Serveur d'exploration sur les dispositifs haptiques

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

Gender and the Communication of Emotion Via Touch

Identifieur interne : 000880 ( Pmc/Curation ); précédent : 000879; suivant : 000881

Gender and the Communication of Emotion Via Touch

Auteurs : Matthew J. Hertenstein [États-Unis] ; Dacher Keltner [États-Unis]

Source :

RBID : PMC:3016097

Abstract

We reanalyzed a data set consisting of a U.S. undergraduate sample (N = 212) from a previous study (Hertenstein et al. 2006a) that showed that touch communicates distinct emotions between humans. In the current reanalysis, we found that anger was communicated at greater-than-chance levels only when a male comprised at least one member of a communicating dyad. Sympathy was communicated at greater-than-chance levels only when a female comprised at least one member of the dyad. Finally, happiness was communicated only if females comprised the entire dyad. The current analysis demonstrates gender asymmetries in the accuracy of communicating distinct emotions via touch between humans.


Url:
DOI: 10.1007/s11199-010-9842-y
PubMed: 21297854
PubMed Central: 3016097

Links toward previous steps (curation, corpus...)


Links to Exploration step

PMC:3016097

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI>
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">Gender and the Communication of Emotion Via Touch</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hertenstein, Matthew J" sort="Hertenstein, Matthew J" uniqKey="Hertenstein M" first="Matthew J." last="Hertenstein">Matthew J. Hertenstein</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="2">
<nlm:aff id="Aff1">Department of Psychology, DePauw University, 7 Larabee St., Harrison Hall, Greencastle, IN 46135 USA</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">États-Unis</country>
<placeName>
<region type="state">Indiana</region>
</placeName>
<wicri:cityArea>Department of Psychology, DePauw University, 7 Larabee St., Harrison Hall, Greencastle</wicri:cityArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Keltner, Dacher" sort="Keltner, Dacher" uniqKey="Keltner D" first="Dacher" last="Keltner">Dacher Keltner</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="2">
<nlm:aff id="Aff2">University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA USA</nlm:aff>
<country>États-Unis</country>
<placeName>
<region type="state">Californie</region>
</placeName>
<wicri:cityArea>University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley</wicri:cityArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">PMC</idno>
<idno type="pmid">21297854</idno>
<idno type="pmc">3016097</idno>
<idno type="url">http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3016097</idno>
<idno type="RBID">PMC:3016097</idno>
<idno type="doi">10.1007/s11199-010-9842-y</idno>
<date when="2010">2010</date>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Corpus">000880</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Curation">000880</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title xml:lang="en" level="a" type="main">Gender and the Communication of Emotion Via Touch</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hertenstein, Matthew J" sort="Hertenstein, Matthew J" uniqKey="Hertenstein M" first="Matthew J." last="Hertenstein">Matthew J. Hertenstein</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="2">
<nlm:aff id="Aff1">Department of Psychology, DePauw University, 7 Larabee St., Harrison Hall, Greencastle, IN 46135 USA</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">États-Unis</country>
<placeName>
<region type="state">Indiana</region>
</placeName>
<wicri:cityArea>Department of Psychology, DePauw University, 7 Larabee St., Harrison Hall, Greencastle</wicri:cityArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Keltner, Dacher" sort="Keltner, Dacher" uniqKey="Keltner D" first="Dacher" last="Keltner">Dacher Keltner</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="2">
<nlm:aff id="Aff2">University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA USA</nlm:aff>
<country>États-Unis</country>
<placeName>
<region type="state">Californie</region>
</placeName>
<wicri:cityArea>University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley</wicri:cityArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<series>
<title level="j">Sex Roles</title>
<idno type="ISSN">0360-0025</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1573-2762</idno>
<imprint>
<date when="2010">2010</date>
</imprint>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">
<p>We reanalyzed a data set consisting of a U.S. undergraduate sample (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 212) from a previous study (Hertenstein et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR42">2006a</xref>
) that showed that touch communicates distinct emotions between humans. In the current reanalysis, we found that anger was communicated at greater-than-chance levels only when a male comprised at least one member of a communicating dyad. Sympathy was communicated at greater-than-chance levels only when a female comprised at least one member of the dyad. Finally, happiness was communicated only if females comprised the entire dyad. The current analysis demonstrates gender asymmetries in the accuracy of communicating distinct emotions via touch between humans.</p>
</div>
</front>
<back>
<div1 type="bibliography">
<listBibl>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Argyle, M" uniqKey="Argyle M">M Argyle</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bailenson, Jn" uniqKey="Bailenson J">JN Bailenson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yee, N" uniqKey="Yee N">N Yee</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brave, S" uniqKey="Brave S">S Brave</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Merget, D" uniqKey="Merget D">D Merget</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Koslow, D" uniqKey="Koslow D">D Koslow</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Banse, R" uniqKey="Banse R">R Banse</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Scherer, Kr" uniqKey="Scherer K">KR Scherer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Becker, Dv" uniqKey="Becker D">DV Becker</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kenrick, Dt" uniqKey="Kenrick D">DT Kenrick</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Neuberg, Sl" uniqKey="Neuberg S">SL Neuberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Blackwell, Kc" uniqKey="Blackwell K">KC Blackwell</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, Dm" uniqKey="Smith D">DM Smith</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Berkowitz, L" uniqKey="Berkowitz L">L Berkowitz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Biernat, M" uniqKey="Biernat M">M Biernat</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Birnbaum, Dw" uniqKey="Birnbaum D">DW Birnbaum</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brody, Lr" uniqKey="Brody L">LR Brody</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brody, Lr" uniqKey="Brody L">LR Brody</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hall, Ja" uniqKey="Hall J">JA Hall</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brody, Lr" uniqKey="Brody L">LR Brody</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hall, Ja" uniqKey="Hall J">JA Hall</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Citrin, Lb" uniqKey="Citrin L">LB Citrin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Roberts, T" uniqKey="Roberts T">T Roberts</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fredrickson, Bl" uniqKey="Fredrickson B">BL Fredrickson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Clark, C" uniqKey="Clark C">C Clark</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Clark, Rd" uniqKey="Clark R">RD Clark</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shields, G" uniqKey="Shields G">G Shields</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Clynes, M" uniqKey="Clynes M">M Clynes</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nettheim, N" uniqKey="Nettheim N">N Nettheim</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Daly, M" uniqKey="Daly M">M Daly</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wilson, M" uniqKey="Wilson M">M Wilson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Day, Al" uniqKey="Day A">AL Day</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Carroll, Sa" uniqKey="Carroll S">SA Carroll</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Eisenberg, N" uniqKey="Eisenberg N">N Eisenberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lennon, R" uniqKey="Lennon R">R Lennon</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Eisenberg, N" uniqKey="Eisenberg N">N Eisenberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fabes, Ra" uniqKey="Fabes R">RA Fabes</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Miller, Pa" uniqKey="Miller P">PA Miller</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fultz, J" uniqKey="Fultz J">J Fultz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shell, R" uniqKey="Shell R">R Shell</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mathy, Rm" uniqKey="Mathy R">RM Mathy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ekman, P" uniqKey="Ekman P">P Ekman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ekman, P" uniqKey="Ekman P">P Ekman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ekman, P" uniqKey="Ekman P">P Ekman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Friesen, Wv" uniqKey="Friesen W">WV Friesen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Elfenbein, Ha" uniqKey="Elfenbein H">HA Elfenbein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ambady, N" uniqKey="Ambady N">N Ambady</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fischer, Ah" uniqKey="Fischer A">AH Fischer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fivush, R" uniqKey="Fivush R">R Fivush</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Reese, E" uniqKey="Reese E">E Reese</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Haden, Ca" uniqKey="Haden C">CA Haden</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Frank, Mg" uniqKey="Frank M">MG Frank</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stennett, J" uniqKey="Stennett J">J Stennett</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gilligan, C" uniqKey="Gilligan C">C Gilligan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Goetz, Jl" uniqKey="Goetz J">JL Goetz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Keltner, D" uniqKey="Keltner D">D Keltner</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gohm, Cl" uniqKey="Gohm C">CL Gohm</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Clore, Gl" uniqKey="Clore G">GL Clore</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gross, Jj" uniqKey="Gross J">JJ Gross</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="John, Op" uniqKey="John O">OP John</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gross, Jj" uniqKey="Gross J">JJ Gross</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fredrickson, Bl" uniqKey="Fredrickson B">BL Fredrickson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Levenson, Rw" uniqKey="Levenson R">RW Levenson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Halberstadt, Ag" uniqKey="Halberstadt A">AG Halberstadt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hayes, Cw" uniqKey="Hayes C">CW Hayes</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pike, K" uniqKey="Pike K">K Pike</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hall, Ja" uniqKey="Hall J">JA Hall</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hall, Ja" uniqKey="Hall J">JA Hall</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Carter, Jd" uniqKey="Carter J">JD Carter</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Horgan, Tg" uniqKey="Horgan T">TG Horgan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hall, Ja" uniqKey="Hall J">JA Hall</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Carney, Dr" uniqKey="Carney D">DR Carney</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Murphy, Na" uniqKey="Murphy N">NA Murphy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hall, Ja" uniqKey="Hall J">JA Hall</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Coats, Ej" uniqKey="Coats E">EJ Coats</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith Lebeau, L" uniqKey="Smith Lebeau L">L Smith LeBeau</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Henley, Nm" uniqKey="Henley N">NM Henley</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Henley, N" uniqKey="Henley N">N Henley</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Henley, Nm" uniqKey="Henley N">NM Henley</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hertenstein, Mj" uniqKey="Hertenstein M">MJ Hertenstein</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hertenstein, Mj" uniqKey="Hertenstein M">MJ Hertenstein</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hertenstein, Mj" uniqKey="Hertenstein M">MJ Hertenstein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Campos, Jj" uniqKey="Campos J">JJ Campos</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hertenstein, Mj" uniqKey="Hertenstein M">MJ Hertenstein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Keltner, D" uniqKey="Keltner D">D Keltner</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="App, B" uniqKey="App B">B App</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bulleit, Ba" uniqKey="Bulleit B">BA Bulleit</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jaskolka, Ar" uniqKey="Jaskolka A">AR Jaskolka</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hertenstein, Mj" uniqKey="Hertenstein M">MJ Hertenstein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Verkamp, Jm" uniqKey="Verkamp J">JM Verkamp</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kerestes, Am" uniqKey="Kerestes A">AM Kerestes</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Holmes, Rm" uniqKey="Holmes R">RM Holmes</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hertenstein, Mj" uniqKey="Hertenstein M">MJ Hertenstein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Holmes, R" uniqKey="Holmes R">R Holmes</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mccullough, M" uniqKey="Mccullough M">M McCullough</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Keltner, D" uniqKey="Keltner D">D Keltner</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hess, U" uniqKey="Hess U">U Hess</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Senecal, S" uniqKey="Senecal S">S Senécal</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kirouac, G" uniqKey="Kirouac G">G Kirouac</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Herrera, P" uniqKey="Herrera P">P Herrera</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Philippot, P" uniqKey="Philippot P">P Philippot</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kleck, Re" uniqKey="Kleck R">RE Kleck</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hess, U" uniqKey="Hess U">U Hess</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Adams, Rb" uniqKey="Adams R">RB Adams</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kleck, Re" uniqKey="Kleck R">RE Kleck</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hess, U" uniqKey="Hess U">U Hess</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Adams, R" uniqKey="Adams R">R Adams</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kleck, R" uniqKey="Kleck R">R Kleck</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct></biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jones, Se" uniqKey="Jones