space hypothesis</article-title>
<source>Cognitive Psychology</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>48</volume>
<fpage>422</fpage>
<lpage>488</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.cogpsych.2003.09.001</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15099798</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR35">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Zeelenberg</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Encoding specificity manipulations do affect retrieval from memory</article-title>
<source>Acta Psychologica</source>
<year>2005</year>
<volume>119</volume>
<fpage>107</fpage>
<lpage>121</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.actpsy.2004.12.001</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15823245</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR36">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Zeelenberg</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pecher</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Shiffrin</surname>
<given-names>RM</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Raaijmakers</surname>
<given-names>JGW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Semantic context effects and priming in word association</article-title>
<source>Psychonomic Bulletin & Review</source>
<year>2003</year>
<volume>10</volume>
<fpage>653</fpage>
<lpage>660</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3758/BF03196528</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14620360</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
<app-group>
<app id="App1">
<sec id="Sec8">
<title>Appendix</title>
<sec id="Sec9">
<title>Instructions</title>
<p>Objects can have different properties. Properties differ in how they are experienced. Some properties are mainly experienced by seeing (e.g., a TUBA being SHINY), whereas others are mainly experienced by hearing (e.g., a TUBA being LOUD). Some properties are experienced by multiple senses.</p>
<p>In this experiment you will be asked to rate to what degree object-property pairs are experienced by seeing, hearing, feeling by touch, tasting and smelling. For each sense, you should give a rating, ranging from 0 (is not perceived by this sense at all) to 5 (is greatly perceived by this sense).</p>
<p>(example)</p>
<p>Select a number by a mouse click.</p>
<p>Click the ‘Next Item’ button to continue to the next item.</p>
<p>If you are unfamiliar with an object or property, you can skip the item by pressing the 'Next Item' button. Keep in mind that you may give a high rating for as many or as few of the senses as you wish, and a low rating for as many or as few of the senses as you wish.</p>
<p>If you have clicked a wrong number, you can correct this by clicking another number. However, we are interested in your first intuitions about the items, so don't think too long about your answers. After all, there are no right or wrong answers.</p>
<p>You can work at your own pace and take a short break now and then. The experiment will take 25–30 min. If you have any further questions, ask the experimenter. Otherwise, press the spacebar to start the experiment.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
</app>
</app-group>
<fn-group>
<fn id="Fn1">
<label>1</label>
<p>Throughout this article, we use uppercase italics to represent concepts, lowercase italics to represent properties, and quotes to represent linguistic forms (words, sentences).</p>
</fn>
</fn-group>
</back>
</pmc>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Ticri/CIDE/explor/HapticV1/Data/Pmc/Curation
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 000765 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Curation/biblio.hfd -nk 000765 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Ticri/CIDE
   |area=    HapticV1
   |flux=    Pmc
   |étape=   Curation
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     PMC:3048290
   |texte=   A sharp image or a sharp knife: norms for the modality-exclusivity of 774 concept-property items
}}

Pour générer des pages wiki

HfdIndexSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Curation/RBID.i   -Sk "pubmed:21287109" \
       | HfdSelect -Kh $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Curation/biblio.hfd   \
       | NlmPubMed2Wicri -a HapticV1 

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.23.
Data generation: Mon Jun 13 01:09:46 2016. Site generation: Wed Mar 6 09:54:07 2024