Serveur d'exploration sur les dispositifs haptiques

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

Basic principles of sensorimotor adaptation to different distortions with different effectors and movement types: a review and synthesis of behavioral findings

Identifieur interne : 001388 ( Pmc/Checkpoint ); précédent : 001387; suivant : 001389

Basic principles of sensorimotor adaptation to different distortions with different effectors and movement types: a review and synthesis of behavioral findings

Auteurs : Otmar Bock

Source :

RBID : PMC:3596763

Abstract

This article reviews seemingly conflicting behavioral data about sensorimotor adaptation. Some earlier studies assert that one common mechanism exists for multiple distortions, and others that multiple mechanisms exist for one given distortion. Some but not others report that adaptation is direction-selective. Some submit that adaptation transfers across effectors, and others that a single effector can adapt to multiple distortions. A model is proposed to account for all these findings. It stipulates that adaptive mechanisms respond to multiple distortions, consist of directionally tuned special-purpose modules, can be switched in dependence on contextual cues, and are connected to practiced movement types with a higher weight than to unpracticed ones.


Url:
DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00081
PubMed: 23503204
PubMed Central: 3596763


Affiliations:


Links toward previous steps (curation, corpus...)


Links to Exploration step

PMC:3596763

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI>
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">Basic principles of sensorimotor adaptation to different distortions with different effectors and movement types: a review and synthesis of behavioral findings</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, Otmar" sort="Bock, Otmar" uniqKey="Bock O" first="Otmar" last="Bock">Otmar Bock</name>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">PMC</idno>
<idno type="pmid">23503204</idno>
<idno type="pmc">3596763</idno>
<idno type="url">http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3596763</idno>
<idno type="RBID">PMC:3596763</idno>
<idno type="doi">10.3389/fnhum.2013.00081</idno>
<date when="2013">2013</date>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Corpus">001D33</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Curation">001D33</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Checkpoint">001388</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title xml:lang="en" level="a" type="main">Basic principles of sensorimotor adaptation to different distortions with different effectors and movement types: a review and synthesis of behavioral findings</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, Otmar" sort="Bock, Otmar" uniqKey="Bock O" first="Otmar" last="Bock">Otmar Bock</name>
</author>
</analytic>
<series>
<title level="j">Frontiers in Human Neuroscience</title>
<idno type="eISSN">1662-5161</idno>
<imprint>
<date when="2013">2013</date>
</imprint>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">
<p>This article reviews seemingly conflicting behavioral data about sensorimotor adaptation. Some earlier studies assert that one common mechanism exists for multiple distortions, and others that multiple mechanisms exist for one given distortion. Some but not others report that adaptation is direction-selective. Some submit that adaptation transfers across effectors, and others that a single effector can adapt to multiple distortions. A model is proposed to account for all these findings. It stipulates that adaptive mechanisms respond to multiple distortions, consist of directionally tuned special-purpose modules, can be switched in dependence on contextual cues, and are connected to practiced movement types with a higher weight than to unpracticed ones.</p>
</div>
</front>
<back>
<div1 type="bibliography">
<listBibl>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Abeele, S" uniqKey="Abeele S">S. Abeele</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Abeele, S" uniqKey="Abeele S">S. Abeele</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Anguera, J A" uniqKey="Anguera J">J. A. Anguera</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Reuter Lorenz, P A" uniqKey="Reuter Lorenz P">P. A. Reuter-Lorenz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Willingham, D T" uniqKey="Willingham D">D. T. Willingham</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Seidler, R D" uniqKey="Seidler R">R. D. Seidler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Abeele, S" uniqKey="Abeele S">S. Abeele</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Eversheim, U" uniqKey="Eversheim U">U. Eversheim</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bloomberg, J J" uniqKey="Bloomberg J">J. J. Bloomberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weigelt, C" uniqKey="Weigelt C">C. Weigelt</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schmitz, G" uniqKey="Schmitz G">G. Schmitz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thomas, M" uniqKey="Thomas M">M. Thomas</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Worringham, C" uniqKey="Worringham C">C. Worringham</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thomas, M" uniqKey="Thomas M">M. Thomas</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Carl, J R" uniqKey="Carl J">J. R. Carl</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gellman, R S" uniqKey="Gellman R">R. S. Gellman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Clower, D M" uniqKey="Clower D">D. M. Clower</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Boussaoud, D" uniqKey="Boussaoud D">D. Boussaoud</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cotti, J" uniqKey="Cotti J">J. Cotti</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Guillaume, A" uniqKey="Guillaume A">A. Guillaume</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Alahyane, N" uniqKey="Alahyane N">N. Alahyane</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pelisson, D" uniqKey="Pelisson D">D. Pélisson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vercher, J L" uniqKey="Vercher J">J.