S">SE Jones</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yarbrough, Ae" uniqKey="Yarbrough A">AE Yarbrough</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kagan, J" uniqKey="Kagan J">J Kagan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kenrick, D" uniqKey="Kenrick D">D Kenrick</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Trost, M" uniqKey="Trost M">M Trost</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sundie, J" uniqKey="Sundie J">J Sundie</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kirouac, G" uniqKey="Kirouac G">G Kirouac</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hess, U" uniqKey="Hess U">U Hess</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Knapp, Ml" uniqKey="Knapp M">ML Knapp</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hall, Ja" uniqKey="Hall J">JA Hall</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kring, Am" uniqKey="Kring A">AM Kring</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kring, Am" uniqKey="Kring A">AM Kring</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gordon, Ah" uniqKey="Gordon A">AH Gordon</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lafrance, M" uniqKey="Lafrance M">M LaFrance</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Henley, Nm" uniqKey="Henley N">NM Henley</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lafrance, M" uniqKey="Lafrance M">M LaFrance</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hecht, Ma" uniqKey="Hecht M">MA Hecht</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lafrance, M" uniqKey="Lafrance M">M LaFrance</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hecht, Ma" uniqKey="Hecht M">MA Hecht</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Paluck, El" uniqKey="Paluck E">EL Paluck</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mayer, Jd" uniqKey="Mayer J">JD Mayer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Caruso, Dr" uniqKey="Caruso D">DR Caruso</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Salovey, P" uniqKey="Salovey P">P Salovey</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Plant, Ea" uniqKey="Plant E">EA Plant</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hyde, Js" uniqKey="Hyde J">JS Hyde</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Keltner, D" uniqKey="Keltner D">D Keltner</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Devine, Pg" uniqKey="Devine P">PG Devine</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rime, B" uniqKey="Rime B">B Rimé</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Corsini, S" uniqKey="Corsini S">S Corsini</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Herbette, G" uniqKey="Herbette G">G Herbette</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Robinson, Md" uniqKey="Robinson M">MD Robinson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnson, Jt" uniqKey="Johnson J">JT Johnson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Roter, Dl" uniqKey="Roter D">DL Roter</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hall, Ja" uniqKey="Hall J">JA Hall</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aoki, Y" uniqKey="Aoki Y">Y Aoki</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Scherer, Kr" uniqKey="Scherer K">KR Scherer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnstone, T" uniqKey="Johnstone T">T Johnstone</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Klasmeyer, G" uniqKey="Klasmeyer G">G Klasmeyer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shields, S" uniqKey="Shields S">S Shields</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shiota, M" uniqKey="Shiota M">M Shiota</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Keltner, D" uniqKey="Keltner D">D Keltner</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="John, O" uniqKey="John O">O John</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Simon, Rw" uniqKey="Simon R">RW Simon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nath, Le" uniqKey="Nath L">LE Nath</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, J" uniqKey="Smith J">J Smith</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Maclean, K" uniqKey="Maclean K">K MacLean</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stier, Ds" uniqKey="Stier D">DS Stier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hall, Ja" uniqKey="Hall J">JA Hall</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tiedens, Lz" uniqKey="Tiedens L">LZ Tiedens</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leach, Cw" uniqKey="Leach C">CW Leach</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Timmers, M" uniqKey="Timmers M">M Timmers</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fischer, Ah" uniqKey="Fischer A">AH Fischer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Manstead, Asr" uniqKey="Manstead A">ASR Manstead</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tracy, Jl" uniqKey="Tracy J">JL Tracy</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Robins, Rw" uniqKey="Robins R">RW Robins</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schriber, Ra" uniqKey="Schriber R">RA Schriber</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weiss, Sj" uniqKey="Weiss S">SJ Weiss</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wood, W" uniqKey="Wood W">W Wood</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Eagly, Ah" uniqKey="Eagly A">AH Eagly</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
</listBibl>
</div1>
</back>
</TEI>
<pmc article-type="research-article">
<pmc-dir>properties open_access</pmc-dir>
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="nlm-ta">Sex Roles</journal-id>
<journal-title-group>
<journal-title>Sex Roles</journal-title>
</journal-title-group>
<issn pub-type="ppub">0360-0025</issn>
<issn pub-type="epub">1573-2762</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Springer US</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>Boston</publisher-loc>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmid">21297854</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmc">3016097</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="publisher-id">9842</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s11199-010-9842-y</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Original Article</subject>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>Gender and the Communication of Emotion Via Touch</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author" corresp="yes">
<name>
<surname>Hertenstein</surname>
<given-names>Matthew J.</given-names>
</name>
<address>
<phone>+1-765-6584609</phone>
<fax>+1-765-6584572</fax>
<email>mhertenstein@depauw.edu</email>
</address>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="Aff1">1</xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Keltner</surname>
<given-names>Dacher</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="Aff2">2</xref>
</contrib>
<aff id="Aff1">
<label>1</label>
Department of Psychology, DePauw University, 7 Larabee St., Harrison Hall, Greencastle, IN 46135 USA</aff>
<aff id="Aff2">
<label>2</label>
University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA USA</aff>
</contrib-group>
<pub-date pub-type="epub">
<day>9</day>
<month>10</month>
<year>2010</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="pmc-release">
<day>9</day>
<month>10</month>
<year>2010</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="ppub">
<month>1</month>
<year>2011</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>64</volume>
<issue>1-2</issue>
<fpage>70</fpage>
<lpage>80</lpage>
<permissions>
<copyright-statement>© The Author(s) 2010</copyright-statement>
</permissions>
<abstract>
<p>We reanalyzed a data set consisting of a U.S. undergraduate sample (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 212) from a previous study (Hertenstein et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR42">2006a</xref>
) that showed that touch communicates distinct emotions between humans. In the current reanalysis, we found that anger was communicated at greater-than-chance levels only when a male comprised at least one member of a communicating dyad. Sympathy was communicated at greater-than-chance levels only when a female comprised at least one member of the dyad. Finally, happiness was communicated only if females comprised the entire dyad. The current analysis demonstrates gender asymmetries in the accuracy of communicating distinct emotions via touch between humans.</p>
</abstract>
<kwd-group>
<title>Keywords</title>
<kwd>Touch</kwd>
<kwd>Emotional communication</kwd>
<kwd>Tactile</kwd>
<kwd>Contact</kwd>
<kwd>Gender differences</kwd>
</kwd-group>
<custom-meta-group>
<custom-meta>
<meta-name>issue-copyright-statement</meta-name>
<meta-value>© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011</meta-value>
</custom-meta>
</custom-meta-group>
</article-meta>
</front>
<body>
<sec id="Sec1">
<title>Introduction</title>
<p>Touch is a rich medium of social exchange and through it, individuals form strong attachments and cooperative alliances, they negotiate status differences, they soothe and calm, and they express sexual and romantic interest (Hertenstein et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR42">2006a</xref>
). Given the centrality of touch to social life, it is likely to be a highly gendered form of human communication.</p>
<p>There is a longstanding interest in exploring the magnitude and sources of gender differences in the expression of emotion. The predominant focus in this work has been on the face and voice (LaFrance, et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR58">2003</xref>
; Scherer et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR64">2003</xref>
). In the present investigation, we examine gender differences in the communication of distinct emotions via touch in humans. We do so in the context of evolutionary and constructionist theories of gender, build upon previous work published in
<italic>Sex Roles</italic>
, and rely upon well-tested methodology in the field of emotion. Although the sample is limited to one in the U.S., the work has implications and raises questions for the communication of emotion via touch in other cultures. In relation to this point, Table 
<xref rid="Tab1" ref-type="table">1</xref>
includes sample characteristics for the empirical articles cited.
<table-wrap id="Tab1">
<label>Table 1</label>
<caption>
<p>Cited empirical studies with demographic of sample and location from which sample was drawn</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th>Author (year)</th>
<th>Demographic of sample</th>
<th>Location</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td>Bailenson et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR2">2007</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Banse and Scherer (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR3">1996</xref>
)</td>
<td>Actors and undergraduates</td>
<td>Germany</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Becker et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR4">2007</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Birnbaum (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR7">1984</xref>
)</td>
<td>Parents, undergraduates, preschoolers</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Clark and Shields (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR13">1997</xref>
)</td>
<td>High School: 14–19 yrs</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Clynes and Nettheim (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR14">1982</xref>
)</td>
<td>University students and staff</td>
<td>Australia</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Day and Carroll (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR16">2004</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>Canada</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Eisenberg et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR18">1989</xref>
)</td>
<td>Children (2nd and 5th grade) and undergraduates</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Frank and Stennett (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR25">2001</xref>
)</td>
<td>Wide age range across adulthood</td>
<td>Australia and U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Gohm and Clore (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR28">2000</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Gross et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR30">1994</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Gross and John (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR29">1998</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Halberstadt et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR31">1988</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Henley (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR36">1973</xref>
)</td>
<td>Broad age range</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Hertenstein and Campos (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR41">2001</xref>
)</td>
<td>Infants</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Hertenstein et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR44">2009</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Hertenstein et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR42">2006a</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>U.S. & Spain</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Hess et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR46">2004</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>U.S. and Canada</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Hess et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR45">2000</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>Canada</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Jones and Yarbrough (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR49">1985</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Kring and Gordon (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR55">1998</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Mayer et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR59">2000</xref>
)</td>
<td>Broad age range</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Plant et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR60">2000</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Robinson and Johnson (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR62">1997</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Shiota et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR66">2006</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Simon and Nath (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR67">2004</xref>
)</td>
<td>Broad age range</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Smith, and MacLean (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR68">2007</xref>
)</td>
<td>University students</td>
<td>Canada</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Timmers et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR71">2003</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>Netherlands</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Tracy et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR72">2009</xref>
)</td>
<td>Undergraduates</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Weiss (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR73">1992</xref>
)</td>
<td>Graduate students</td>
<td>U.S.</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</table-wrap>
</p>
<sec id="Sec2">
<title>Touch and Emotion in Human Communication</title>
<p>Some research indicates that touch communicates the hedonic tone of emotion predominantly, that is overall warmth or distress (Hertenstein and Campos
