Serveur d'exploration sur les dispositifs haptiques

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

Neuronal Plasticity and Multisensory Integration in Filial Imprinting

Identifieur interne : 002401 ( Pmc/Curation ); précédent : 002400; suivant : 002402

Neuronal Plasticity and Multisensory Integration in Filial Imprinting

Auteurs : Stephen Michael Town ; Brian John Mccabe

Source :

RBID : PMC:3053393

Abstract

Many organisms sample their environment through multiple sensory systems and the integration of multisensory information enhances learning. However, the mechanisms underlying multisensory memory formation and their similarity to unisensory mechanisms remain unclear. Filial imprinting is one example in which experience is multisensory, and the mechanisms of unisensory neuronal plasticity are well established. We investigated the storage of audiovisual information through experience by comparing the activity of neurons in the intermediate and medial mesopallium of imprinted and naïve domestic chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus) in response to an audiovisual imprinting stimulus and novel object and their auditory and visual components. We find that imprinting enhanced the mean response magnitude of neurons to unisensory but not multisensory stimuli. Furthermore, imprinting enhanced responses to incongruent audiovisual stimuli comprised of mismatched auditory and visual components. Our results suggest that the effects of imprinting on the unisensory and multisensory responsiveness of IMM neurons differ and that IMM neurons may function to detect unexpected deviations from the audiovisual imprinting stimulus.


Url:
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017777
PubMed: 21423770
PubMed Central: 3053393

Links toward previous steps (curation, corpus...)