-L. Vercher</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cunningham, H A" uniqKey="Cunningham H">H. A. Cunningham</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cunningham, H A" uniqKey="Cunningham H">H. A. Cunningham</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Welch, R B" uniqKey="Welch R">R. B. Welch</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Deubel, H" uniqKey="Deubel H">H. Deubel</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gaveau, J" uniqKey="Gaveau J">J. Gaveau</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Paizis, C" uniqKey="Paizis C">C. Paizis</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Berret, B" uniqKey="Berret B">B. Berret</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pozzo, T" uniqKey="Pozzo T">T. Pozzo</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Papaxanthis, C" uniqKey="Papaxanthis C">C. Papaxanthis</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gentilucci, M" uniqKey="Gentilucci M">M. Gentilucci</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Daprati, E" uniqKey="Daprati E">E. Daprati</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Toni, I" uniqKey="Toni I">I. Toni</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chieffi, S" uniqKey="Chieffi S">S. Chieffi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Saetti, M C" uniqKey="Saetti M">M. C. Saetti</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ghahramani, Z" uniqKey="Ghahramani Z">Z. Ghahramani</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wolpert, D M" uniqKey="Wolpert D">D. M. Wolpert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grigorova, V" uniqKey="Grigorova V">V. Grigorova</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ilieva, M" uniqKey="Ilieva M">M. Ilieva</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schmitz, G" uniqKey="Schmitz G">G. Schmitz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kagerer, F A" uniqKey="Kagerer F">F. A. Kagerer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Contreras Vidal, J L" uniqKey="Contreras Vidal J">J. L. Contreras-Vidal</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kohler, I" uniqKey="Kohler I">I. Kohler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Krakauer, J W" uniqKey="Krakauer J">J. W. Krakauer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pine, Z M" uniqKey="Pine Z">Z. M. Pine</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ghilardi, M F" uniqKey="Ghilardi M">M.-F. Ghilardi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ghez, C" uniqKey="Ghez C">C. Ghez</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lackner, J R" uniqKey="Lackner J">J. R. Lackner</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dizio, P" uniqKey="Dizio P">P. DiZio</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lazar, G" uniqKey="Lazar G">G Lazar</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Laer, J" uniqKey="Van Laer J">J. van Laer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lee, J Y" uniqKey="Lee J">J. Y. Lee</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schweighofer, N" uniqKey="Schweighofer N">N. Schweighofer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mclaughlin, S C" uniqKey="Mclaughlin S">S. C. McLaughlin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcnay, E C" uniqKey="Mcnay E">E. C. McNay</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Willingham, D B" uniqKey="Willingham D">D. B. Willingham</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mikaelian, H H" uniqKey="Mikaelian H">H. H. Mikaelian</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mulavara, A P" uniqKey="Mulavara A">A. P. Mulavara</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ruttley, T" uniqKey="Ruttley T">T. Ruttley</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cohen, H S" uniqKey="Cohen H">H. S. Cohen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Peters, B T" uniqKey="Peters B">B. T. Peters</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Miller, C" uniqKey="Miller C">C. Miller</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brady, R" uniqKey="Brady R">R. Brady</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Prablanc, C" uniqKey="Prablanc C">C. Prablanc</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tzavaras, A" uniqKey="Tzavaras A">A. Tzavaras</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jeannerod, M" uniqKey="Jeannerod M">M. Jeannerod</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Redding, G M" uniqKey="Redding G">G. M. Redding</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wallace, B" uniqKey="Wallace B">B. Wallace</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Roby Brami, A" uniqKey="Roby Brami A">A. Roby-Brami</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Burnod, Y" uniqKey="Burnod Y">Y. Burnod</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schmidt, R A" uniqKey="Schmidt R">R. A. Schmidt</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Seidler, R D" uniqKey="Seidler R">R. D. Seidler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bloomberg, J J" uniqKey="Bloomberg J">J. J. Bloomberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stelmach, G E" uniqKey="Stelmach G">G. E. Stelmach</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shadmehr, R" uniqKey="Shadmehr R">R. Shadmehr</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mussa