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR41">2001</xref>
; Jones and Yarbrough
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR49">1985</xref>
; Knapp and Hall
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR53">1997</xref>
), or that touch intensifies the meaning of emotional displays in other modalities (Knapp and Hall
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR53">1997</xref>
). Recent studies by Hertenstein and colleagues have documented, however, that touch communicates several distinct emotions between humans (Hertenstein et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR42">2006a</xref>
, see Clynes and Nettheim (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR14">1982</xref>
) for the association of distinct states by people pressing a pressure-sensitive button). In this research, two strangers were placed in a room in which they were separated by a barrier. They could not see one another, but they could reach each other through an aperture in a curtain. One person touched the other on the forearm, in each instance trying to convey one of 12 emotions. After each touch, the person touched had to choose which emotion s/he thought the encoder was communicating by selecting a term in a modified forced-choice format (Frank and Stennett
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR25">2001</xref>
).</p>
<p>In two different undergraduate samples from Spain and the U.S., participants accurately decoded anger, fear, disgust, love, gratitude, and sympathy at above-chance levels (Hertenstein et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR42">2006a</xref>
). Participants did not decode happiness, surprise, sadness, embarrassment, envy, and pride at above chance levels. Accuracy rates ranged from 48% to 83% for the accurately decoded emotions, which is comparable to those observed in studies of facial displays and vocal communication with samples around the world (Elfenbein and Ambady
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR22">2002</xref>
; Scherer et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR64">2003</xref>
). Specific tactile behaviors demonstrated by the U.S. sample were associated with each of the emotions. For example, sympathy was associated with stroking and patting, anger with hitting and squeezing, disgust with a pushing motion, gratitude shaking of the hand, fear with trembling, and love with stroking.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec3">
<title>Sympathy, Anger, Happiness and the Gendered Nature of Touch</title>
<p>The study of gender and emotion is one of the richest traditions in the field of emotion (for reviews, see Brody and Hall
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR10">2008</xref>
; Citrin et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR11">2004</xref>
; LaFrance et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR58">2003</xref>
). Researchers interested in gender have focused on stereotypes (e.g., Hess et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR45">2000</xref>
; Robinson and Johnson
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR62">1997</xref>
; Timmers et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR71">2003</xref>
), self-reported experience (e.g., Gross and John
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR29">1998</xref>
; Hess et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR45">2000</xref>
; Simon and Nath
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR67">2004</xref>
), verbalization of emotion (e.g., Roter et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR63">2002</xref>
), emotional expression (e.g., Halberstadt et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR31">1988</xref>
; Kring and Gordon
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR55">1998</xref>
), physiology (e.g., Kring & Gordon), nonverbal decoding of emotions (e.g., Hall
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR32">1990</xref>
), and constructs such as emotional intelligence (e.g. Day and Carroll
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR16">2004</xref>
; Mayer et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR59">2000</xref>
) and emotional competence (e.g., Gohm and Clore
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR28">2000</xref>
). The empirical areas of inquiry within this tradition have established several regularities: women are stereotyped as being more emotional (e.g., Plant et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR60">2000</xref>
); females smile more (LaFrance et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR58">2003</xref>
) and cry more often (Gross et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR30">1994</xref>
); women are more expressive of emotion in general and better at decoding emotion (Brody and Hall
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR10">2008</xref>
). The present research contributes to this literature by exploring an understudied modality of emotion-communication between humans—touch.</p>
<p>With respect to touch in human communication, scattered studies have yielded certain regularities. Males appear to initiate touch more than females (Henley
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR36">1973</xref>
; although see Stier and Hall
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR69">1984</xref>
). Studies also find gender differences in the perceived valence of a touch (Hall et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR35">2005</xref>
; Hertenstein et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR43">2006</xref>
). In this research, women are more likely than men to perceive touch from opposite-gender strangers as unpleasant and an invasion of privacy. Moreover, the more women perceive a touch as sexual from a male stranger, the less they perceive the touch as warm and friendly; whereas, the more men perceive a touch as sexual from a female stranger, the more they perceive it as warm, pleasant, and friendly (for a review, see Hertenstein et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR43">2006b</xref>
).</p>
<p>In the present study, we tested predictions regarding gender differences in the accuracy with which individuals can communicate distinct emotions through touch in human communication, relying on previously published data (Hertenstein et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR42">2006a</xref>
). This study included the requisite four different dyad groups (encoder-decoder): female–female, female–male, male–male, and male–female. Overall accuracy across 12 emotions did not vary by gender, as reported in the original article. However, gender differences were not analyzed for each emotion separately. These aggregate analyses limited the inferences that could be made regarding gender differences in the communication of emotion via touch. In the present study, we analyzed each of the emotions separately focusing on two emotions that evolutionary and social role accounts both suggest should vary by gender: sympathy and anger. We also focused on happiness, an emotion that has shown consistent gender differences (e.g., LaFrance et al
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR58">2003</xref>
).</p>
<p>Evolutionary and social role accounts suggest potential and consistent gender differences in the communication of sympathy and anger via touch between humans. Sympathy is a care-taking emotion that supports other-oriented, altruistic behavior (Eisenberg et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR18">1989</xref>
; Goetz, and Keltner
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR27">2007</xref>
). Within evolutionary accounts, it is assumed that women disproportionately take on the care-taking demands of raising offspring. Within social role accounts, it is well documented that central socialization practices—parental discourse, child rearing manuals, cultural stereotypes—amplify the place of sympathy in women’s psyches (e.g., Clark
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR12">1997</xref>
; Clark and Shields
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR13">1997</xref>
). Both accounts suggest that women should be more likely to experience and express sympathy. Consistent with this analysis, females report experiencing more sympathy than do men (Brody and Hall
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR9">2000</xref>
; Shiota et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR66">2006</xref>
). In the present study we predicted that when females are in an experimental dyad, sympathy will be decoded at above-chance levels.</p>
<p>Anger, in contrast, promotes aggression (Berkowitz
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR5">1993</xref>
). Given that anger produces assertive, competitive behavior in face-to-face interactions, anger is intertwined with status contests and affordances (e.g., Tiedens and Leach
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR70">2004</xref>
). Evolutionary accounts (e.g., Daly and Wilson
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR15">1994</xref>
; Kenrick et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR51">2004</xref>
) contend that men more readily enter into confrontational encounters to rise in hierarchies and gain preferential access to mates, and should more readily experience and express anger. Guided by this theorizing, one study recently found that participants could more accurately and more quickly detect male than female angry facial displays (Becker et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR4">2007</xref>
). Social role accounts likewise assume that anger is a gendered emotion, one more fitting with the stereotypical roles granted to men, revolving around self-assertion, competition, and status (Kring
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR54">2000</xref>
). Guided by such theorizing, it has been found, for example, that while mothers talk more about most emotions to their young daughters than their young sons—to socialize them in the ways of care-taking—they talk more with their boys about anger (Fivush et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR24">2006</xref>
). And in adults, it has been found that men consistently report experiencing and expressing more anger than women (Brody and Hall
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR9">2000</xref>
; Kring
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR54">2000</xref>
; Plant et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR60">2000</xref>
). We therefore predicted that dyads involving males would communicate anger with touch at above-chance levels.</p>
<p>Finally, one of the most consistent gender differences identified in the literature on emotion relates to the stereotypes, experience, and expression of happiness (Hess et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR46">2004</xref>
). Women are assumed to experience and express happiness more than men in a variety of contexts (Brody and Hall
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR10">2008</xref>
; Fischer
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR23">1993</xref>
; Hall et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR33">2000</xref>
). Stereotypes such as this have been documented as early as toddlerhood and are thought to arise from socialization practices originating from television, parental stereotypes, differential reinforcement of emotional expression, and actual observations of emotionality (Birnbaum
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR7">1984</xref>
).</p>
<p>Researchers have documented empirical support for such stereotypes (Brody and Hall
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR9">2000</xref>
); women report experiencing more happiness than men (Brody
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR8">1993</xref>
) and they smile more than men (Hall et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR34">2002</xref>
; LaFrance et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR58">2003</xref>
). A number of explanations have been proposed to explain these gender differences, some of which emphasize power and status (Henley
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR37">1977</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR38">1995</xref>
; LaFrance and Henley
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR56">1994</xref>
), some of which emphasize the social roles of the genders (Brody and Hall
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR9">2000</xref>
; Shields
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR65">2000</xref>
), and some that combine these two explanations (LaFrance and Hecht
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR57">1999</xref>
; LaFrance et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR58">2003</xref>
). Predicated upon theory and the empirical work demonstrating that women experience and express more positive emotionality than men, we predicted that dyads comprised solely of females would communicate happiness with touch at above-chance levels.</p>
<p>In summary, based on theoretical and empirical evidence reviewed above, we made predictions regarding three different emotions: sympathy, anger, and happiness. More specifically, we predicted that when females are in an experimental dyad, sympathy will be decoded at above-chance levels. We also predicted that dyads involving males would accurately communicate anger with touch. Finally, we predicted that dyads comprised solely of females would accurately communicate happiness with touch.</p>
<p>Although the primary goal of the current study was to address the decoding of emotion via touch, a subsidiary purpose was to provide tactile signals that were used to communicate sympathy, anger, and happiness between humans. The field of emotion has been advanced by precise characterizations of emotion-specific signals in the face (Ekman
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR20">1993</xref>
) and voice (Scherer, et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR64">2003</xref>
). The two most common coding systems for the face include Izard’s (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR48">1979</xref>
) maximally discriminative facial movement coding system (MAX) and Ekman and Friesen’s (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR21">1978</xref>
) Facial Action Coding System (FACS). These systems are anatomically based coding systems that require frame-by-frame video analysis of muscle movements. Researchers have also devised techniques to analyze spectrograms of vocal expressions of emotion (Scherer, et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR64">2003</xref>
). Researchers attend to a number of technical parameters when analyzing vocal expressions of emotion including the mean, variability, and range of frequency, as well as vocal intensity and spectral noise (Scherer, et al.).</p>
<p>Researchers have sometimes, but not always, relied on bottom-up descriptions of emotion signals rather than making a priori predictions of what should be observed (Ekman
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR20">1993</xref>
). Kagan (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR50">2007</xref>
) has called for those in the affective sciences to take more of a Baconian, bottom-up, approach given that the field is still in its early stages of development. In line with these traditions, we provide tactile descriptions of emotion and do so with a more modest coding system than those available for the face and voice. We describe the duration and most often used tactile behaviors to communicate sympathy, anger, and happiness. Our coding system includes several qualities of touch (e.g., squeezing, stroking, tapping, trembling, hitting, scratching,) and is based on a number of other systems used by researchers investigating touch in human communication (e.g., Argyle