Links to Exploration step

PMC:3053393

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI>
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">Neuronal Plasticity and Multisensory Integration in Filial Imprinting</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Town, Stephen Michael" sort="Town, Stephen Michael" uniqKey="Town S" first="Stephen Michael" last="Town">Stephen Michael Town</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="aff1"></nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mccabe, Brian John" sort="Mccabe, Brian John" uniqKey="Mccabe B" first="Brian John" last="Mccabe">Brian John Mccabe</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="aff1"></nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">PMC</idno>
<idno type="pmid">21423770</idno>
<idno type="pmc">3053393</idno>
<idno type="url">http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3053393</idno>
<idno type="RBID">PMC:3053393</idno>
<idno type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0017777</idno>
<date when="2011">2011</date>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Corpus">002401</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Curation">002401</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title xml:lang="en" level="a" type="main">Neuronal Plasticity and Multisensory Integration in Filial Imprinting</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Town, Stephen Michael" sort="Town, Stephen Michael" uniqKey="Town S" first="Stephen Michael" last="Town">Stephen Michael Town</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="aff1"></nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mccabe, Brian John" sort="Mccabe, Brian John" uniqKey="Mccabe B" first="Brian John" last="Mccabe">Brian John Mccabe</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="aff1"></nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<series>
<title level="j">PLoS ONE</title>
<idno type="eISSN">1932-6203</idno>
<imprint>
<date when="2011">2011</date>
</imprint>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">
<p>Many organisms sample their environment through multiple sensory systems and the integration of multisensory information enhances learning. However, the mechanisms underlying multisensory memory formation and their similarity to unisensory mechanisms remain unclear. Filial imprinting is one example in which experience is multisensory, and the mechanisms of unisensory neuronal plasticity are well established. We investigated the storage of audiovisual information through experience by comparing the activity of neurons in the intermediate and medial mesopallium of imprinted and naïve domestic chicks (
<italic>Gallus gallus domesticus</italic>
) in response to an audiovisual imprinting stimulus and novel object and their auditory and visual components. We find that imprinting enhanced the mean response magnitude of neurons to unisensory but not multisensory stimuli. Furthermore, imprinting enhanced responses to incongruent audiovisual stimuli comprised of mismatched auditory and visual components. Our results suggest that the effects of imprinting on the unisensory and multisensory responsiveness of IMM neurons differ and that IMM neurons may function to detect unexpected deviations from the audiovisual imprinting stimulus.</p>
</div>
</front>
<back>
<div1 type="bibliography">
<listBibl>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ernst, Mo" uniqKey="Ernst M">MO Ernst</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bulthoff, Hh" uniqKey="Bulthoff H">HH Bulthoff</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ernst, Mo" uniqKey="Ernst M">MO Ernst</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Banks, Ms" uniqKey="Banks M">MS Banks</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Alais, D" uniqKey="Alais D">D Alais</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Burr, D" uniqKey="Burr D">D Burr</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Seitz, Ar" uniqKey="Seitz A">AR Seitz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kim, R" uniqKey="Kim R">R Kim</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shams, L" uniqKey="Shams L">L Shams</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kim, Rs" uniqKey="Kim R">RS Kim</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Seitz, Ar" uniqKey="Seitz A">AR Seitz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shams, L" uniqKey="Shams L">L Shams</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stein, Be" uniqKey="Stein B">BE Stein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meredith, Ma" uniqKey="Meredith M">MA Meredith</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ghazanfar, Aa" uniqKey="Ghazanfar A">AA Ghazanfar</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schroeder, Ce" uniqKey="Schroeder C">CE Schroeder</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shams, L" uniqKey="Shams L">L Shams</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Seitz, Ar" uniqKey="Seitz A">AR Seitz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hein, G" uniqKey="Hein G">G Hein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Doehrmann, O" uniqKey="Doehrmann O">O Doehrmann</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Muller, Ng" uniqKey="Muller N">NG Muller</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kaiser, J" uniqKey="Kaiser J">J Kaiser</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Muckli, L" uniqKey="Muckli L">L Muckli</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Naumer, Mj" uniqKey="Naumer M">MJ Naumer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Doehrmann, O" uniqKey="Doehrmann O">O Doehrmann</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Muller, Ng" uniqKey="Muller N">NG Muller</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Muckli, L" uniqKey="Muckli L">L Muckli</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kaiser, J" uniqKey="Kaiser J">J Kaiser</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tanabe, Hc" uniqKey="Tanabe H">HC Tanabe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Honda, M" uniqKey="Honda M">M Honda</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sadato, N" uniqKey="Sadato N">N Sadato</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Xiang, Jz" uniqKey="Xiang J">JZ Xiang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brown, Mw" uniqKey="Brown M">MW Brown</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Horn, G" uniqKey="Horn G">G Horn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nicol, Au" uniqKey="Nicol A">AU Nicol</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brown, Mw" uniqKey="Brown M">MW Brown</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nakamori, T" uniqKey="Nakamori T">T Nakamori</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sato, K" uniqKey="Sato K">K Sato</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Atoji, Y" uniqKey="Atoji Y">Y Atoji</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kanamatsu, T" uniqKey="Kanamatsu T">T Kanamatsu</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tanaka, K" uniqKey="Tanaka K">K Tanaka</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sluckin, W" uniqKey="Sluckin W">W Sluckin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mccabe, Bj" uniqKey="Mccabe B">BJ McCabe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Horn, G" uniqKey="Horn G">G Horn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bateson, Ppg" uniqKey="Bateson P">PPG Bateson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Horn, G" uniqKey="Horn G">G Horn</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brown, Mw" uniqKey="Brown M">MW Brown</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Horn, G" uniqKey="Horn G">G Horn</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nicol, Au" uniqKey="Nicol A">AU Nicol</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brown, Mw" uniqKey="Brown M">MW Brown</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Horn, G" uniqKey="Horn G">G Horn</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jackson, C" uniqKey="Jackson C">C Jackson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mccabe, Bj" uniqKey="Mccabe B">BJ McCabe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nicol, Au" uniqKey="Nicol A">AU Nicol</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grout, As" uniqKey="Grout A">AS Grout</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brown, Mw" uniqKey="Brown M">MW Brown</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gray, Cm" uniqKey="Gray C">CM Gray</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Maldonado, Pe" uniqKey="Maldonado P">PE Maldonado</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wilson, M" uniqKey="Wilson M">M Wilson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcnaughton, B" uniqKey="Mcnaughton B">B McNaughton</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meredith, Ma" uniqKey="Meredith M">MA Meredith</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stein, Be" uniqKey="Stein B">BE Stein</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zahar, Y" uniqKey="Zahar Y">Y Zahar</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Reches, A" uniqKey="Reches A">A Reches</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gutfreund, Y" uniqKey="Gutfreund Y">Y Gutfreund</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kayser, C" uniqKey="Kayser C">C Kayser</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Logothetis, Nk" uniqKey="Logothetis N">NK Logothetis</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Panzeri, S" uniqKey="Panzeri S">S Panzeri</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bradley, P" uniqKey="Bradley P">P Bradley</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Davies, Dc" uniqKey="Davies D">DC Davies</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Horn, G" uniqKey="Horn G">G Horn</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Taylor, A" uniqKey="Taylor A">A Taylor</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sluckin, W" uniqKey="Sluckin W">W Sluckin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hewitt, R" uniqKey="Hewitt R">R Hewitt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Guiton, P" uniqKey="Guiton P">P Guiton</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mccabe, Bj" uniqKey="Mccabe B">BJ McCabe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Horn, G" uniqKey="Horn G">G Horn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kendrick, Km" uniqKey="Kendrick K">KM Kendrick</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dicarlo, Jj" uniqKey="Dicarlo J">JJ DiCarlo</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lane, Jw" uniqKey="Lane J">JW Lane</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hsiao, Ss" uniqKey="Hsiao S">SS Hsiao</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnson, Ko" uniqKey="Johnson K">KO Johnson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Davies, Dc" uniqKey="Davies D">DC Davies</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Horn, G" uniqKey="Horn G">G Horn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mccabe, Bj" uniqKey="Mccabe B">BJ McCabe</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cipolla Neto, J" uniqKey="Cipolla Neto J">J Cipolla-Neto</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Horn, G" uniqKey="Horn G">G Horn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mccabe, Bj" uniqKey="Mccabe B">BJ McCabe</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Kampen, Hs" uniqKey="Van Kampen H">HS Van Kampen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bolhuis, Jj" uniqKey="Bolhuis J">JJ Bolhuis</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lewicki, Ms" uniqKey="Lewicki M">MS Lewicki</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stanford, Tr" uniqKey="Stanford T">TR Stanford</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Quessy, S" uniqKey="Quessy S">S Quessy</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stein, Be" uniqKey="Stein B">BE Stein</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
</listBibl>
</div1>
</back>
</TEI>
<pmc article-type="research-article">
<pmc-dir>properties open_access</pmc-dir>
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="nlm-ta">PLoS One</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="publisher-id">plos</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="pmc">plosone</journal-id>
<journal-title-group>
<journal-title>PLoS ONE</journal-title>
</journal-title-group>
<issn pub-type="epub">1932-6203</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Public Library of Science</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>San Francisco, USA</publisher-loc>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmid">21423770</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmc">3053393</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="publisher-id">PONE-D-11-02205</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0017777</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Research Article</subject>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v2">