Ivaldi, F A" uniqKey="Mussa Ivaldi F">F. A. Mussa-Ivaldi</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, M A" uniqKey="Smith M">M. A. Smith</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ghazizadeh, A" uniqKey="Ghazizadeh A">A. Ghazizadeh</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shadmehr, R" uniqKey="Shadmehr R">R. Shadmehr</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Snoddy, G S" uniqKey="Snoddy G">G. S. Snoddy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stratton, G M" uniqKey="Stratton G">G. M. Stratton</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tanaka, H" uniqKey="Tanaka H">H. Tanaka</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sejnowski, T J" uniqKey="Sejnowski T">T. J. Sejnowski</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Krakauer, J W" uniqKey="Krakauer J">J. W. Krakauer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thomas, M" uniqKey="Thomas M">M. Thomas</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thomas, M" uniqKey="Thomas M">M. Thomas</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wada, Y" uniqKey="Wada Y">Y. Wada</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kawabata, Y" uniqKey="Kawabata Y">Y. Kawabata</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kotosaka, S" uniqKey="Kotosaka S">S. Kotosaka</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yamamoto, K" uniqKey="Yamamoto K">K. Yamamoto</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kitazawa, S" uniqKey="Kitazawa S">S. Kitazawa</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kawato, M" uniqKey="Kawato M">M. Kawato</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wang, J" uniqKey="Wang J">J. Wang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sainburg, R L" uniqKey="Sainburg R">R. L. Sainburg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wang, J" uniqKey="Wang J">J. Wang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sainburg, R L" uniqKey="Sainburg R">R. L. Sainburg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weigelt, C" uniqKey="Weigelt C">C. Weigelt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weigelt, C" uniqKey="Weigelt C">C. Weigelt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Werner, S" uniqKey="Werner S">S. Werner</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Werner, S" uniqKey="Werner S">S. Werner</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bock, O" uniqKey="Bock O">O. Bock</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wigmore, V" uniqKey="Wigmore V">V. Wigmore</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tong, C" uniqKey="Tong C">C. Tong</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Flanagan, J R" uniqKey="Flanagan J">J. R. Flanagan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wolpert, D M" uniqKey="Wolpert D">D. M. Wolpert</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kawato, M" uniqKey="Kawato M">M. Kawato</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Woolley, D G" uniqKey="Woolley D">D. G. Woolley</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tresilian, J R" uniqKey="Tresilian J">J. R. Tresilian</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Carson, R G" uniqKey="Carson R">R. G. Carson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rick, S" uniqKey="Rick S">S. Rick</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
</listBibl>
</div1>
</back>
</TEI>
<pmc article-type="research-article">
<pmc-dir>properties open_access</pmc-dir>
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="nlm-ta">Front Hum Neurosci</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="iso-abbrev">Front Hum Neurosci</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="publisher-id">Front. Hum. Neurosci.</journal-id>
<journal-title-group>
<journal-title>Frontiers in Human Neuroscience</journal-title>
</journal-title-group>
<issn pub-type="epub">1662-5161</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Frontiers Media S.A.</publisher-name>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmid">23503204</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmc">3596763</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3389/fnhum.2013.00081</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Neuroscience</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Mini Review Article</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>Basic principles of sensorimotor adaptation to different distortions with different effectors and movement types: a review and synthesis of behavioral findings</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>Otmar</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="author-notes" rid="fn001">
<sup>*</sup>
</xref>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<aff>
<institution>Institute of Physiology and Anatomy, German Sport University</institution>
<country>Köln, Germany</country>
</aff>
<author-notes>
<fn fn-type="edited-by">
<p>Edited by:
<italic>Rachael D. Seidler, University of Michigan, USA</italic>
</p>
</fn>
<fn fn-type="edited-by">
<p>Reviewed by:
<italic>Herbert Heuer, Leibniz Research Centre for Working Environment and Human Factors, Germany; Lauren E. Sergio, York University, Canada</italic>
</p>
</fn>
<corresp id="fn001">*Correspondence:
<italic>Otmar Bock, Institute of Physiology and Anatomy, German Sport University, Am Sportpark Müngersdorf 6, 50933 Köln, Germany. e-mail:
<email xlink:type="simple">bock@dshs-koeln.de</email>
</italic>
</corresp>
</author-notes>
<pub-date pub-type="epub">
<day>14</day>
<month>3</month>
<year>2013</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="collection">
<year>2013</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>7</volume>
<elocation-id>81</elocation-id>
<history>
<date date-type="received">