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR1">1975</xref>
; Jones and Yarbrough
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR49">1985</xref>
; Weiss
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR73">1992</xref>
).</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec4" sec-type="methods">
<title>Method</title>
<sec id="Sec5">
<title>Participants</title>
<p>The sample consisted of 212 participants (106 unacquainted dyads) from a large public university who ranged in age from 18 to 40 years (
<italic>M</italic>
 = 20.15 years,
<italic>SD</italic>
 = 3.20). Participants received extra credit for an introductory psychology course for participating. The self-identified ethnic background of the sample was primarily Caucasian (34%), Chinese (30%), and Korean (12%). One member of the dyad was randomly assigned to the role of encoder, the other to the role of decoder. Like Banse and Scherer (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR3">1996</xref>
), we use the terms
<italic>encoding</italic>
and
<italic>decoding</italic>
because they connote the research method; no inference should be made that a “code” exists in the emotional signal. The gender breakdown of the four possible dyads was as follows (encoder–decoder): female–female (
<italic>n</italic>
 = 24), female–male (
<italic>n</italic>
 = 27), male–male (
<italic>n</italic>
 = 27), and male–female (
<italic>n</italic>
 = 28) (Hertenstein et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR42">2006a</xref>
).</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec6">
<title>Procedure and Materials</title>
<p>Upon arrival, the encoder and decoder sat at a table and were separated by an opaque black curtain. The participants could neither see nor talk to each other during the experiment, to preclude the possibility that they might provide nontactile clues to the emotion being communicated. Twelve emotion words were displayed serially to the encoder on sheets of paper in a randomized order. The encoder was instructed to think about how he or she wanted to communicate each emotion and then to make contact with the decoder’s bare arm from the elbow to the end of the hand to signal each emotion, using any form of touch he or she deemed appropriate. The decoder could not see any part of the touch because his or her arm was positioned on the encoder’s side of the curtain. We restricted the location of the touch because we wanted to limit the possibility that participants would receive any cues about their partner’s height, gender, build, etc. Participants were not told the partner’s gender and all tactile displays were video recorded. After each tactile display was administered, the decoder was administered a forced-choice response sheet reading “Please choose the term that best describes what this person is communicating to you.” The response sheet contained the following 13 response options:
<italic>anger</italic>
,
<italic>disgust</italic>
,
<italic>fear</italic>
,
<italic>happiness</italic>
,
<italic>sadness</italic>
,
<italic>surprise</italic>
,
<italic>sympathy</italic>
,
<italic>embarrassment</italic>
,
<italic>love</italic>
,
<italic>envy</italic>
,
<italic>pride</italic>
, and
<italic>gratitude</italic>
, as well as
<italic>none of these terms are correct</italic>
, to possibly reduce artificial inflation of accuracy rates (see Frank and Stennett
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR25">2001</xref>
). These emotions were listed in random order across participants (Hertenstein et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR42">2006a</xref>
).</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec7">
<title>Measures and Coding</title>
<p>The dependent measure of interest to address our hypotheses was the proportion of participants selecting each response option when decoding the tactile stimuli. Thus, for each of the 12 target emotions, the proportion that participants chose each response option was computed (e.g., the proportion of participants that chose
<italic>sympathy</italic>
when sympathy was intended to be communicated by the encoder). This measure is in line with a long tradition in the emotion literature in which forced-choice methodologies have been employed (e.g., Ekman
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR19">1972</xref>
; Frank and Stennett
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR25">2001</xref>
; Tracy et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR72">2009</xref>
). In addition to this measure, we asked decoders on a questionnaire at the end of the study the following: “Do you think a male or a female was touching you in this experiment?” The response options included
<italic>male</italic>
or
<italic>female</italic>
.</p>
<p>All of the tactile displays were coded on a second-by-second basis by research assistants who were naive to the emotion being communicated. The coding system was informed by a survey of coding systems used by researchers investigating touch (e.g., Argyle
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR1">1975</xref>
; Jones and Yarbrough
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR49">1985</xref>
; Weiss
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR73">1992</xref>
). The specific types of touch that were coded included holding the other, squeezing, stroking, rubbing, pushing, pulling, pressing, patting, tapping, shaking, pinching, trembling, poking, hitting, scratching, massaging, tickling, slapping, lifting, picking, finger interlocking, swinging, and tossing (i.e., tossing the decoder’s hand). In addition, the duration that each encoder touched the decoder for each emotion was calculated. Interrater agreement on all of the codes, based on 20% overlap in coders’ judgments, ranged from .83 to .99.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec8">
<title>Results</title>
<sec id="Sec9">
<title>Decoding of Emotions</title>
<p>In Table 
<xref rid="Tab2" ref-type="table">2</xref>
, we present the proportion of participants choosing the two most common response options for each of the target emotions of interest—sympathy, anger, and happiness. We display these data for each of the four gender dyad combinations. For example, amongst the all-male dyads, 70.4% of the decoders chose
<italic>anger</italic>
and 14.8% chose
<italic>fear</italic>
in the condition in which anger was attempted to be communicated by the encoder.
<table-wrap id="Tab2">
<label>Table 2</label>
<caption>
<p>Mean recognitions rates (%) for the anger, happiness, and sympathy tactile conditions</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th rowspan="3">Encoder–decoder group</th>
<th colspan="6">Emotion</th>
</tr>
<tr>
<th colspan="2">Anger</th>
<th colspan="2">Happiness</th>
<th colspan="2">Sympathy</th>
</tr>
<tr>
<th>Target</th>
<th>2nd</th>
<th>Target</th>
<th>2nd</th>
<th>Target</th>
<th>2nd</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td>Male–Male</td>
<td>70.4**</td>
<td>FE 14.8</td>
<td>18.5</td>
<td>GR 33.3</td>
<td>40.7</td>
<td>LO 33.3</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Male–Female</td>
<td>57.1**</td>
<td>FE 14.3</td>
<td>28.6</td>
<td>GR 25.0</td>
<td>64.3**</td>
<td>GR 10.7</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Female–Female</td>
<td>37.5</td>
<td>DI 20.8</td>
<td>50.0**</td>
<td>GR 16.7</td>
<td>62.5**</td>
<td>SA, LO, GR 8.3</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Female–Male</td>
<td>59.3**</td>
<td>DI 25.9</td>
<td>25.9</td>
<td>GR 18.5</td>
<td>59.3**</td>
<td>LO 18.5</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
<table-wrap-foot>
<p>Values under “target” refer to the proportion of time that the target emotion was chosen as the first choice (e.g.,
<italic>anger</italic>
chosen in the anger touch condition) and the values in the “2nd” column refer to the next most common response option that was chosen. Asterisks denote greater-than-chance accuracy rates greater than 25% as tested by binomials</p>
<p>
<italic>DI</italic>
disgust,
<italic>FE</italic>
fear,
<italic>SA</italic>
sadness,
<italic>LO</italic>
love,
<italic>GR</italic>
gratitude</p>
<p>**
<italic>p</italic>
 > .01</p>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
</p>
<p>For each of our predictions, we conducted four binomial tests—one for each of the four possible gender dyad combinations—for each emotion of interest (i.e., sympathy, anger, and happiness). Specifically, the proportion of participants who chose each response option was assessed against chance for all of the target emotions. Following Frank and Stennett (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR25">2001</xref>
), we set chance at 25% (for a rationale, see Hertenstein et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR42">2006a</xref>
). This strategy allowed us to test if a given gender dyad type was capable of communicating a given emotion at above chance levels. In the example described above, the binomial tests indicated that
<italic>anger</italic>
was chosen at above-chance levels, whereas
<italic>fear</italic>
was not.</p>
<p>The data presented in Table 
<xref rid="Tab2" ref-type="table">2</xref>
support our hypotheses. Our first prediction was that when females are in an experimental dyad, sympathy would be decoded at above-chance levels. Supporting this hypothesis, sympathy was communicated at greater-than-chance levels only when a female comprised at least one member of the dyad (on average, 62% accuracy). Importantly, the second most commonly chosen response option for all of these dyad types never exceeded chance levels. Dyads consisting solely of males did not communicate sympathy at above chance levels.</p>
<p>Our second prediction was that dyads involving males would communicate anger with touch at above-chance levels. Consistent with this hypothesis, anger was communicated at greater-than-chance levels only when a male comprised at least one member of the dyad (on average, 62% accuracy). Moreover, the second most commonly chosen response option for all of these dyad types did not exceed chance. Dyads consisting only of females did not communicate anger at above chance levels.</p>
<p>Finally, we predicted that dyads comprised solely of females would communicate happiness with touch at above-chance levels. Supporting this hypothesis, only dyads consisting solely of females communicated happiness and the secondly most common response option, gratitude, was not above chance levels. This finding on happiness dovetails with studies showing that women smile more (LaFrance et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR58">2003</xref>
), share emotions more (Rimé et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR61">2002</xref>
), and experience more prosocial emotions (Shiota et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR66">2006</xref>
). It should be mentioned that the other target emotions investigated were either communicated by all of the four dyad types at greater-than-chance levels (fear, disgust, love, gratitude), or none of the four dyad types (sadness, surprise, embarrassment, envy, and pride).</p>
<p>It is clearly possible that decoders could reliably infer the gender of the encoder, perhaps from the quality of the touch administered. This categorization, furthermore, could have influenced their judgments of the emotion-related touches (Hess et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR46">2004</xref>
). As indicated, the gender of the encoder was not verbally revealed to the decoder. However, was it possible for decoders to ascertain encoders’ gender via touch? To address this question, we computed the percentage of cases in which decoders accurately inferred the gender of the encoder. Setting chance at 50%, we conducted binomial tests and found that 79% of female decoders correctly identified male encoders and 96% correctly identified female encoders (both
<italic>p</italic>
s < .01). For male decoders, 70% (
<italic>p</italic>
 = .052) correctly identified male encoders and 81% (
<italic>p</italic>
 < .01) correctly identified female encoders. These results indicate that decoders were capable of accurately decoding the encoder’s gender.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec10">
<title>Encoding of Emotions</title>
<p>Did the gender composition of the dyads influence the tactile actions associated with the communication of the different emotions? In Table 
<xref rid="Tab3" ref-type="table">3</xref>
, we present the average durations of tactile contact in the four dyads for the emotions of interest. One-way omnibus ANOVAS were performed for the three emotions of interest entering the gender dyad type (4 levels) as the independent factor and the duration of time that elapsed for the tactile behaviors as the dependent variable. The duration of tactile behaviors did not differ between the dyad gender types for the emotions (all
<italic>p</italic>
s > .05). Post-hoc pair-wise comparisons were conducted for each emotion to examine whether there was any difference in duration between any two gender dyad combinations. These analyses yielded no statistically significant results (all
<italic>p</italic>
s > .05). These analyses indicate that the observed gender differences in the accuracy of communicating sympathy, anger, and happiness could not be attributed to differences in how long participants made tactile contact.
<table-wrap id="Tab3">
<label>Table 3</label>
<caption>
<p>Duration of tactile behaviors (in seconds) that were accurately decoded as a function of dyad gender</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th rowspan="3">Encoder–Decoder group</th>
<th colspan="6">Emotion</th>
</tr>
<tr>
<th colspan="2">Anger</th>
<th colspan="2">Happiness</th>
<th colspan="2">Sympathy</th>
</tr>
<tr>
<th>
<italic>M</italic>
</th>
<th>
<italic>SD</italic>
</th>
<th>
<italic>M</italic>
</th>
<th>
<italic>SD</italic>
</th>
<th>
<italic>M</italic>
</th>
<th>
<italic>SD</italic>
</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td>Male–Male</td>
<td>3.47</td>
<td>(1.68)</td>
<td>7.46</td>
<td>(4.39)</td>
<td>6.91</td>
<td>(3.91)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Male–Female</td>
<td>5.14</td>
<td>(3.25)</td>
<td>8.00</td>
<td>(5.06)</td>
<td>8.33</td>
<td>(6.28)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Female–Female</td>
<td>5.23</td>
<td>(7.63)</td>
<td>8.58</td>
<td>(7.42)</td>
<td>7.50</td>
<td>(4.20)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Female–Male</td>
<td>4.38</td>
<td>(2.58)</td>
<td>8.43</td>
<td>(3.50)</td>
<td>7.50</td>
<td>(5.83)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Average</td>
<td>4.54</td>
<td>(3.74)</td>
<td>8.10</td>
<td>(5.55)</td>
<td>7.57</td>
<td>(5.21)</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</table-wrap>
</p>
<p>Table 
<xref rid="Tab4" ref-type="table">4</xref>