<subject>Biology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Model Organisms</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Animal Models</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Chicken</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group>
<subject>Neuroscience</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Neurophysiology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Central Nervous System</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group>
<subject>Behavioral Neuroscience</subject>
<subject>Learning and Memory</subject>
<subject>Neuroethology</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>Neuronal Plasticity and Multisensory Integration in Filial Imprinting</article-title>
<alt-title alt-title-type="running-head">Multisensory Plasticity in Filial Imprinting</alt-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Town</surname>
<given-names>Stephen Michael</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff1"></xref>
<xref ref-type="corresp" rid="cor1">
<sup>*</sup>
</xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>McCabe</surname>
<given-names>Brian John</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff1"></xref>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<aff id="aff1">
<addr-line>Sub-department of Animal Behaviour, Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom</addr-line>
</aff>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Chapouthier</surname>
<given-names>Georges</given-names>
</name>
<role>Editor</role>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="edit1"></xref>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<aff id="edit1">Université Pierre et Marie Curie, France</aff>
<author-notes>
<corresp id="cor1">* E-mail:
<email>stephentown42@gmail.com</email>
</corresp>
<fn fn-type="con">
<p>Conceived and designed the experiments: SMT. Performed the experiments: SMT. Analyzed the data: SMT. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: SMT BJM. Wrote the paper: SMT BJM.</p>
</fn>
</author-notes>
<pub-date pub-type="collection">
<year>2011</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="epub">
<day>10</day>
<month>3</month>
<year>2011</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>6</volume>
<issue>3</issue>
<elocation-id>e17777</elocation-id>
<history>
<date date-type="received">
<day>26</day>
<month>1</month>
<year>2011</year>
</date>
<date date-type="accepted">
<day>9</day>
<month>2</month>
<year>2011</year>
</date>
</history>
<permissions>
<copyright-statement>Town, McCabe. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.</copyright-statement>
</permissions>
<abstract>
<p>Many organisms sample their environment through multiple sensory systems and the integration of multisensory information enhances learning. However, the mechanisms underlying multisensory memory formation and their similarity to unisensory mechanisms remain unclear. Filial imprinting is one example in which experience is multisensory, and the mechanisms of unisensory neuronal plasticity are well established. We investigated the storage of audiovisual information through experience by comparing the activity of neurons in the intermediate and medial mesopallium of imprinted and naïve domestic chicks (
<italic>Gallus gallus domesticus</italic>
) in response to an audiovisual imprinting stimulus and novel object and their auditory and visual components. We find that imprinting enhanced the mean response magnitude of neurons to unisensory but not multisensory stimuli. Furthermore, imprinting enhanced responses to incongruent audiovisual stimuli comprised of mismatched auditory and visual components. Our results suggest that the effects of imprinting on the unisensory and multisensory responsiveness of IMM neurons differ and that IMM neurons may function to detect unexpected deviations from the audiovisual imprinting stimulus.</p>
</abstract>
<counts>
<page-count count="11"></page-count>
</counts>
</article-meta>
</front>
<body>
<sec id="s1">
<title>Introduction</title>
<p>The integration of information from several sensory modalities offers an enriched perception of the world and provides a more robust method for representing and recognizing objects. Multisensory integration increases information content and disambiguates information that might otherwise have multiple interpretations
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Ernst1">[1]</xref>
. Furthermore, integrating multisensory information enhances the reliability of sensory estimates
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Ernst2">[2]</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Alais1">[3]</xref>
and increases the speed of perceptual learning
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Seitz1">[4]</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Kim1">[5]</xref>
.</p>
<p>How is multisensory information stored in the brain? The neuronal basis of multisensory integration has been investigated in several behaviors
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Stein1">[6]</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Ghazanfar1">[7]</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Shams1">[8]</xref>
. However, relatively few studies have directly assessed the effect of experience on the neuronal representation of multisensory information: Familiar and unfamiliar audiovisual stimuli evoke differential activation of the posterior superior temporal sulcus
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Hein1">[9]</xref>
, left inferior frontal cortex, intraparietal sulcus
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Naumer1">[10]</xref>
, occipitotemporal junction and parahippocampal gyrus
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Tanabe1">[11]</xref>
. However, at the level of the single neuron, comparable studies of stimulus familiarity are lacking and therefore the neuronal basis of multisensory memory formation remains unclear.</p>
<p>Additionally, one would like to know whether the mechanisms underlying the storage of unisensory and multisensory information resemble one another. Despite the prevalence of multisensory stimuli in the natural world, many studies of object recognition have investigated the representation of unisensory information
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Xiang1">[12]</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Horn1">[13]</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Nakamori1">[14]</xref>
. It therefore remains to be tested whether the findings of these studies extend to multisensory information and whether one can explain multisensory information storage in terms of the storage of information about its unisensory components.</p>
<p>To address these questions, we studied an animal model of object recognition; filial imprinting, in which young birds learn to recognize an audiovisual stimulus
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Sluckin1">[15]</xref>
. We recorded neurons from a critical forebrain region
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-McCabe1">[16]</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Horn2">[17]</xref>
; the intermediate and medial mesopallium (IMM) of imprinted and naïve domestic chicks during presentation of an audiovisual imprinting stimulus and novel object, and of their auditory and visual components. We presented a fully balanced stimulus set (
<xref ref-type="table" rid="pone-0017777-t001">Table 1</xref>
) that included incongruent audiovisual combinations, in which the auditory and visual components of an imprinting stimulus and novel object were mismatched. This experimental design allowed us to compare the effect of imprinting on unisensory and multisensory neuronal responses and to investigate the nature of any multisensory representation formed through experience. We find that imprinting enhanced the mean magnitude of neuronal response to unisensory components of the imprinting stimuli but not to the multisensory imprinting stimulus itself. Rather imprinting most strongly enhanced the response of neurons to a mismatched audiovisual stimulus combining the visual component of the imprinting stimulus and auditory component of a novel object.</p>
<table-wrap id="pone-0017777-t001" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0017777.t001</object-id>
<label>Table 1</label>
<caption>
<title>Stimulus set presented to each animal with abbreviations.</title>
</caption>
<alternatives>
<graphic id="pone-0017777-t001-1" xlink:href="pone.0017777.t001"></graphic>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<colgroup span="1">
<col align="left" span="1"></col>
<col align="center" span="1"></col>
<col align="center" span="1"></col>
<col align="center" span="1"></col>
<col align="center" span="1"></col>
</colgroup>
<thead>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1"></td>
<td colspan="4" align="left" rowspan="1">Visual Component</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Auditory component</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">None (Unisensory)</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Imprinting stimulus</td>
<td colspan="2" align="left" rowspan="1">Novel Object</td>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">None (Unisensory)</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Not Applicable</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>V
<sub>IS</sub>
</bold>
</td>
<td colspan="2" align="left" rowspan="1">
<bold>V
<sub>NO</sub>
</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Imprinting stimulus</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>A
<sub>IS</sub>
</bold>
</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
</bold>
</td>
<td colspan="2" align="left" rowspan="1">
<bold>A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>NO</sub>
</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Novel Object</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>A
<sub>NO</sub>
</bold>
</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
</bold>
</td>
<td colspan="2" align="left" rowspan="1">
<bold>A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>NO</sub>
</bold>
</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</alternatives>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="nt101">
<label></label>
<p>Each stimulus was presented for 4 seconds 15–20 times with a minimum inter-stimulus interval of 4 s.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
</sec>
<sec id="s2">
<title>Results</title>
<p>We recorded activity from 157 neurons in the IMM of three imprinted and three naive chicks (see
<xref ref-type="sec" rid="s4">Methods</xref>
) during presentation of an audiovisual imprinting stimulus (IS) and novel object (NO) and their auditory and visual components. We characterized the response of each neuron to each stimulus using response magnitude - the firing rate during presentation expressed as a percentage of the baseline firing rate measured before presentation.</p>
<sec id="s2a">
<title>Visual Stimuli (V
<sub>IS</sub>
and V
<sub>NO</sub>
)</title>
<p>We found that neurons recorded from imprinted chicks responded more strongly to the visual component of the imprinting stimulus (V
<sub>IS</sub>
) than the visual component of the novel object (V
<sub>NO</sub>
) whereas neurons recorded from naïve chicks did not.
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g001">
<bold>Figure 1a</bold>
</xref>
shows the firing rate of two neurons recorded from an imprinted chick and a naïve chick before and during presentation of visual stimuli. Across the neuronal population (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g001">
<bold>Fig. 1b</bold>
</xref>
), we found that the mean response magnitude was significantly greater towards V
<sub>IS</sub>
than V
<sub>NO</sub>
in the neurons recorded from imprinted by not naïve chicks (ANOVA, interaction between effects of group and stimulus:
<italic>F</italic>
<sub>1, 155</sub>
 = 6.01,
<italic>P</italic>
 = 0.015).</p>
<fig id="pone-0017777-g001" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0017777.g001</object-id>
<label>Figure 1</label>
<caption>
<title>Single cell and population responses to visual stimuli.</title>
<p>(
<bold>A</bold>
) Raster plot and peri-stimulus time histograms illustrating the firing rate of two neurons recorded from a naïve and an imprinted chick before and during presentation of the visual components of the imprinting stimulus (V
<sub>IS</sub>
) and novel object (V
<sub>NO</sub>
). Percentage values indicate the response magnitude calculated as the firing rate during presentation (0 to 4 s) expressed as a percentage of pre-stimulus baseline firing rate (−4 to 0 s). (
<bold>B</bold>
) Mean (± s.e.m.) response magnitude of neuronal populations recorded in naïve (white: n = 85) and imprinted (black: n = 72) chicks. (*) indicates a significant interaction between group and stimulus (
<italic>P</italic>
 = 0.015).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0017777.g001"></graphic>
</fig>
</sec>
<sec id="s2b">
<title>Auditory Stimuli (A
<sub>IS</sub>
and A
<sub>NO</sub>
)</title>
<p>Similarly, the neuron presented in
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g001">
<bold>figure 1a</bold>
</xref>
recorded from an imprinted chick responded more strongly to the auditory component of the imprinting stimulus (A
<sub>IS</sub>
) than that of the novel object (A
<sub>NO</sub>