<day>02</day>
<month>11</month>
<year>2012</year>
</date>
<date date-type="accepted">
<day>28</day>
<month>2</month>
<year>2013</year>
</date>
</history>
<permissions>
<copyright-statement>Copyright © Bock.</copyright-statement>
<copyright-year>2013</copyright-year>
<license license-type="open-access" xlink:href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/">
<license-p> This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.</license-p>
</license>
</permissions>
<abstract>
<p>This article reviews seemingly conflicting behavioral data about sensorimotor adaptation. Some earlier studies assert that one common mechanism exists for multiple distortions, and others that multiple mechanisms exist for one given distortion. Some but not others report that adaptation is direction-selective. Some submit that adaptation transfers across effectors, and others that a single effector can adapt to multiple distortions. A model is proposed to account for all these findings. It stipulates that adaptive mechanisms respond to multiple distortions, consist of directionally tuned special-purpose modules, can be switched in dependence on contextual cues, and are connected to practiced movement types with a higher weight than to unpracticed ones.</p>
</abstract>
<kwd-group>
<kwd>motor learning</kwd>
<kwd>plasticity</kwd>
<kwd>context-dependence</kwd>
<kwd>transfer</kwd>
<kwd>multiple adaptation</kwd>
</kwd-group>
<counts>
<fig-count count="1"></fig-count>
<table-count count="0"></table-count>
<equation-count count="0"></equation-count>
<ref-count count="54"></ref-count>
<page-count count="5"></page-count>
<word-count count="0"></word-count>
</counts>
</article-meta>
</front>
<body>
<p>Human sensorimotor adaptation has been evaluated with a baffling number of experimental paradigms. Subjects were exposed to distortions of visual (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B41">Stratton, 1897</xref>
), acoustic (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B31">Mikaelian, 1974</xref>
) and proprioceptive inputs (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B26">Lackner and DiZio, 1994</xref>
), to topographical (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B24">Kohler, 1955</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B17">Cunningham and Welch, 1994</xref>
) and to dynamical distortions (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B38">Shadmehr and Mussa-Ivaldi, 1994</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B5">Bock, 2003</xref>
), to distortions experienced while tracking (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B17">Cunningham and Welch, 1994</xref>
), pointing (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B31">Mikaelian, 1974</xref>
) or grasping with the hand (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B20">Gentilucci et al., 1995</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B48">Weigelt and Bock, 2007</xref>
), while executing pursuit eye movements (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B13">Carl and Gellman, 1986</xref>
), reflexive (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B29">McLaughlin, 1967</xref>
) or volitional saccades (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B18">Deubel, 1995</xref>
). Given this wealth of paradigms, it seems reasonable to question whether all authors dealt with the same phenomenon: is all adaptation achieved by one common mechanism, or rather by multiple mechanisms, each specific for a given paradigm?</p>
<p>This question has been addressed in behavioral studies by testing for the transfer of adaptation from one visual rotation to another, or from one lateral shift to another. This work invariably found that subjects started under the second distortion with the behavior they acquired under the first, and then gradually modified it until it became adequate for the second distortion; as a consequence, they performed better than novices when the second distortion was
<italic>larger</italic>
than the first, but worse than novices when the second distortion was
<italic>opposite</italic>
to the first (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B27">Lazar and van Laer, 1968</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B52">Wigmore et al., 2002</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B8">Bock et al., 2003</xref>
). Thus transfer was compulsory, occurring even where it degraded performance. Other work found compulsory transfer even between distortions of a different type, i.e., between a visual rotation and a visual velocity-dependent lateral shift (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B43">Thomas and Bock, 2010</xref>
), between a visual rotation and a force field (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B11">Bock and Thomas, 1999</xref>
), and between a visual and an acoustic rotation (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B23">Kagerer and Contreras-Vidal, 2009</xref>
). In those studies, performance benefits again emerged when both distortions were of equal sign, and costs when they were of opposite sign. Taken together, these findings suggest that adaptation to a wide range of distortions might be based on a common mechanism; this is illustrated in
<bold>Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F1">1A</xref>
</bold>
, where a universal adaptive mechanism receives sensory inputs
<italic>I</italic>
<sub>j</sub>
from different sensory modalities distorted in different ways, and sends motor outputs
<italic>O</italic>
<sub>k</sub>
to different effectors executing different types of movement.</p>
<fig id="F1" position="float">
<label>FIGURE 1</label>
<caption>
<p>
<bold>Tentative models of sensorimotor adaptation</bold>
.
<bold>(A)</bold>
Model of an adaptive mechanism that receives inputs