presents data relevant to the more specific patterns of tactile behavior that women and men relied on to communicate the emotions of interest. The values reported indicate the percentage of time encoders used each quality of touch as a function of the total time touched for the trial. Here, one finds differences that might explain the decoding differences reported earlier. Sympathy was associated most with patting for all dyads, but the second most common behavior for all male dyads was shaking, whereas this was not the case for the other dyads. Turning to anger, one finds that this emotion was associated with squeezing for all dyad groups except all female dyads, the group that did not communicate anger at above-chance levels. In addition, pushing was not one of the most frequent types of touch for the male–female group, whereas it was for the other dyad types. Finally, finger interlocking was in the most frequently employed tactile behaviors except the group that accurately communicated happiness—the group comprised solely of females. Patting, however, was one of the most frequent types of touch used by the all female dyad, whereas this was not true for the other dyad types.
<table-wrap id="Tab4">
<label>Table 4</label>
<caption>
<p>Percentage of most frequent types of touch used that were accurately decoded as a function of dyad gender</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th colspan="13">Encoder–Decoder group</th>
</tr>
<tr>
<th rowspan="2">Emotion</th>
<th colspan="3">Male–Male</th>
<th colspan="3">Male–Female</th>
<th colspan="3">Female–Female</th>
<th colspan="3">Female–Male</th>
</tr>
<tr>
<th>Tactile behavior</th>
<th>
<italic>M</italic>
</th>
<th>
<italic>SD</italic>
</th>
<th>Tactile behavior</th>
<th>
<italic>M</italic>
</th>
<th>
<italic>SD</italic>
</th>
<th>Tactile behavior</th>
<th>
<italic>M</italic>
</th>
<th>
<italic>SD</italic>
</th>
<th>Tactile behavior</th>
<th>
<italic>M</italic>
</th>
<th>
<italic>SD</italic>
</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td rowspan="5">Anger</td>
<td>Squeeze</td>
<td>40.44</td>
<td>47.07</td>
<td>Hit</td>
<td>30.31</td>
<td>43.23</td>
<td>Shake</td>
<td>22.22</td>
<td>44.10</td>
<td>Tremble</td>
<td>18.00</td>
<td>38.78</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Hit</td>
<td>27.17</td>
<td>43.75</td>
<td>Squeeze</td>
<td>16.31</td>
<td>33.89</td>
<td>Press</td>
<td>18.56</td>
<td>37.06</td>
<td>Hit</td>
<td>16.25</td>
<td>33.74</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Tremble</td>
<td>16.94</td>
<td>35.86</td>
<td>Lift</td>
<td>12.06</td>
<td>26.19</td>
<td>Hit</td>
<td>16.62</td>
<td>30.90</td>
<td>Slap</td>
<td>15.75</td>
<td>29.78</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Push</td>
<td>7.39</td>
<td>24.38</td>
<td>Shake</td>
<td>11.81</td>
<td>24.7</td>
<td>Slap</td>
<td>13.89</td>
<td>33.33</td>
<td>Squeeze</td>
<td>13.75</td>
<td>30.76</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Slap</td>
<td>5.11</td>
<td>15.98</td>
<td>Slap</td>
<td>8.31</td>
<td>25.8</td>
<td>Push</td>
<td>11.11</td>
<td>33.33</td>
<td>Push</td>
<td>9.44</td>
<td>25.80</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="5">Happiness</td>
<td>Shake</td>
<td>40.00</td>
<td>52.92</td>
<td>Swing</td>
<td>58.00</td>
<td>40.25</td>
<td>Swing</td>
<td>66.32</td>
<td>48.36</td>
<td>Swing</td>
<td>63.71</td>
<td>36.73</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Swing</td>
<td>33.33</td>
<td>57.74</td>
<td>Shake</td>
<td>17.63</td>
<td>28.75</td>
<td>Shake</td>
<td>14.08</td>
<td>25.78</td>
<td>Finger interlocking</td>
<td>14.29</td>
<td>37.80</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Slap</td>
<td>26.67</td>
<td>46.19</td>
<td>Lift</td>
<td>7.63</td>
<td>7.46</td>
<td>Pat</td>
<td>8.33</td>
<td>20.77</td>
<td>Shake</td>
<td>6.00</td>
<td>12.37</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Lift</td>
<td>13.33</td>
<td>23.09</td>
<td>Finger interlocking</td>
<td>7.63</td>
<td>21.57</td>
<td>Tap</td>
<td>8.33</td>
<td>28.87</td>
<td>Slap</td>
<td>3.86</td>
<td>10.21</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Finger interlock</td>
<td>4.39</td>
<td>9.28</td>
<td>Stroke</td>
<td>3.13</td>
<td>8.84</td>
<td>Lift</td>
<td>4.17</td>
<td>8.55</td>
<td>Lift</td>
<td>1.57</td>
<td>4.16</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="5">Sympathy</td>
<td>Pat</td>
<td>38.36</td>
<td>36.77</td>
<td>Pat</td>
<td>34.78</td>
<td>38.67</td>
<td>Pat</td>
<td>39.57</td>
<td>29.25</td>
<td>Pat</td>
<td>27.87</td>
<td>34.20</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Shake</td>
<td>15.73</td>
<td>32.11</td>
<td>Tremble</td>
<td>11.67</td>
<td>20.08</td>
<td>Stroke</td>
<td>22.22</td>
<td>31.65</td>
<td>Stroke</td>
<td>17.00</td>
<td>30.68</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Stroke</td>
<td>10.18</td>
<td>16.52</td>
<td>Stroke</td>
<td>10.89</td>
<td>22.39</td>
<td>Shake</td>
<td>6.60</td>
<td>10.06</td>
<td>Tremble</td>
<td>8.87</td>
<td>26.61</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Push</td>
<td>6.45</td>
<td>21.41</td>
<td>Squeeze</td>
<td>9.28</td>
<td>21.88</td>
<td>Rub</td>
<td>4.33</td>
<td>10.29</td>
<td>Rub</td>
<td>8.80</td>
<td>26.07</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Rub</td>
<td>4.55</td>
<td>15.08</td>
<td>Rub</td>
<td>8.39</td>
<td>17.02</td>
<td>Squeeze</td>
<td>3.07</td>
<td>8.93</td>
<td>Shake</td>
<td>4.07</td>
<td>13.02</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
<table-wrap-foot>
<p>Mean values refer to the percentage of total touch time each touch quality was used by the encoder to communicate the given emotion. Values range from 0 to 100 and greater values reflect a greater percentage of time that a particular quality of touch was utilized by the encoder</p>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
</p>
<p>No formal hypotheses were proposed for the tactile encoding behaviors and there were a large number of touch qualities coded making inferential analyses unwieldy and difficult to interpret. Moreover, it is important to note that variability in the types of touch used by the dyads was marked. For these reasons, great inferential caution should be exercised when considering the links between encoding behaviors and decoding accuracy, as well as making comparative statements between the gender dyad types. Moreover, the great variability in the data obviates against there being a “prototype” expression of emotion as is often implicated in the face and voice (Ekman
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR20">1993</xref>
); instead, there seems to be a multitude of ways in which emotion can be communicated via touch between humans.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec11">
<title>Discussion</title>
<p>Here, we documented gender differences in the communication of distinct emotions via touch between humans. Guided by evolutionary and social role accounts of emotion, as well as the empirical literature, we hypothesized that women would be able to communicate sympathy and happiness through brief touches to the arm of a stranger, whereas men would be able to communicate anger. The data from the present study supported these predictions. We observed no gender-related differences in the communication of disgust, fear, envy, embarrassment, sadness, pride, love, and gratitude.</p>
<p>Sympathy was only communicated accurately through tactile contact to the arm in dyads comprised of at least one female; dyads consisting of only males communicated sympathy at less-than-chance levels. This result is consistent with studies documenting gender differences in self-reports of compassion (Shiota et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR66">2006</xref>
), self-reports of empathy (Eisenberg and Lennon
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR17">1983</xref>
), and interests in care-taking (Gilligan
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR26">1982</xref>
). Whether similar gender differences in the communication of sympathy would be observed in studies of emotion-related facial display or vocalization is an important question, and one that would more fully characterize the extent to which women enjoy an advantage in communicating the quintessential care-taking emotion—sympathy.</p>
<p>Anger, in contrast, was communicated accurately only when the dyad contained at least one male; dyads comprised of only females communicated anger at less-than-chance levels. Interestingly, the most accurate dyad groups were those comprised solely of males. These findings dovetail with the well documented tendency for men to show more aggressive behavior than women (Daly and Wilson
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR15">1994</xref>
), stereotypes of men as more angry, and recent evidence generated by Becker et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR4">2007</xref>
) in the realm of facial displays finding that humans’ perceptual systems are tuned to be particularly sensitive to angry facial expression by males. In their research, participants more quickly and accurately classified the word
<italic>angry</italic>
with male faces than female faces, and more quickly and accurately judged angry faces when they were displayed by males than females.</p>
<p>Finally, we found that the gender composition of the dyads also affected the communication of happiness. That is, only dyads comprised solely of females communicated happiness at greater-than-chance levels. As mentioned, this finding dovetails with studies showing that women smile more (LaFrance et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR58">2003</xref>
), share emotions more (Rimé et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR61">2002</xref>
), and experience more prosocial emotions (Shiota et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR66">2006</xref>
). The data are also consistent with Becker et al.’s (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR4">2007</xref>
) work indicating that participants (a) thought of female facial displays more often than male displays when asked to spontaneously generate a mental image of a happy face, (b) more quickly and accurately classified the word
<italic>happy</italic>
with female faces than male faces, (c) more quickly and accurately judged happy faces when they were displayed by females than males, and (d) perceived faces as more happy when they were feminized.</p>
<p>The present study adopted the design of some traditional emotion recognition investigations in the field. Given that the study was not a true experiment, causal inferences must be made cautiously. However, several features of the paradigm increase our confidence in the findings (Hertenstein et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR42">2006a</xref>
). In most previous judgment studies, observers judged highly prototypical displays or those posed by actors, whereas in our study people decoded emotion from the idiosyncratic tactile actions of other untrained participants (see Hertenstein
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR40">2010</xref>
for a discussion regarding bottom-up approaches to emotion; also, see Clynes and Nettheim (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR14">1982</xref>
) for a unique approach to studying button-pressing and the association of some emotions). Second, our response format included the response option
<italic>none of these terms are correct</italic>
, which reduced the likelihood of inflated accuracy rates (Frank and Stennett
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR25">2001</xref>
). Finally, we restricted the tactile stimulation to one location on the body, thus eliminating one aspect of tactile communication—location on the body of the touch recipient—that is likely to provide additional information with respect to the emotion communicated.</p>
<p>What might explain the findings we observed in terms of gender and touch? Because decoders could not see the “tactile interaction” that transpired on the other side of the screen, decoders must have relied upon tactile cues alone to ascertain the gender of the encoder. Our data indicated that decoders accurately perceived encoders’ gender between 70% and 96% of the cases depending on the specific gender-dyad composition. As a result, it is possible that decoders may have been interpreting the encoder’s touch by means of gender-stereotypes (Hess et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR46">2004</xref>
). If this were occurring, even at non-conscious levels, stereotypes may have inflated accuracy for the emotions most readily associated with gender stereotypes like anger and sympathy. According to Hess et al., women are expected to display sadness more and men to display more anger. Indeed, when subjects rated the likelihood of neutral facial displays to exhibit various emotions, women’s faces were expected to display more stereotypic female emotions whereas men’s faces were expected to show more male stereotypic faces (Hess et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR47">2007</xref>
). Although our study was not designed to investigate whether these findings held in the tactile modality, similar processes may underlie our results. This points to a future area of investigation.</p>
<p>It is also possible that decoders’ knowledge of the encoders’ gender may play a role in the meaning that they attributed to touch because of stereotypes (Brody and Hall
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR10">2008</xref>
). For example, decoders may be more likely to interpret a particular tactile gestalt from a female as sympathy whereas decoders may interpret the same tactile gestalt from a male as a different emotion. These explanations are consistent with stereotype theorists (e.g., Biernat
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR6">2003</xref>
), as well as empirical studies (e.g., Hess et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR45">2000</xref>
) which indicate that membership in a stereotyped group—in this case, the gender of both the encoder and decoder—can drive ambiguous perceptions in the direction of the stereotypes.</p>
<p>The above explanations focus on the decoder, but the tactile behaviors used by the encoders may well lead to the observed gender differences in perception. There is evidence of gender differences in the behaviors used by encoders to communicate emotion (Hertenstein et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR44">2009</xref>
). In the current study, the gender of decoders was never verbally revealed to encoders by the experimenter. However, given that encoders administered the tactile stimulation on their side of the opaque screen to the decoders, the encoders were able to visually see the morphology of the decoder’s arm. Although we did not ask encoders at the end of the study whether they believed they were touching a male or female decoder, we think it likely that encoders knew the gender of decoders. It is possible that the gender of the decoder may have influenced the tactile behaviors used by the encoder to communicate the emotions. Moreover, the gender of the decoder may interact with the gender of the encoder to influence the demonstrated behavior. Indeed, there is evidence of this in Table 
<xref rid="Tab4" ref-type="table">4</xref>
. For example, dyads comprised entirely of males squeezed each other 40% of the time, whereas males squeezed females less than half of this time in the anger condition. Also noteworthy in this condition is that squeezing behavior in dyads comprised entirely of females was not in the five most common types of touch and was only evident 14% of the time in dyads comprised of female encoders and male decoders. Overall, there was evidence that the target’s gender influenced the behavior of the encoder and that the gender of both participants interacted. Again, this is consistent with the literature indicating that group membership norms—in this case gender—for displaying particular emotions influences both the decoding and encoding of emotions (Kirouac and Hess
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR52">1999</xref>
).</p>
<p>In sum, our study documents gender differences in the communication of distinct emotions between humans via touch. Studies have shown that females more accurately identify the meaning of a variety of non-verbal cues, including expressions of emotion (Brody and Hall