) whereas the neuron recorded from a naïve chick showed little difference in response between stimuli (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g002">
<bold>Fig. 2a</bold>
</xref>
). Across the recorded populations, the mean response of neurons from imprinted chicks to A
<sub>IS</sub>
was greater than to A
<sub>NO</sub>
whereas the mean response of neurons from naïve chicks to A
<sub>IS</sub>
was weaker than to A
<sub>NO</sub>
(
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g002">
<bold>Fig. 2b</bold>
</xref>
) (interaction between group and stimulus:
<italic>F</italic>
<sub>1, 155</sub>
 = 5.86,
<italic>P</italic>
 = 0.017).</p>
<fig id="pone-0017777-g002" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0017777.g002</object-id>
<label>Figure 2</label>
<caption>
<title>Single cell and population responses to auditory stimuli.</title>
<p>(
<bold>A</bold>
) Raster plot and peri-stimulus time histograms illustrating the firing rate of the same neurons shown in
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g001">figure 1</xref>
before and during presentation of the auditory components of the imprinting stimulus (A
<sub>IS</sub>
) and novel object (A
<sub>NO</sub>
). Percentage values indicate the response magnitude calculated as the firing rate during presentation (0 to 4 s) expressed as a percentage of pre-stimulus baseline firing rate (−4 to 0 s). (
<bold>B</bold>
) Mean (± s.e.m.) response magnitude of neuronal populations recorded in naïve (white) and imprinted (black) chicks. (*) indicates a significant interaction between group and stimulus (
<italic>P</italic>
 = 0.017).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0017777.g002"></graphic>
</fig>
</sec>
<sec id="s2c">
<title>Congruent Audiovisual Stimuli (A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
and A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>NO</sub>
)</title>
<p>In contrast, single neurons recorded from the imprinted chick such as that presented in
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g001">
<bold>Fig. 1</bold>
</xref>
and
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g002">
<bold>Fig. 2</bold>
</xref>
responded similarly to the audiovisual imprinting stimulus (A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
) and novel object (A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>NO</sub>
) and there was little difference in response to the audiovisual imprinting stimulus between neurons recorded from imprinted and naïve chicks (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g003">
<bold>Fig. 3a</bold>
</xref>
). Comparison between neuronal populations recorded from imprinted and naïve chicks revealed no main effect of group or stimulus and no interaction between these factors (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g003">
<bold>Fig. 3b</bold>
</xref>
).</p>
<fig id="pone-0017777-g003" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0017777.g003</object-id>
<label>Figure 3</label>
<caption>
<title>Single cell and population responses to congruent audiovisual stimuli.</title>
<p>(
<bold>A</bold>
) Raster plot and peri-stimulus time histograms illustrating the firing rate of the same neurons from
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g001">figures 1</xref>
and
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g002">2</xref>
before and during presentation of the audiovisual imprinting stimulus (A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
) and novel object (A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>NO</sub>
). Percentage values indicate the response magnitude calculated as the firing rate during presentation (0 to 4 s) expressed as a percentage of pre-stimulus baseline firing rate (−4 to 0 s). (
<bold>B</bold>
) Mean (± s.e.m.) response magnitude of neuronal populations recorded in naïve (white) and imprinted (black) chicks. (ns) indicates the absence of interaction between group and stimulus.</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0017777.g003"></graphic>
</fig>
<p>The effect of imprinting on mean response magnitude to the imprinting stimulus varied with modality (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g004">
<bold>Fig. 4</bold>
</xref>
): Comparison between visual and audiovisual modalities demonstrated that enhancement of mean response magnitude to V
<sub>IS</sub>
was absent for A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
(interaction between group and modality;
<italic>F</italic>
<sub>1, 155</sub>
 = 3.93,
<italic>P</italic>
 = 0.049). Similarly, comparison between auditory (A
<sub>IS</sub>
) and audiovisual (A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
) modalities demonstrated that the enhancement of mean response magnitude was limited to auditory stimuli (
<italic>F</italic>
<sub>1, 154</sub>
 = 7.91,
<italic>P</italic>
 = 0.006). Thus imprinting leads to the modification of neuronal responses that are limited to the unisensory components of the imprinting stimulus. Comparison between the effects of imprinting on responses to the audiovisual novel object and its auditory or visual components revealed no interactions between modality and group.</p>
<fig id="pone-0017777-g004" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0017777.g004</object-id>
<label>Figure 4</label>
<caption>
<title>Comparison between the effects of imprinting on different modalities.</title>
<p>Mean (± s.e.m.) response magnitude to the audiovisual imprinting stimulus (A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
) and its auditory (A
<sub>IS</sub>
) and visual components (V
<sub>IS</sub>
). (**) indicates interaction between the effects of imprinting on auditory and audiovisual stimuli (
<italic>P</italic>
 = 0.006). (*) indicates interaction between the effects of imprinting on auditory and audiovisual stimuli (
<italic>P</italic>
 = 0.049).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0017777.g004"></graphic>
</fig>
</sec>
<sec id="s2d">
<title>Incongruent Audiovisual Stimuli (A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>NO</sub>
and A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
)</title>
<p>By mismatching the auditory and visual components of the imprinting stimulus and novel object, it was possible to create two incongruent audiovisual stimuli (A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>NO</sub>
and A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
). We found that single neurons recorded from imprinted chicks responded more strongly to the combination A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
than neurons recorded from naïve chicks (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g005">
<bold>Fig. 5a</bold>
</xref>
). Comparing neuronal populations revealed a significant effect of group (
<italic>F</italic>
<sub>1, 155</sub>
 = 26.23,
<italic>P</italic>
<0.001) (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g005">
<bold>Fig. 5b</bold>
</xref>
). However, there was no effect of imprinting on the mean response to the alternative incongruent combination A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>NO</sub>
(
<italic>P</italic>
>0.1).</p>
<fig id="pone-0017777-g005" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0017777.g005</object-id>
<label>Figure 5</label>
<caption>
<title>Single cell and population responses to incongruent audiovisual stimuli.</title>
<p>(
<bold>A</bold>
) Raster plot and peri-stimulus time histograms illustrating the firing rate of the same neurons from
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g001">figures 1</xref>
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g002"></xref>
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g003">3</xref>
before and during presentation of incongruent audiovisual stimulus (A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
and A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>NO</sub>
). Percentage values indicate the response magnitude calculated as the firing rate during presentation (0 to 4 s) expressed as a percentage of pre-stimulus baseline firing rate (−4 to 0 s). (
<bold>B</bold>
) Mean (± s.e.m.) response magnitude of neuronal populations recorded in naïve (white) and imprinted (black) chicks. (*) indicates a significant effect of group on mean response magnitude to A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
(
<italic>P</italic>
<0.001).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0017777.g005"></graphic>
</fig>
</sec>
<sec id="s2e">
<title>Multisensory Integration</title>
<p>We also investigated whether multisensory integration was affected by imprinting by calculating an additivity index for each neuron for each audiovisual stimulus. In order to calculate additivity, we measured the change in firing rate during presentation of an audiovisual stimulus (AV) and its auditory (A) and visual (V) components (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g006">
<bold>Fig. 6a</bold>
</xref>
). We then calculated the difference between the change in firing rate during presentation of AV and the sum of changes in firing rate during presentation of A and V, and divided this by the total sum of changes in firing rate (see
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g006">
<bold>Fig. 6a</bold>
</xref>
and
<xref ref-type="sec" rid="s4">Methods</xref>
). The resulting variable therefore ranges from −1 to 1 with values greater than and less than zero indicate subadditivity and superadditivity respectively, whereas zero indicates that the change in firing rate during presentation of the audiovisual stimulus is equal to the sum of changes in firing rated during presentation of its auditory and visual components.</p>
<fig id="pone-0017777-g006" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0017777.g006</object-id>
<label>Figure 6</label>
<caption>
<title>Experience-dependent audiovisual integration.</title>
<p>(
<bold>A</bold>
) The response of a neuron recorded from an imprinted chick to the audiovisual imprinting stimulus (A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
) and its auditory (A
<sub>IS</sub>
) and visual components (V
<sub>IS</sub>
) (see also
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g001">Figs. 1</xref>
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g003">3</xref>
). The change in firing rate was calculated for each stimulus and used to calculate the additivity index. (
<bold>B</bold>
) Mean (± s.e.m.) additvity index of neuronal populations recorded from naive and imprinted chicks to the audiovisual imprinting stimulus (A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
) and novel object (A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>NO</sub>
) and to incongruent audiovisual stimuli (A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
and A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>NO</sub>
). (***) indicates significant effect of group on additivity index of A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
(
<italic>P</italic>
<0.001).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0017777.g006"></graphic>
</fig>
<p>
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g006">
<bold>Figure 6b</bold>
</xref>
shows the mean additivity index for neurons recorded from naïve and imprinted chicks for each audiovisual stimulus. In accordance with our earlier findings, imprinting led to the increase in additivity in the case of incongruent responses to A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
(
<italic>T</italic>
<sub>153</sub>
 = 4.68,
<italic>P</italic>
<0.001). This result can be explained by the strong increase in audiovisual response induced by imprinting, coupled with the weaker increase in familiar visual response and decrease in unfamiliar auditory response. The difference in mean additivity between groups was not significant for any of the other audiovisual stimuli.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="s3">
<title>Discussion</title>
<sec id="s3a">
<title>Unisensory and multisensory neuroplasticity</title>
<p>We report an imprinting-related enhancement in responses of neurons within the IMM for the auditory and visual components of an imprinting stimulus but not the audiovisual imprinting stimulus itself. This leads us to conclude that imprinting-related enhancement of the response magnitude of IMM neurons is limited to the unisensory components of an imprinting stimulus and does not extend to the audiovisual compound.</p>
<p>Our findings are consistent with earlier reports of the selective enhancement of neuronal responsiveness to auditory and visual components of an imprinting stimulus
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Horn1">[13]</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Brown1">[18]</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Nicol1">[19]</xref>
. Our findings also support preliminary results of more recent work in which the proportion of IMM neurons found to be responsive to the visual component of an imprinting stimulus increased following imprinting whereas the proportion responsive to the audiovisual imprinting stimulus did not (Nicol & Horn,
<italic>Proceedings of the Physiological Society</italic>
2009 Cardiff, UK. Available at
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.physoc.org/Proceedings">www.physoc.org/Proceedings</ext-link>