<italic>I</italic>
<sub>k</sub>
from different sensory modalities distorted in different ways, and sends outputs
<italic>O</italic>
<sub>k</sub>
to different effectors executing different types of movement.
<bold>(B)</bold>
More elaborate model that includes functional modules
<italic>G</italic>
<sub>i</sub>
for gradual changes,
<italic>X</italic>
<sub>i</sub>
for axis inversions, and S for scaling; modules
<italic>G</italic>
<sub>i</sub>
and
<italic>X</italic>
<sub>i</sub>
are laid out in parallel, each being tuned to a limited range of target directions.
<bold>(C)</bold>
Final model that includes multiple mechanisms linked to the motor output by a context-dependent switch, and weighting factors that are higher for practiced than for unpracticed effectors and movement types.</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="fnhum-07-00081-g001"></graphic>
</fig>
<p>Other findings have refined this view by indicating that the proposed universal mechanism can be subdivided into several functionally specialized modules. Thus, subjects exposed to different visual rotations perform less and less well as the magnitude of rotation increases toward 90°, but improve again as rotation continues to increase from 90° toward 180°; in fact, performance under a 180° rotation is not dramatically poorer than under no rotation (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B16">Cunningham, 1989</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B1">Abeele and Bock, 2001</xref>
). Furthermore, subjects exposed to a rotation of more than 90° quickly change their response direction by 180° and then gradually change it “back” toward the required angle (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B8">Bock et al., 2003</xref>
). These findings call for the existence of two functional modules, one that gradually changes spatial coordinates by up to 90°, and a second one that quickly changes them by 180°; the latter module possibly exploits the mathematical equivalence between a 180° rotation and an inversion of the horizontal and vertical axis.</p>
<p>Further work suggests that the presumed gradual-change modules are selective to only a limited range of movement directions around the practiced direction (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">Krakauer et al., 2000</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B47">Wang and Sainburg, 2005</xref>
). This range can be estimated from published data as 45° (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B42">Tanaka et al., 2009</xref>
) to 80° (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B35">Roby-Brami and Burnod, 1995</xref>
), which fits well with the finding that adaptation shows only modest signs of interference when eight targets, located 45° apart, are associated with different rotational transformations (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B51">Werner and Bock, 2010</xref>
). We posit that the axis-inversion modules are direction-selective as well, i.e., they operate only for movement directions similar to the trained ones; however, this issue has not been addressed experimentally yet. In contrast, adaptation to a new scaling factor seems not to be directionally tuned: adaptation of one movement direction transfers obligatorily to the full 360° range of possible directions (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B4">Bock, 1992</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">Krakauer et al., 2000</xref>
).
<bold>Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F1">1B</xref>
</bold>
therefore depicts an adaptive mechanism that responds to multiple distortions with a number of special-purpose modules: several directionally tuned ones for gradual changes of direction (
<italic>G</italic>
<sub>i</sub>
), several directionally tuned ones for axis inversions (
<italic>X</italic>
<sub>j</sub>
), and a single one for scaling (S). This layout correctly predicts the obligatory transfer between distortions, the concurrence of quick and gradual changes under one given distortion, and the distinct adaptation characteristics with rotations and scalings.</p>
<p>The interplay of special-purpose modules such as those in
<bold>Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F1">1B</xref>
</bold>
can be readily illustrated with available data on the adaptation to mirror-reversed vision. This distortion initiates quick 180° changes of response directions for targets presented at the left and right, quick 180° changes followed by gradual 90° clockwise changes for targets along the right diagonal, quick 180° changes followed by gradual 90° counter-clockwise changes for targets along the left diagonal, and only a transient increase of response variability for targets at the top and bottom (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B51">Werner and Bock, 2010</xref>
). This pattern of findings can be easily explained by the model in
<bold>Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F1">1B</xref>
</bold>
: targets at the left, right, and along either diagonal activate the corresponding directionally tuned axis-inversion modules, and targets along the diagonals additionally activate the corresponding gradual-change modules. Note that such an interpretation puts the minimum number of gradual-change modules to eight: the distortion activates four modules tuned to the diagonal directions, and has no effect on four modules tuned to the interleaved orthogonal directions. As noted above, this number of modules fits well with their reported tuning width of 40–80°, since 360/8 = 45. Similarly, the minimum number of axis-inversion modules seems to be 4: the distortion activates modules at the right and left, but not those at the top and bottom. For reasons of parsimony, one might therefore postulate eight gradual-change and four axis-inversion modules, but for reasons of symmetry, one might postulate eight modules of either type. Further research is needed to resolve this issue.</p>