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR10">2008</xref>
). However, Brody and Hall persuasively argue that the goal for current researchers is to identify and document specific variables that moderate and mediate gender differences evident in non-verbal communication. Indeed, our study demonstrates that there is not an overall female advantage in encoding and decoding emotion as is sometimes suggested, but that it is emotion specific in the tactile modality.</p>
<p>Our study suggests a number of important directions for future research. First, it will be important for research to identify the sources of the gender differences in the communication of emotion that we observed. We examined whether these differences might be related to differences in tactile behaviors. Experimental studies could directly document whether these behavioral differences produce differences in the decoding of sympathy, anger, and happiness. Second, studies of stratified and egalitarian cultures with respect to gender could more explicitly address whether these gender differences in sympathy, anger, and happiness hold across cultures where the gender roles are more differentiated or not (Wood and Eagly
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR74">2002</xref>
). This kind of research will document the deeper origins of likely gender differences in the communication of emotion via touch. Third, future research should examine why some paradigms used to study tactile human communication do not yield gender differences, at least in the decoding of emotion (e.g., Bailenson et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR2">2007</xref>
; Hertenstein et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR44">2009</xref>
). Fourth, investigations specifically designed to uncover potential gender differences in how emotions are communicated via haptic devices (e.g., Bailenson et al.
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR2">2007</xref>
; Smith and MacLean
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR68">2007</xref>
) and button-pressing (e.g., Clynes and Nettheim
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR14">1982</xref>
) would be valuable. Finally, it will be important to examine possible gender differences from a developmental perspective (Hertenstein
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR39">2002</xref>
). Our study contained a few participants that extended the age range of our sample which may have influenced the findings (4 members over 30-years-old). It will be important for future research to systematically examine how age interacts with gender in the communication of emotion via touch.</p>
</sec>
</body>
<back>
<ack>
<p>
<bold>Open Access</bold>
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.</p>
</ack>
<ref-list id="Bib1">
<title>References</title>
<ref id="CR1">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Argyle</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>Bodily communication</source>
<year>1975</year>
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>International Universities Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR2">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bailenson</surname>
<given-names>JN</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Yee</surname>
<given-names>N</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brave</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Merget</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Koslow</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Virtual interpersonal touch: Expressing and recognizing emotions through haptic devices</article-title>
<source>Human-Computer Interaction</source>
<year>2007</year>
<volume>22</volume>
<fpage>325</fpage>
<lpage>353</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR3">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Banse</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Scherer</surname>
<given-names>KR</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression</article-title>
<source>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</source>
<year>1996</year>
<volume>70</volume>
<fpage>614</fpage>
<lpage>636</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.614</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8851745</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR4">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Becker</surname>
<given-names>DV</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kenrick</surname>
<given-names>DT</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Neuberg</surname>
<given-names>SL</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Blackwell</surname>
<given-names>KC</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>DM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The confounded nature of angry men and happy women</article-title>
<source>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</source>
<year>2007</year>
<volume>92</volume>
<fpage>179</fpage>
<lpage>190</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.179</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17279844</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR5">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Berkowitz</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control</source>
<year>1993</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>McGraw-Hill</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR6">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Biernat</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Toward a broader view of social stereotyping</article-title>
<source>The American Psychologist</source>
<year>2003</year>
<volume>58</volume>
<fpage>1019</fpage>
<lpage>1027</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0003-066X.58.12.1019</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14664690</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR7">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Birnbaum</surname>
<given-names>DW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The etiology of children’s stereotypes about sex differences in emotionality</article-title>
<source>Sex Roles</source>
<year>1984</year>
<volume>10</volume>
<fpage>677</fpage>
<lpage>691</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/BF00287379</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR8">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Brody</surname>
<given-names>LR</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Ablon</surname>
<given-names>SL</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brown</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Khantzian</surname>
<given-names>EJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mack</surname>
<given-names>JE</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>On understanding gender differences in the expression of emotion: Gender roles, socialization and language</article-title>
<source>Human feelings: Exploration in affect development and meaning</source>
<year>1993</year>
<publisher-loc>England</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Analytic</publisher-name>
<fpage>87</fpage>
<lpage>121</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR9">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Brody</surname>
<given-names>LR</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hall</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Lewis</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Haviland-Jones</surname>
<given-names>JM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Gender, emotion, and expression</article-title>
<source>Handbook of emotions</source>
<year>2000</year>
<edition>2</edition>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Guilford</publisher-name>
<fpage>338</fpage>
<lpage>349</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR10">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Brody</surname>
<given-names>LR</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hall</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Lewis</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Haviland-Jones</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Gender and emotion in context</article-title>
<source>Handbook of emotions</source>
<year>2008</year>
<edition>3</edition>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Guilford</publisher-name>
<fpage>395</fpage>
<lpage>408</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR11">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Citrin</surname>
<given-names>LB</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Roberts</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fredrickson</surname>
<given-names>BL</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Tiedens</surname>
<given-names>LZ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Leach</surname>
<given-names>CW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Objectification theory and emotions: A feminist psychological perspective on gendered affect</article-title>
<source>The social life of emotions</source>
<year>2004</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
<fpage>203</fpage>
<lpage>223</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR12">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Clark</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>Misery and company: Sympathy in everyday life</source>
<year>1997</year>
<publisher-loc>Chicago</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>University of Chicago Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR13">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Clark</surname>
<given-names>RD</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Shields</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Family communication and delinquency</article-title>
<source>Adolescence</source>
<year>1997</year>
<volume>32</volume>
<fpage>81</fpage>
<lpage>92</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">9105493</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR14">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Clynes</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nettheim</surname>
<given-names>N</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Clynes</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The living quality of music: Neurobiologic patterns of communicating feeling</article-title>
<source>Music, mind and brain: The neuropsychology of music</source>
<year>1982</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Plenum</publisher-name>
<fpage>47</fpage>
<lpage>82</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR15">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Daly</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wilson</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Archer</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Evolutionary psychology of male violence</article-title>
<source>Male violence</source>
<year>1994</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Routledge</publisher-name>
<fpage>253</fpage>
<lpage>288</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR16">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Day</surname>
<given-names>AL</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Carroll</surname>
<given-names>SA</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Using an ability-based measure of emotional intelligence to predict individual performance, group performance, and group citizenship behaviors</article-title>
<source>Personality and Individual Differences</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>36</volume>
<fpage>1443</fpage>
<lpage>1458</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00240-X</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR17">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Eisenberg</surname>
<given-names>N</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lennon</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Sex differences in empathy and related capacities</article-title>
<source>Psychological Bulletin</source>
<year>1983</year>
<volume>94</volume>
<fpage>100</fpage>
<lpage>131</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-2909.94.1.100</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR18">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Eisenberg</surname>
<given-names>N</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fabes</surname>
<given-names>RA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Miller</surname>
<given-names>PA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fultz</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Shell</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mathy</surname>
<given-names>RM</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
<article-title>Relation of sympathy and personal distress to prosocial behavior: A multimethod study</article-title>
<source>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</source>
<year>1989</year>
<volume>57</volume>
<fpage>55</fpage>
<lpage>66</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.57.1.55</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">2754604</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR19">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ekman</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Cole</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion</article-title>
<source>Nebraska symposium on motivation 1971</source>
<year>1972</year>
<publisher-loc>Lincoln</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>University of Nebraska Press</publisher-name>
<fpage>207</fpage>
<lpage>283</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR20">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ekman</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Facial expressions of emotion</article-title>
<source>The American Psychologist</source>
<year>1993</year>
<volume>48</volume>
<fpage>384</fpage>
<lpage>392</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0003-066X.48.4.384</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8512154</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR21">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ekman</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Friesen</surname>
<given-names>WV</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>Facial action coding system: A technique for the measurement of facial movement</source>
<year>1978</year>
<publisher-loc>Palo Alto</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Consulting Psychologists</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR22">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Elfenbein</surname>
<given-names>HA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ambady</surname>
<given-names>N</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis</article-title>
<source>Psychological Bulletin</source>
<year>2002</year>
<volume>128</volume>
<fpage>203</fpage>
<lpage>235</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.203</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11931516</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR23">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fischer</surname>
<given-names>AH</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Sex differences in emotionality: Fact or stereotype?</article-title>
<source>Feminism & Psychology</source>
<year>1993</year>
<volume>3</volume>
<fpage>303</fpage>