: Last Accessed Jan 2011).</p>
<p>However, our findings conflict with an earlier study by Brown and Horn
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Brown1">[18]</xref>
in which the proportion of sites within the IMM responsive to an audiovisual imprinting stimulus increased with imprinting (albeit, this increase was smaller than that reported for the visual component of the imprinting stimulus). This finding led to the assumption that imprinting similarly affects responsiveness to the audiovisual imprinting stimulus and its visual component
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Jackson1">[20]</xref>
. Our findings challenge this assumption and suggest that unisensory and multisensory stimuli cannot be considered equivalent in the study of the neurophysiological basis of filial imprinting. The disparity between the present findings and those of Brown and Horn cannot be attributed to the difference in measurement used to characterize neurons (proportion of responsive sites vs. response magnitude) because reanalysis of our data according to the same method confirmed our finding: imprinting enhanced the proportion of sites responsive to the visual component of the imprinting stimulus but not the audiovisual stimulus itself (
<xref ref-type="table" rid="pone-0017777-t002">Table 2</xref>
). Furthermore, recent work by other investigators has also found a dissociation in the effects of imprinting on proportion of responsive neurons to the audiovisual imprinting stimulus and its visual component (Nicol & Horn,
<italic>Proceedings of the Physiological Society</italic>
2009 Cardiff, UK. Available at
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.physoc.org/Proceedings">www.physoc.org/Proceedings</ext-link>
: Last Accessed Jan 2011). Our results may differ from those of Brown and Horn because of differences in recording method: In the current study, we used tetrodes to identify the responses of single neurons whereas the earlier results were obtained using multi-unit recordings of the activity of clusters of neurons. It is possible that multi-unit recordings are limited in their sensitivity as a particularly responsive neuron can cause an entire cluster to be identified as responsive when the majority of units are unresponsive. Tetrodes allow the separation of neurons within such a cluster and therefore may avoid such biases, providing a more sensitive index of neuronal activity that could explain the difference between past studies and the present findings.</p>
<table-wrap id="pone-0017777-t002" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0017777.t002</object-id>
<label>Table 2</label>
<caption>
<title>Proportion of neurons responsive to the audiovisual imprinting stimulus and its visual component.</title>
</caption>
<alternatives>
<graphic id="pone-0017777-t002-2" xlink:href="pone.0017777.t002"></graphic>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<colgroup span="1">
<col align="left" span="1"></col>
<col align="center" span="1"></col>
<col align="center" span="1"></col>
<col align="center" span="1"></col>
</colgroup>
<thead>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<underline>Modality</underline>
</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<underline>Group</underline>
</td>
<td colspan="2" align="center" rowspan="1">
<underline>Proportion responsive</underline>
</td>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Visual</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Naïve</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">17/85</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">20.0%</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1"></td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Imprinted</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">24/72</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">33.3%</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Audiovisual</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Naïve</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">39/85</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">45.9%</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1"></td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Imprinted</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">33/72</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">45.8%</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</alternatives>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="nt102">
<label></label>
<p>Responsive neurons were defined as those whose firing rate during stimulus presentation significantly differed from the baseline firing rate before stimulus presentation (T-test: see ref.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Brown1">[18]</xref>
for details).</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
<p>The dissociation between changes in mean response magnitude to the multisensory imprinting stimulus and its unisensory components may be explained by the principle of inverse effectiveness. This principle describes the phenomenon occurring in both mammals and birds in which the effect of adding an additional modality to a stimulus on response magnitude is inversely proportional to the original salience of the stimulus when presented alone
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Meredith1">[22]</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Zahar1">[23]</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Kayser1">[24]</xref>
. In the current study, the enhancement of neuronal responses to the auditory or visual components of the imprinting stimulus may lead to a reduction in the effectiveness of adding a second modality when the audiovisual imprinting stimulus is presented. This would lead to a relatively constant mean response magnitude to the audiovisual imprinting stimulus despite an increase in response magnitude to auditory and visual components, as we report. This interpretation is supported by the relatively weak correlation between response magnitude of neurons to the visual and audiovisual imprinting stimulus (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g007">
<bold>Fig. 7a</bold>
</xref>
) and not novel object (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g007">
<bold>Fig. 7c</bold>
</xref>
). However, there are strong correlations between the response of neurons to audiovisual stimuli and their auditory components, both for the imprinting stimulus (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g007">
<bold>Fig. 7b</bold>
</xref>
) and the novel object (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g007">
<bold>Fig. 7d</bold>
</xref>
). These correlations would not be predicted by the principle of inverse effectiveness; however it is possible that correlations are present because auditory stimuli are sufficiently salient that little enhancement through multisensory integration occurs anyway (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g004">
<bold>Fig. 4</bold>
</xref>
). Under such circumstances, correlations between auditory and audiovisual responses might be expected as the audiovisual stimulus is no more salient than its auditory component.</p>
<fig id="pone-0017777-g007" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0017777.g007</object-id>
<label>Figure 7</label>
<caption>
<title>Principle of inverse effectiveness.</title>
<p>Axes indicate the response magnitude (% of baseline activity) of individual neurons recorded from naïve (grey) and imprinted (black) chicks to unisensory (x-axis) and multisensory stimuli (y-axis). Equations and r-values indicate regression and correlation coefficients respectively. (
<bold>A</bold>
) Visual component (V
<sub>IS</sub>
) vs. audiovisual imprinting stimulus (A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
). (
<bold>B</bold>
) Auditory component (A
<sub>IS</sub>
) vs. audiovisual imprinting stimulus (A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
). (
<bold>C</bold>
) Visual component (V
<sub>NO</sub>
) vs. audiovisual novel object (A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>NO</sub>
). (
<bold>D</bold>
) Auditory component (A
<sub>NO</sub>
) vs. audiovisual novel object (A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>NO</sub>
).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0017777.g007"></graphic>
</fig>
<p>The relationship between the behavior of chicks and the responses of IMM neurons to the audiovisual imprinting stimulus and novel object remains unclear. Imprinted but not naïve chicks were able to discriminate between the audiovisual imprinting stimulus and novel object (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g008">
<bold>Fig. 8</bold>
</xref>
) yet the mean response magnitude of neurons to both stimuli in both groups were similar (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone-0017777-g004">
<bold>Fig. 4</bold>
</xref>
). It therefore seems unlikely that response magnitudes of IMM neurons directly contribute to the discrimination between imprinting stimulus and novel object; rather our results suggest the neurons within the IMM may serve to identify unexpected auditory properties of the imprinting stimulus (see below).</p>
<fig id="pone-0017777-g008" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0017777.g008</object-id>
<label>Figure 8</label>
<caption>
<title>Behaviour during presentation of audiovisual stimuli.</title>
<p>Preference scores for audiovisual imprinting stimulus. Box plots indicate median preference scores (center bar), upper and lower quartiles (box) and whiskers represent the range. (**) Comparison of individual medians revealed that the preferences for the audiovisual imprinting stimulus of imprinted (n = 8) but not naïve chicks (n = 9) were greater than chance (50%, sign test: naïve chicks,
<italic>P</italic>
>0.5; imprinted,
<italic>P</italic>
 = 0.008).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0017777.g008"></graphic>
</fig>
</sec>
<sec id="s3b">
<title>Audiovisual Incongruence</title>
<p>We report that the mean response magnitude to the incongruent audiovisual stimulus A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
(the visual component of the imprinting stimulus combined with the auditory component of the novel object) was greater in imprinted than naïve chicks. This result should be interpreted with caution as the difference in response magnitude derived mainly from the unusually weak responses to A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
recorded from neurons in naïve chicks. There is no prior reason to expect this audiovisual stimulus to differ so notably in its salience to naïve chicks from other audiovisual stimuli, raising the possibility that the finding is anomalous. However, there is also no reason to believe that the recording of neuronal responses to A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
was any less accurate than all other audiovisual stimuli: Presentation order was randomized across neuronal tests, making it unlikely that a consistent time of presentation biased the results. Furthermore, the analysis of neuronal activity and calculation of response magnitude following single unit isolation was automated for all stimuli, and therefore any inaccuracy in measurement of response magnitude for A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
should also be manifest in the measurement of response magnitude for all other stimuli. Moreover, it is also notable that of all audiovisual stimuli, A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
evoked the strongest mean response magnitude in the population recorded from imprinted chicks. We therefore believe that it is unlikely that the imprinting-related enhancement of mean response magnitude to A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
was anomalous but rather interpret the effect tentatively as a result of imprinting that may reveal important details regarding the function of IMM neurons; namely the detection of incongruous auditory accompaniments to the visual imprinting stimulus.</p>
<p>The suggestion that imprinting enhances neuronal responses to incongruous auditory accompaniments of the visual imprinting stimulus is consistent with the finding that the mean response magnitude to the visual component of the imprinting stimulus (V
<sub>IS</sub>
) was stronger in imprinted than naïve birds, as presentation of V
<sub>IS</sub>
in the absence of any call could be considered an incongruent auditory condition given the original imprinting exposure was to the audiovisual compound A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
. The suggestion is also consistent with the more general proposal that neurons in the IMM respond to unexpected variations from the original imprinting experience as neurons in the IMM also respond more strongly to A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
when presented in an unfamiliar than familiar visual context following imprinting (Town & McCabe, Unpublished). It remains to be seen whether this hypothesis accurately predicts the effects of imprinting on neuronal responses to more ethologically relevant stimuli such as live hens and naturalistic situations.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="s3c">
<title>Multisensory integration in the IMM</title>
<p>IMM neurons recorded from imprinted chicks responded strongly to auditory stimuli and to the visual component of the imprinting stimulus demonstrating that, at least following imprinting, information from multiple sensory modalities is integrated in the IMM. The ability of IMM neurons to respond to multiple modalities of sensory information is consistent with previous findings
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Brown1">[18]</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Nicol1">[19]</xref>
and the projection of afferents from visual (optic tectum, arcopallium intermedium, nidopallium and the Wulst) and auditory (Field L) regions of the brain to the IMM
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Bradley1">[25]</xref>
. The afferents sent to the IMM from the nidopallium may also convey somatosensory information
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Bradley1">[25]</xref>
and chicks show the ability to imprint on tactile information
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Taylor1">[26]</xref>
. It would therefore be interesting to test whether IMM neurons also respond to somatosensory stimuli and whether these responses are dependent upon imprinting.</p>
<p>When calculating additivity in the IMM, we found that mean values of neurons recorded from imprinted chicks were near zero for the incongruent audiovisual stimulus A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
(mean ± s.e.m  =  −1.78±4.5; comparison vs. 0:
<italic>P</italic>
>0.5). This suggests that on average, the sum of changes in neuronal activity during presentation of the audiovisual stimulus was similar to the sum of changes in neuronal activity during separate presentation of its unisensory components. Mean additivity was also near zero for the incongruent stimulus A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>NO</sub>
and audiovisual imprinting stimulus; however neither is likely to reflect audiovisual integration at the population level because the visual component (V
<sub>NO</sub>
) of the stimulus did not evoke strong responses from neurons and therefore a mean additivity value near zero may reflect similar responses to audiovisual and auditory stimuli. Thus, at least during presentation of A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS,</sub>
neurons may integrate visual and auditory information.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="s3d">
<title>Remaining Questions</title>
<p>At present it remains unclear how incongruence detection is performed, or what its function might be. Additionally, it is unclear how auditory and visual information may be integrated, at least during the response of neurons recorded from imprinted chicks to A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
. Much of this obscurity stems from the lack of structural and biochemical knowledge about neurophysiologically characterized neurons.</p>
<p>In terms of incongruence detection and multisensory integration, the underlying mechanisms will depend upon the form in which information reaches the IMM. It is not clear whether auditory and visual information are provided through separate (auditory and visual) or mixed (auditory, visual and audiovisual) channels. Recent evidence has demonstrated that the optic tectum, a structure thought to provide visual input to the IMM, is capable of multisensory integration in the Barn Owl
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Zahar1">[23]</xref>
. Therefore, it may be likely that IMM neurons receive unisensory and multisensory information at synapses from structures that were originally described as unimodal. This speculation remains to be confirmed and will require paired recordings of neurons from the IMM and their presynaptic inputs from other regions of the brain. Furthermore, understanding the computations performed during synaptic integration within the dendritic tree will require the application of techniques such as
<italic>in-vivo</italic>
calcium-imaging to measure post-synaptic potentials at multiple synapses in deep tissue of behaving animals.</p>
<p>With regard to the function of incongruence detection, future studies will need to elucidate the regions of the brain to which specific neurons, responding most strongly to A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
and V
<sub>IS</sub>
, project. Neurons may project axons locally within the IMM or to relatively distant cognitive and motor regions such as the hyperpallium apicale, arcopallium (homologous to the mammalian amygdala) and striatum
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Bradley1">[25]</xref>
and therefore imprinting-related responses of an IMM neuron to A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
and V
<sub>IS</sub>
may serve one or more of several functions (e.g. social recognition, emotional behavior or generation of motor output) depending on its innervations pattern. It is notable therefore, that imprinted chicks did not differ greatly in the extent to which they approached A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
or A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
(i.e. congruent and incongruent stimuli) during stimulus presentations suggesting that the mean response magnitude of neurons within the IMM and approach behavior in the training wheel are not directly linked (median approach distance: A
<sub>IS</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
 =  1.25 m, A
<sub>NO</sub>
V
<sub>IS</sub>
 =  1.23 m). The nature of synapses in innervated regions will also be of crucial importance in understanding the function of IMM neurons; within the IMM there is an imprinting-related enhancement of potassium stimulated GABA (γ-aminobutyric acid) release suggesting that imprinting may alter the balance of inhibition within the IMM
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-McCabe2">[27]</xref>
; however understanding how changes in inhibitory synapses affect the circuits in which IMM neurons take part will require knowledge of the neurophysiological properties of pre- and post-synaptic neurons.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="s3e">
<title>Conclusions</title>
<p>In summary, we report a dissociation between the effects of imprinting on the responses of IMM neurons to an audiovisual imprinting stimulus and its auditory and visual components, challenging the existing assumption that the effects of imprinting on unisensory and multisensory responsiveness are equivalent. We report an enhancement in mean response magnitude to an incongruent audiovisual stimulus, suggesting that neurons within the IMM may signal incongruous auditory accompaniments to the visual component of the imprinting stimulus. In future, it will be important to simultaneously characterize the neurophysiological, structural and biochemical properties of neurons in order to better understand the function of the IMM during imprinting.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec sec-type="materials|methods" id="s4">
<title>Materials and Methods</title>
<sec id="s4a">
<title>Subjects</title>
<p>Subjects were domestic chicks (
<italic>Gallus gallus domesticus</italic>
, Ros 308 Strain) incubated and reared in darkness within incubators maintained at 32–35°C. All procedures were performed in accordance with the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986, under the UK Home Office Project License No. 80/2276 and were approved by the University Biomedical Support Services (UBSS) ethical review committee, University of Cambridge.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="s4b">
<title>Imprinting</title>
<p>Approximately 24 hours after hatching, 30 chicks were imprinted using methods similar to those described elsewhere
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-McCabe1">[16]</xref>
. Briefly; chicks were placed in running wheels within a darkened training box maintained at 30°C and exposed to an imprinting stimulus - a rotating, illuminated red box presented in conjunction with a maternal hen call (Call A, see below for further details about stimuli) - for two sessions of 60 minutes separated by an hour interval in which chicks were returned to incubators. Imprinted chicks were then identified by their ability to discriminate between the imprinting stimulus and novel object (a rotating, illuminated blue cylinder) in a sequential preference test in which chicks were returned to training wheels and presented with the visual component of the imprinting stimulus and novel object for two periods of four minutes in an ABBA design in which stimulus order was counterbalanced; neither stimulus was accompanied by explicit auditory stimulation. For each presentation, the distance the chick ran was measured and a preference score calculated as the distance run towards the imprinting stimulus (IS) as a percentage of the total distance run during the test (IS +NO):</p>
<p>Preference  =  100 × [IS / (IS + NO)]</p>
<p>For the imprinted group, only chicks with strong preferences for the imprinting stimulus (>70%: n = 8) were selected for surgical implantation of microelectrodes. Chicks in the naïve group (n = 9) remained in a holding incubator and received no exposure to the imprinting stimulus prior to implantation.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="s4c">
<title>Microelectrode Design and Implantation</title>
<p>Neuronal activity was recorded using four platinum/iridium wires (Neuralynx, Bozeman, MT, USA) wound together and bonded (tetrodes)
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Gray1">[21]</xref>
. In order to penetrate the brain, tetrodes were mounted onto thin (dia. 125 µm) tungsten wire (Advent Research Materials, Oxford, UK) with cyanoacrylate superglue. The resultant structure was then fixed into a guide cannula and the protruding end coated in 1,1′-dioctadecyl−3,3,3′,3′-tetramethylindocarbocyanine perchlorate (DiI) (Sigma); a neuronal tracer allowing electrode localization
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-DiCarlo1">[28]</xref>
. The tetrode tips were then gold-plated to an impedance of 0.2–0.4 MΩ prior to surgery.</p>
<p>Chicks were anaesthetized (0.12 ml Equithesin, intraperitoneal)
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Davies1">[29]</xref>
and positioned in a sterotaxic frame. A craniotomy was performed 0.8 mm lateral to the midline and 2.5 mm anterior to the frontoparietal suture and the dura mater removed. A microdrive assembly
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-CipollaNeto1">[30]</xref>
was then glued to the dorsal surface of the skull, allowing one tetrode to be positioned over the left or right IMM. A reference electrode was also placed under the skull permitting differential recording and the assembly was stabilized in dental cement. At the end of surgery, each tetrode was then advanced approximately 1.25 mm over a period of 2.5 hrs.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="s4d">
<title>Neuronal recording</title>
<p>Following recovery from surgery overnight, neuronal activity was detected in the awake animal, placed in a modified running wheel in which a tether connected the microdrive to recording equipment: Signals were amplified 10,000 times, band-pass filtered between 300 and 3,000 Hz (CyberAmp; Axon Instruments, Union City, CA, USA) and sampled at 14 kHz for offline analysis (Power1401 laboratory interface and Spike2; Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK). Tetrodes were advanced until spontaneous neuronal activity was detected and chicks were then presented with familiar and unfamiliar visual, auditory and audiovisual stimuli.</p>
<p>Detailed accounts of visual and auditory stimuli can be found elsewhere
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-McCabe1">[16]</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-VanKampen1">[31]</xref>
. Briefly; the visual stimulus was either a red and black box (9×17.5×18 cm; l×w×h), or a blue and white cylinder (diameter, 15.5 cm; height, 19 cm). Both were illuminated from within by 24 W bulbs, rotated at 30 revolutions per minute and placed 65 cm from the running wheel. During stimulus presentation, current was provided to the stimuli to cause illumination and rotation. Between presentations, the stimuli were dim and static and elicited little interest from the animals. Auditory stimuli were maternal calls (Calls A and B) recorded from two hens and presented at approximately 75 dB using a cassette player controlled by a TTL pulse from the Power1401 laboratory interface and a pair of loud speakers placed out of view of the animal. Audiovisual stimuli consisted of all possible combinations of visual and auditory components of the imprinting stimulus and novel object (
<xref ref-type="table" rid="pone-0017777-t001">Table 1</xref>
).</p>
<p>All stimulus presentations lasted four seconds and throughout presentation of visual and audiovisual stimuli, chicks were required to look towards the visual stimulus with both eyes during presentation. This was ensured by monitoring head position via video camera and excluding presentations in which either or both eyes were turned away from the stimulus. Stimuli were presented in a consecutive sequence an average of 15 times and the stimulus order was randomized between chicks. The approach behavior of the chick was also recorded during stimulus presentation and this data was used to confirm the ability of subjects to discriminate between audiovisual as well as visual stimuli: For each four second presentation, the distance run during presentation was measured and preference scores calculated using the mean distance run towards the audiovisual imprinting stimulus and novel object.</p>