<lpage>318</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0959353593033002</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR24">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fivush</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Reese</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Haden</surname>
<given-names>CA</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Elaborating on elaborations: Role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development</article-title>
<source>Child Development</source>
<year>2006</year>
<volume>77</volume>
<fpage>1568</fpage>
<lpage>1588</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00960.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17107447</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR25">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Frank</surname>
<given-names>MG</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Stennett</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The forced-choice paradigm and the perception of facial expressions of emotion</article-title>
<source>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</source>
<year>2001</year>
<volume>80</volume>
<fpage>75</fpage>
<lpage>85</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.75</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11195893</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR26">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gilligan</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>In a different voice: Psychological theory and women’s development</source>
<year>1982</year>
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Harvard University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR27">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Goetz</surname>
<given-names>JL</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Keltner</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Tracy</surname>
<given-names>JL</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Robins</surname>
<given-names>RW</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tangney</surname>
<given-names>JP</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Shifting meanings of self-conscious emotions across cultures: A social-functional approach</article-title>
<source>The self-conscious emotions: Theory and research</source>
<year>2007</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Guilford</publisher-name>
<fpage>153</fpage>
<lpage>173</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR28">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gohm</surname>
<given-names>CL</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Clore</surname>
<given-names>GL</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Individual differences in emotional experience: Mapping available scales to processes</article-title>
<source>Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin</source>
<year>2000</year>
<volume>26</volume>
<fpage>679</fpage>
<lpage>697</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0146167200268004</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR29">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gross</surname>
<given-names>JJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>John</surname>
<given-names>OP</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Mapping the domain of expressivity: Multimethod evidence for a hierarchical model</article-title>
<source>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</source>
<year>1998</year>
<volume>74</volume>
<fpage>170</fpage>
<lpage>191</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.170</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">9457781</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR30">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gross</surname>
<given-names>JJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fredrickson</surname>
<given-names>BL</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Levenson</surname>
<given-names>RW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The psychophysiology of crying</article-title>
<source>Psychophysiology</source>
<year>1994</year>
<volume>31</volume>
<fpage>460</fpage>
<lpage>468</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb01049.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">7972600</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR31">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Halberstadt</surname>
<given-names>AG</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hayes</surname>
<given-names>CW</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pike</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Gender and gender role differences in smiling and communication consistency</article-title>
<source>Sex Roles</source>
<year>1988</year>
<volume>19</volume>
<fpage>589</fpage>
<lpage>604</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/BF00289738</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR32">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hall</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>Nonverbal sex difference: Accuracy of communication and expressive style</source>
<year>1990</year>
<publisher-loc>Baltimore</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Johns Hopkins University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR33">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hall</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Carter</surname>
<given-names>JD</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Horgan</surname>
<given-names>TG</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Fischer</surname>
<given-names>AH</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Gender differences in nonverbal communication of emotion</article-title>
<source>Gender and emotion: Social psychological perspectives</source>
<year>2000</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
<fpage>97</fpage>
<lpage>117</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR34">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hall</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Carney</surname>
<given-names>DR</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Murphy</surname>
<given-names>NA</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Abel</surname>
<given-names>MH</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Gender differences in smiling</article-title>
<source>An empirical reflection on the smile</source>
<year>2002</year>
<publisher-loc>Lewiston</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Edwin Mellen</publisher-name>
<fpage>155</fpage>
<lpage>185</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR35">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hall</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Coats</surname>
<given-names>EJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Smith LeBeau</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Nonverbal behavior and the vertical dimension of social relations: A meta-analysis</article-title>
<source>Psychological Bulletin</source>
<year>2005</year>
<volume>131</volume>
<fpage>898</fpage>
<lpage>924</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.898</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16351328</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR36">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Henley</surname>
<given-names>NM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Status and sex: Some touching observations</article-title>
<source>Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society</source>
<year>1973</year>
<volume>2</volume>
<fpage>91</fpage>
<lpage>93</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR37">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Henley</surname>
<given-names>N</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>Body politics: Power, sex and nonverbal communication</source>
<year>1977</year>
<publisher-loc>Englewood Cliffs</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Prentice-Hall</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR38">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Henley</surname>
<given-names>NM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Kalbfleisch</surname>
<given-names>PJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cody</surname>
<given-names>MJ</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Body politics revisited: What do you know today?</article-title>
<source>Gender, power, and communication in human relationships</source>
<year>1995</year>
<publisher-loc>Hillsdale</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Erlbaum</publisher-name>
<fpage>27</fpage>
<lpage>61</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR39">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hertenstein</surname>
<given-names>MJ</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Touch: Its communicative functions in infancy</article-title>
<source>Human Development</source>
<year>2002</year>
<volume>45</volume>
<fpage>70</fpage>
<lpage>94</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1159/000048154</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR40">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hertenstein</surname>
<given-names>MJ</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Cautions in the study of infant emotional displays</article-title>
<source>Emotion Review</source>
<year>2010</year>
<volume>2</volume>
<fpage>130</fpage>
<lpage>131</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/1754073909355005</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR41">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hertenstein</surname>
<given-names>MJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Campos</surname>
<given-names>JJ</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Emotion regulation via maternal touch</article-title>
<source>Infancy</source>
<year>2001</year>
<volume>2</volume>
<fpage>549</fpage>
<lpage>566</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1207/S15327078IN0204_09</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR42">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hertenstein</surname>
<given-names>MJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Keltner</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>App</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bulleit</surname>
<given-names>BA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jaskolka</surname>
<given-names>AR</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Touch communicates distinct emotions</article-title>
<source>Emotion</source>
<year>2006a</year>
<volume>6</volume>
<fpage>528</fpage>
<lpage>533</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.528</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16938094</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR43">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hertenstein</surname>
<given-names>MJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Verkamp</surname>
<given-names>JM</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kerestes</surname>
<given-names>AM</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Holmes</surname>
<given-names>RM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The communicative functions of touch in humans, nonhuman primates, and rats: A review and synthesis of the empirical research</article-title>
<source>Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs</source>
<year>2006b</year>
<volume>132</volume>
<fpage>5</fpage>
<lpage>94</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3200/MONO.132.1.5-94</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR44">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hertenstein</surname>
<given-names>MJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Holmes</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McCullough</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Keltner</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The communication of emotion via touch</article-title>
<source>Emotion</source>
<year>2009</year>
<volume>9</volume>
<fpage>566</fpage>
<lpage>573</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0016108</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19653781</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR45">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hess</surname>
<given-names>U</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Senécal</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kirouac</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Herrera</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Philippot</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kleck</surname>
<given-names>RE</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Emotional expressivity in men and women: Stereotypes and self-perceptions</article-title>
<source>Cognition & Emotion</source>
<year>2000</year>
<volume>14</volume>
<fpage>609</fpage>
<lpage>642</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/02699930050117648</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR46">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hess</surname>
<given-names>U</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Adams</surname>
<given-names>RB</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kleck</surname>
<given-names>RE</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Facial appearance, gender, and emotion expression</article-title>
<source>Emotion</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>4</volume>
<fpage>378</fpage>
<lpage>388</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/1528-3542.4.4.378</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15571436</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR47">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hess</surname>
<given-names>U</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Adams</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kleck</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Hess</surname>
<given-names>U</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Philippot</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>When two do the same, it might not mean the same: The perception of emotional expressions shown by men and women</article-title>
<source>Group dynamics and emotional expression</source>
<year>2007</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
<fpage>33</fpage>
<lpage>50</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR48">
<mixed-citation publication-type="other">Izard, C. E. (1979).
<italic>The maximally discriminative facial movement coding system (MAX</italic>
). Unpublished manuscript. Available from Instructional Resource Center, University of Delaware, Newark, DE.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR49">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Jones</surname>
<given-names>SE</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Yarbrough</surname>
<given-names>AE</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>A naturalistic study of the meanings of touch</article-title>
<source>Communication Monographs</source>
<year>1985</year>
<volume>52</volume>
<fpage>19</fpage>
<lpage>56</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/03637758509376094</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR50">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kagan</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>What is emotion? History, measures, and meaning</source>
<year>2007</year>