<p>Following a testing session, tetrodes were advanced at least 200 µm to avoid repeated sampling of the same neurons. Chicks were then returned to holding incubators for at least 45 minutes between tests. In six birds, spontaneous activity was not satisfactorily detected at any depth and therefore only behavioral data from these birds were analyzed.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="s4e">
<title>Electrode Localization</title>
<p>At the end of the experiment, chicks were euthanized (0.1 ml Euthatal, intraperitoneal) and perfused transcardially with 0.9% saline and 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS (Sigma). The brain was removed and stored in 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS until 24 hours before sectioning, at which point the brain was transferred to 20% sucrose (Sigma). Frozen sections were then cut at 180 µm thickness and tetrode location confirmed by the presence of DiI stained neurons. Data from five subjects were excluded because tetrodes were positioned outside the IMM.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="s4f">
<title>Data Analysis</title>
<p>Single units were isolated from recorded data using standard cluster cutting techniques
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Gray1">[21]</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="pone.0017777-Lewicki1">[32]</xref>
. Briefly; events with amplitudes between two and ten times the background noise on at least one channel of the tetrode were selected by threshold detection (Spike2). Events were then sorted by waveform parameters and principal components using k-means and manual clustering. Events that did not resemble action potentials on at least one channel were discarded. Single unit isolation was assessed using spike interval histograms and visual inspection of waveform shape; the minimum interval between spikes was greater than 2 ms for all neurons. Following isolation, the times of stimulus presentation and spikes were saved and subsequently analyzed in Matlab (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA).</p>
<p>Single-unit responses were then assessed using the normalized response magnitude, calculated as:</p>
<p>RM  =  100 × (P/B).</p>
<p>Where P is the mean firing rate of a neuron during the 4 s stimulus presentation, and B is the firing rate in the 4 s baseline period before presentation. Mean response magnitudes to the audiovisual imprinting stimulus and novel object, their auditory and visual components and incongruent audiovisual stimuli were compared between imprinted and naive birds in a 2×2 (stimulus × group) analysis of variance (ANOVA; Genstat, VSN International, Hemel Hempstead, UK). Modality replaced stimulus as a factor for comparisons between visual or auditory and audiovisual imprinting stimuli. By comparing naïve and imprinted birds it was possible to control for stimulus salience. By comparing neuronal responses to the imprinting stimulus and novel object or their unisensory components, it was possible to determine whether the effect of imprinting was generalized or specific to the imprinting stimuli previously experienced. Therefore by observing the interaction of group and stimulus, we could exclude the influences of stimulus salience or generalization from our interpretation. Regression and correlation coefficients used to describe the relationship between the magnitudes of responses to audiovisual stimuli and their auditory and visual components were calculated in Matlab.</p>
<p>For each neuron we also characterized the integration of auditory and visual information using the additivity index of multisensory integration [modified from 33]. In our index, additivity was calculated in two stages, firstly we calculated the corrected the firing rate of a neuron in response to an audiovisual stimulus (AV) by deducting the baseline firing rate before presentation (B
<sub>AV</sub>
) from the firing rate during presentation (P
<sub>AV</sub>
). The same corrections were also applied to responses to auditory (A) and visual (V) components of the audiovisual stimulus:</p>
<p>AV  =  P
<sub>AV</sub>
− B
<sub>AV</sub>
</p>
<p>A  =  P
<sub>A</sub>
− B
<sub>A</sub>
</p>
<p>V  =  P
<sub>V</sub>
− B
<sub>V</sub>
</p>
<p>Corrected firing rates were then used in the following equation to calculate additivity:</p>
<p>Additivity  =  (AV − A − V) / (|AV| + |A| + |V|)</p>
<p>Denominator values were made absolute because the combination of positive and negative values (i.e. responses at a rate lower than baseline firing rate) could lead to cancellation that made the total sum of neuronal activity inaccurately low. For each audiovisual stimulus, the mean additivity index was compared between imprinted and naïve birds by t-test.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
</body>
<back>
<ack>
<p>We thank Zoltan Cseresnyes for assistance with confocal microscopy and Gabriel Horn for valuable discussion.</p>
</ack>
<fn-group>
<fn fn-type="conflict">
<p>
<bold>Competing Interests: </bold>
The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.</p>
</fn>
<fn fn-type="financial-disclosure">
<p>
<bold>Funding: </bold>
This research was supported by a BBSRC studentship and a research grant from the Balfour Trust fund. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.</p>
</fn>
</fn-group>
<ref-list>
<title>References</title>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Ernst1">
<label>1</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ernst</surname>
<given-names>MO</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bulthoff</surname>
<given-names>HH</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>2004</year>
<article-title>Merging the senses into a robust percept.</article-title>
<source>Trends in Cognitive Sciences</source>
<volume>8</volume>
<fpage>162</fpage>
<lpage>169</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15050512</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Ernst2">
<label>2</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ernst</surname>
<given-names>MO</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Banks</surname>
<given-names>MS</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>2002</year>
<article-title>Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion.</article-title>
<source>Nature</source>
<volume>415</volume>
<fpage>429</fpage>
<lpage>433</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11807554</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Alais1">
<label>3</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Alais</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Burr</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>2004</year>
<article-title>The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration.</article-title>
<source>Current Biology</source>
<volume>14</volume>
<fpage>257</fpage>
<lpage>262</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14761661</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Seitz1">
<label>4</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Seitz</surname>
<given-names>AR</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kim</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Shams</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>2006</year>
<article-title>Sound facilitates visual learning.</article-title>
<source>Current Biology</source>
<volume>16</volume>
<fpage>1422</fpage>
<lpage>1427</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16860741</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Kim1">
<label>5</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kim</surname>
<given-names>RS</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Seitz</surname>
<given-names>AR</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Shams</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>2008</year>
<article-title>Benefits of Stimulus Congruency for Multisensory Facilitation of Visual Learning.</article-title>
<source>Plos One</source>
<volume>3</volume>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Stein1">
<label>6</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Stein</surname>
<given-names>BE</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Meredith</surname>
<given-names>MA</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>1993</year>
<article-title>The Merging of the Senses.</article-title>
<publisher-loc>Cambridge, Massachusetts</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>The MIT Press</publisher-name>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Ghazanfar1">
<label>7</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ghazanfar</surname>
<given-names>AA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schroeder</surname>
<given-names>CE</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>2006</year>
<article-title>Is neocortex essentially multisensory?</article-title>
<source>Trends in Cognitive Sciences</source>
<volume>10</volume>
<fpage>278</fpage>
<lpage>285</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16713325</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Shams1">
<label>8</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Shams</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Seitz</surname>
<given-names>AR</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>2008</year>
<article-title>Benefits of multisensory learning.</article-title>
<source>Trends in Cognitive Sciences</source>
<volume>12</volume>
<fpage>411</fpage>
<lpage>417</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18805039</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Hein1">
<label>9</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hein</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Doehrmann</surname>
<given-names>O</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Muller</surname>
<given-names>NG</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kaiser</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Muckli</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
<year>2007</year>
<article-title>Object familiarity and semantic congruency modulate responses in cortical audiovisual integration areas.</article-title>
<source>Journal of Neuroscience</source>
<volume>27</volume>
<fpage>7881</fpage>
<lpage>7887</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17652579</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Naumer1">
<label>10</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Naumer</surname>
<given-names>MJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Doehrmann</surname>
<given-names>O</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Muller</surname>
<given-names>NG</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Muckli</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kaiser</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
<year>2009</year>
<article-title>Cortical Plasticity of Audio-Visual Object Representations.</article-title>
<source>Cerebral Cortex</source>
<volume>19</volume>
<fpage>1641</fpage>
<lpage>1653</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19015373</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Tanabe1">
<label>11</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tanabe</surname>
<given-names>HC</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Honda</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sadato</surname>
<given-names>N</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>2005</year>
<article-title>Functionally segregated neural substrates for arbitrary audiovisual paired-association learning.</article-title>
<source>Journal of Neuroscience</source>
<volume>25</volume>
<fpage>6409</fpage>
<lpage>6418</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16000632</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Xiang1">
<label>12</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Xiang</surname>
<given-names>JZ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brown</surname>
<given-names>MW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>1998</year>
<article-title>Differential neuronal encoding of novelty, familiarity and recency in regions of the anterior temporal lobe.</article-title>
<source>Neuropharmacology</source>
<volume>37</volume>
<fpage>657</fpage>
<lpage>676</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">9705004</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Horn1">
<label>13</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Horn</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nicol</surname>
<given-names>AU</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brown</surname>
<given-names>MW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>2001</year>
<article-title>Tracking memory's trace.</article-title>
<source>Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America</source>
<volume>98</volume>
<fpage>5282</fpage>
<lpage>5287</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11296266</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Nakamori1">