<publisher-loc>New Haven</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Yale University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR51">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kenrick</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Trost</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sundie</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Eagly</surname>
<given-names>AH</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sternberg</surname>
<given-names>RJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Beall</surname>
<given-names>AE</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Sex roles as adaptations: An evolutionary perspective on gender differences and similarities</article-title>
<source>The psychology of gender</source>
<year>2004</year>
<edition>2</edition>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Guilford</publisher-name>
<fpage>65</fpage>
<lpage>91</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR52">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kirouac</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hess</surname>
<given-names>U</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Philippot</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Feldman</surname>
<given-names>RS</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Coats</surname>
<given-names>EJ</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Group membership and the decoding of nonverbal behavior</article-title>
<source>The social context of nonverbal behavior</source>
<year>1999</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
<fpage>182</fpage>
<lpage>210</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR53">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Knapp</surname>
<given-names>ML</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hall</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>Nonverbal communication in human interaction</source>
<year>1997</year>
<edition>4</edition>
<publisher-loc>Fort Worth</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Harcourt Brace College</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR54">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kring</surname>
<given-names>AM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Fischer</surname>
<given-names>AH</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Gender and anger</article-title>
<source>Gender and emotion: Social psychological perspectives</source>
<year>2000</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
<fpage>211</fpage>
<lpage>231</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR55">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kring</surname>
<given-names>AM</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gordon</surname>
<given-names>AH</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Sex differences in emotion: Expression, experience, and physiology</article-title>
<source>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</source>
<year>1998</year>
<volume>74</volume>
<fpage>686</fpage>
<lpage>703</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.686</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">9523412</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR56">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>LaFrance</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Henley</surname>
<given-names>NM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Radtke</surname>
<given-names>HL</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Stam</surname>
<given-names>HJ</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>On oppressing hypotheses: Or differences in nonverbal sensitivity revisited</article-title>
<source>Power/gender: Social relations in theory and practice</source>
<year>1994</year>
<publisher-loc>Thousand Oaks</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Sage</publisher-name>
<fpage>287</fpage>
<lpage>311</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR57">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>LaFrance</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hecht</surname>
<given-names>MA</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Philippot</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Feldman</surname>
<given-names>RS</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Coats</surname>
<given-names>EJ</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Option or obligation to smile: The effects of power and gender on facial expression</article-title>
<source>The social context of nonverbal behavior</source>
<year>1999</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
<fpage>45</fpage>
<lpage>70</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR58">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>LaFrance</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hecht</surname>
<given-names>MA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Paluck</surname>
<given-names>EL</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The contingent smile: A meta-analysis of sex differences in smiling</article-title>
<source>Psychological Bulletin</source>
<year>2003</year>
<volume>129</volume>
<fpage>305</fpage>
<lpage>334</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.305</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12696842</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR59">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Mayer</surname>
<given-names>JD</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Caruso</surname>
<given-names>DR</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Salovey</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Emotional intelligence meets traditional standards for an intelligence</article-title>
<source>Intelligence</source>
<year>2000</year>
<volume>27</volume>
<fpage>267</fpage>
<lpage>298</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0160-2896(99)00016-1</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR60">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Plant</surname>
<given-names>EA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hyde</surname>
<given-names>JS</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Keltner</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Devine</surname>
<given-names>PG</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The gender stereotyping of emotions</article-title>
<source>Psychology of Women Quarterly</source>
<year>2000</year>
<volume>24</volume>
<fpage>81</fpage>
<lpage>92</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1471-6402.2000.tb01024.x</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR61">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rimé</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Corsini</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Herbette</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Fussell</surname>
<given-names>SR</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Emotion, verbal expression, and the social sharing of emotion</article-title>
<source>The verbal communication of emotions: Interdisciplinary perspectives</source>
<year>2002</year>
<publisher-loc>Mahwah</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Erlbaum</publisher-name>
<fpage>185</fpage>
<lpage>208</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR62">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Robinson</surname>
<given-names>MD</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Johnson</surname>
<given-names>JT</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Is it emotion or is it stress? Gender stereotypes and the perception of subjective experience</article-title>
<source>Sex Roles</source>
<year>1997</year>
<volume>36</volume>
<fpage>235</fpage>
<lpage>258</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/BF02766270</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR63">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Roter</surname>
<given-names>DL</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hall</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Aoki</surname>
<given-names>Y</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Physician gender effects in medical communication: A meta-analytic review</article-title>
<source>Journal of the American Medical Association</source>
<year>2002</year>
<volume>288</volume>
<fpage>756</fpage>
<lpage>764</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1001/jama.288.6.756</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12169083</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR64">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Scherer</surname>
<given-names>KR</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Johnstone</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Klasmeyer</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Davidson</surname>
<given-names>RJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Goldsmith</surname>
<given-names>HH</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Scherer</surname>
<given-names>KR</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Vocal expression of emotion</article-title>
<source>Handbook of affective sciences</source>
<year>2003</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
<fpage>433</fpage>
<lpage>456</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR65">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Shields</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Fischer</surname>
<given-names>AH</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Thinking about gender, thinking about theory: Gender and emotional experience</article-title>
<source>Gender and emotion: Social psychological perspectives</source>
<year>2000</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
<fpage>3</fpage>
<lpage>23</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR66">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Shiota</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Keltner</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>John</surname>
<given-names>O</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Positive emotion dispositions differentially associated with Big Five personality and attachment style</article-title>
<source>The Journal of Positive Psychology</source>
<year>2006</year>
<volume>1</volume>
<fpage>61</fpage>
<lpage>71</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/17439760500510833</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR67">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Simon</surname>
<given-names>RW</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nath</surname>
<given-names>LE</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Gender and emotion in the United States: Do men and women differ in self-reports of feelings and expressive behavior?</article-title>
<source>The American Journal of Sociology</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>109</volume>
<fpage>1137</fpage>
<lpage>1176</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1086/382111</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR68">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>MacLean</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Communicating emotion through a haptic link: Design space and methodology</article-title>
<source>International Journal of Human Computer Studies</source>
<year>2007</year>
<volume>65</volume>
<fpage>376</fpage>
<lpage>387</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.ijhcs.2006.11.006</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR69">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Stier</surname>
<given-names>DS</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hall</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Gender differences in touch: An empirical and theoretical review</article-title>
<source>Journal of Personality and Social Psychology</source>
<year>1984</year>
<volume>47</volume>
<fpage>440</fpage>
<lpage>459</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.47.2.440</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR70">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tiedens</surname>
<given-names>LZ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Leach</surname>
<given-names>CW</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>The social life of emotions</source>
<year>2004</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR71">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Timmers</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fischer</surname>
<given-names>AH</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Manstead</surname>
<given-names>ASR</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Ability versus vulnerability: Beliefs about men’s and women’s emotional behavior</article-title>
<source>Cognition & Emotion</source>
<year>2003</year>
<volume>17</volume>
<fpage>41</fpage>
<lpage>63</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/02699930302277</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR72">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tracy</surname>
<given-names>JL</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Robins</surname>
<given-names>RW</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schriber</surname>
<given-names>RA</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Development of a FACS-verified set of basic and self-conscious emotion expressions</article-title>
<source>Emotion</source>
<year>2009</year>
<volume>9</volume>
<fpage>554</fpage>
<lpage>559</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0015766</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19653779</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR73">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Weiss</surname>
<given-names>SJ</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Measurement of the sensory qualities in tactile interaction</article-title>
<source>Nursing Research</source>
<year>1992</year>
<volume>41</volume>
<fpage>82</fpage>
<lpage>86</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1097/00006199-199203000-00005</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">1549524</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR74">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wood</surname>
<given-names>W</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Eagly</surname>
<given-names>AH</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>A cross-cultural analysis of the behavior of women and men: Implications for the origins of sex differences</article-title>
<source>Psychological Bulletin</source>
<year>2002</year>
<volume>128</volume>
<fpage>699</fpage>
<lpage>727</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-2909.128.5.699</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12206191</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</pmc>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Ticri/CIDE/explor/HapticV1/Data/Pmc/Curation
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 000880 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Curation/biblio.hfd -nk 000880 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Ticri/CIDE
   |area=    HapticV1
   |flux=    Pmc
   |étape=   Curation
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     PMC:3016097
   |texte=   Gender and the Communication of Emotion Via Touch
}}

Pour générer des pages wiki

HfdIndexSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Curation/RBID.i   -Sk "pubmed:21297854" \
       | HfdSelect -Kh $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Curation/biblio.hfd   \
       | NlmPubMed2Wicri -a HapticV1 

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.23.
Data generation: Mon Jun 13 01:09:46 2016. Site generation: Wed Mar 6 09:54:07 2024