<label>14</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Nakamori</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sato</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Atoji</surname>
<given-names>Y</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kanamatsu</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tanaka</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
<year>2010</year>
<article-title>Demonstration of a Neural Circuit Critical for Imprinting Behavior in Chicks.</article-title>
<source>Journal of Neuroscience</source>
<volume>30</volume>
<fpage>4467</fpage>
<lpage>4480</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20335483</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Sluckin1">
<label>15</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sluckin</surname>
<given-names>W</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>1972</year>
<article-title>Imprinting and early learning.</article-title>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Butcher</surname>
<given-names>HJ</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Methuen & Co Ltd</publisher-name>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-McCabe1">
<label>16</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>McCabe</surname>
<given-names>BJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Horn</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bateson</surname>
<given-names>PPG</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>1981</year>
<article-title>Effects of restricted lesions of the chick forebrain on the acquisition of filial preferences during imprinting.</article-title>
<source>Brain Research</source>
<volume>205</volume>
<fpage>29</fpage>
<lpage>37</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">7470865</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Horn2">
<label>17</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Horn</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>2004</year>
<article-title>Pathways of the past: The imprint of memory.</article-title>
<source>Nature Reviews Neuroscience</source>
<volume>5</volume>
<fpage>108</fpage>
<lpage>U113</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Brown1">
<label>18</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Brown</surname>
<given-names>MW</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Horn</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>1994</year>
<article-title>Learning-related alterations in the visual responsiveness of neurons in a memory system of the chick brain.</article-title>
<source>European Journal of Neuroscience</source>
<volume>6</volume>
<fpage>1479</fpage>
<lpage>1490</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8000571</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Nicol1">
<label>19</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Nicol</surname>
<given-names>AU</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brown</surname>
<given-names>MW</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Horn</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>1995</year>
<article-title>Neurophysiological investigation of a recognition memory system for imprinting in the domestic chick.</article-title>
<source>European Journal of Neuroscience</source>
<volume>7</volume>
<fpage>766</fpage>
<lpage>776</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">7620625</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Jackson1">
<label>20</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Jackson</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McCabe</surname>
<given-names>BJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nicol</surname>
<given-names>AU</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Grout</surname>
<given-names>AS</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brown</surname>
<given-names>MW</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
<year>2008</year>
<article-title>Dynamics effects of of a memory trace: Sleep on consolidation.</article-title>
<source>Current Biology</source>
<volume>18</volume>
<fpage>393</fpage>
<lpage>400</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18356057</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Gray1">
<label>21</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gray</surname>
<given-names>CM</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Maldonado</surname>
<given-names>PE</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wilson</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McNaughton</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>1995</year>
<article-title>Tetrodes markedly improve the reliability and yield of multiple single-unit isolation from multi-unit recordings in cat striate cortex.</article-title>
<source>Journal of Neuroscience Methods</source>
<volume>63</volume>
<fpage>43</fpage>
<lpage>54</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8788047</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Meredith1">
<label>22</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Meredith</surname>
<given-names>MA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Stein</surname>
<given-names>BE</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>1986</year>
<article-title>Visual, auditory and somatosensory convergence on cells in Superior Colliculus.</article-title>
<source>Journal of Neurophysiology</source>
<volume>56</volume>
<fpage>640</fpage>
<lpage>662</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">3537225</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Zahar1">
<label>23</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Zahar</surname>
<given-names>Y</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Reches</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gutfreund</surname>
<given-names>Y</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>2009</year>
<article-title>Multisensory Enhancement in the Optic Tectum of the Barn Owl: Spike Count and Spike Timing.</article-title>
<source>Journal of Neurophysiology</source>
<volume>101</volume>
<fpage>2380</fpage>
<lpage>2394</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19261710</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Kayser1">
<label>24</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kayser</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Logothetis</surname>
<given-names>NK</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Panzeri</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>2010</year>
<article-title>Visual Enhancement of the Information Representation in Auditory Cortex.</article-title>
<source>Current Biology</source>
<volume>20</volume>
<fpage>19</fpage>
<lpage>24</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20036538</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Bradley1">
<label>25</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bradley</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Davies</surname>
<given-names>DC</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Horn</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>1985</year>
<article-title>Connections of the hyperstriatum ventrale of the domestic chick (Gallus domesticus).</article-title>
<source>Journal of Anatomy</source>
<volume>140</volume>
<fpage>577</fpage>
<lpage>589</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">4077698</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Taylor1">
<label>26</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Taylor</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sluckin</surname>
<given-names>W</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hewitt</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Guiton</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>1967</year>
<article-title>FORMATION OF ATTACHMENTS BY DOMESTIC CHICKS TO 2 TEXTURES.</article-title>
<source>Animal Behaviour</source>
<volume>15</volume>
<fpage>514</fpage>
<lpage>&</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">6055105</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-McCabe2">
<label>27</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>McCabe</surname>
<given-names>BJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Horn</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kendrick</surname>
<given-names>KM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>2001</year>
<article-title>GABA, taurine and learning: Release of amino acids from slices of chick brain following filial imprinting.</article-title>
<source>Neuroscience</source>
<volume>105</volume>
<fpage>317</fpage>
<lpage>324</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11672599</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-DiCarlo1">
<label>28</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>DiCarlo</surname>
<given-names>JJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lane</surname>
<given-names>JW</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hsiao</surname>
<given-names>SS</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Johnson</surname>
<given-names>KO</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>1996</year>
<article-title>Marking microelectrode penetrations with fluorescent dyes.</article-title>
<source>Journal of Neuroscience Methods</source>
<volume>64</volume>
<fpage>75</fpage>
<lpage>81</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8869487</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Davies1">
<label>29</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Davies</surname>
<given-names>DC</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Horn</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McCabe</surname>
<given-names>BJ</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>1983</year>
<article-title>Changes in telencephalic catecholamine levels in the domestic chick. Effects of age and visual experience.</article-title>
<source>Brain Res</source>
<volume>312</volume>
<fpage>251</fpage>
<lpage>255</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">6652517</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-CipollaNeto1">
<label>30</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cipolla-Neto</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Horn</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McCabe</surname>
<given-names>BJ</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>1979</year>
<article-title>Method for recording unit-activity from the brain of the freely moving chick.</article-title>
<source>Journal of Physiology - London</source>
<volume>295</volume>
<fpage>8</fpage>
<lpage>9</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-VanKampen1">
<label>31</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Van Kampen</surname>
<given-names>HS</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bolhuis</surname>
<given-names>JJ</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>1991</year>
<article-title>Auditory learning and filial imprinting in the chick.</article-title>
<source>Behaviour</source>
<volume>117</volume>
<fpage>303</fpage>
<lpage>319</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Lewicki1">
<label>32</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lewicki</surname>
<given-names>MS</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>1998</year>
<article-title>A review of methods for spike sorting: the detection and classification of neural action potentials.</article-title>
<source>Network-Computation in Neural Systems</source>
<volume>9</volume>
<fpage>R53</fpage>
<lpage>R78</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0017777-Stanford1">
<label>33</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Stanford</surname>
<given-names>TR</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Quessy</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Stein</surname>
<given-names>BE</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<year>2005</year>
<article-title>Evaluating the operations underlying multisensory integration in the cat superior colliculus.</article-title>
<source>Journal of Neuroscience</source>
<volume>25</volume>
<fpage>6499</fpage>
<lpage>6508</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16014711</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</pmc>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Ticri/CIDE/explor/HapticV1/Data/Pmc/Curation
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 002401 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Curation/biblio.hfd -nk 002401 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Ticri/CIDE
   |area=    HapticV1
   |flux=    Pmc
   |étape=   Curation
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     PMC:3053393
   |texte=   Neuronal Plasticity and Multisensory Integration in Filial Imprinting
}}

Pour générer des pages wiki

HfdIndexSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Curation/RBID.i   -Sk "pubmed:21423770" \
       | HfdSelect -Kh $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Curation/biblio.hfd   \
       | NlmPubMed2Wicri -a HapticV1 

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.23.
Data generation: Mon Jun 13 01:09:46 2016. Site generation: Wed Mar 6 09:54:07 2024