<p>Adaptation to a given distortion does not transfer well to unpracticed movement types. A moderate transfer was observed between manual tracking and pointing (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B2">Abeele and Bock, 2003</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B6">Bock, 2005</xref>
), grasping and pointing (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B49">Weigelt and Bock, 2010</xref>
), as well as volitional saccades and pointing (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B15">Cotti et al., 2007</xref>
), but no transfer was found between reactive and volitional saccades (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B18">Deubel, 1995</xref>
), nor between reactive saccades and pointing (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B15">Cotti et al., 2007</xref>
). Transfer between the two arms varied widely between studies and seems not to be obligatory, since both arms can concurrently adapt to opposite visual rotations with no sign of interference (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B33">Prablanc et al., 1975</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B46">Wang and Sainburg, 2003</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B12">Bock et al., 2005</xref>
). Similarly, manual pointing and reactive saccades can concurrently adapt to two opposite distortions with only moderate interference (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B22">Grigorova et al., 2013</xref>
). It even has been shown that one single arm, pointing at a single set of targets, can concurrently adapt to two opposite distortions if they are coded by contextual cues such as hemi-workspace (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B21">Ghahramani and Wolpert, 1997</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B54">Woolley et al., 2007</xref>
), head position (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B37">Seidler et al., 2001</xref>
), or screen color (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B45">Wada et al., 2003</xref>
). In fact, subjects can adapt with no noticeable interference to as many as
<italic>four</italic>
distortions, each coded by a unique combination of arm and hemi-workspace (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B44">Thomas and Bock, 2012</xref>
). Even when contextual cues are not available, subjects can use a “probing” movement to find out whether a previously established adaptive change should be preserved or rather abandoned (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B46">Wang and Sainburg, 2003</xref>
). To account for these findings,
<bold>Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F1">1C</xref>
</bold>
shows four distinct multi-distortion mechanisms that can be alternately connected to the motor output via a context-dependent switch; the signal is then weighted, with the trained effector and movement type receiving the highest weight.</p>
<p>A model of sensorimotor adaptation, consisting of multiple mechanisms that are selectable by context, has been proposed before (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B21">Ghahramani and Wolpert, 1997</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B53">Wolpert and Kawato, 1998</xref>
). The present article refines this model by adding multi-distortion sensitivity, special-purpose modules, directional tuning, and output weighting. The available database provides robust evidence for the existence of these key characteristics of adaptation, but future experimental findings may require an increase in the number of adaptive mechanisms and/or special-purpose modules. Additional research is also desirable to find out whether adaptive mechanisms are truly universal, i.e., respond to any conceivable type of distortion, and to determine the actual tuning widths of modules and weights of outputs. This would allow a quantitative rather than qualitative comparison of experimental data with model predictions.</p>
<p>The model in
<bold>Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F1">1C</xref>
</bold>
was designed to illustrate the known functional characteristics of adaptation; it was not meant to show the actual anatomical layout of the underlying neuronal circuitry. In fact, given the preponderance of parallel distributed processing in the brain, it is quite likely that the depicted modules and mechanisms are implemented within a highly interconnected neural network with only a limited topographical segregation. In a way, the model in
<bold>Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F1">1C</xref>
</bold>
could be interpreted as a specific version of schema theory, which posits that movements are executed by tailoring a generalized motor program to the needs of a specific movement (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B36">Schmidt, 1975</xref>
).</p>
<p>As complex as it is, the model proposed in
<bold>Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F1">1C</xref>
</bold>
still disregards two crucial aspects of sensorimotor adaptation. One of them is the existence of multiple time scales. Gradual rotation proceeds with a time constant τ
<sub>1</sub>
in the order of several movements, and a second one with a time constant τ
<sub>2</sub>
in the order of several tens of movements (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B40">Snoddy, 1926</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B39">Smith et al., 2006</xref>
); additional time scales in the order of days to months have been reported by classical accounts (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B41">Stratton, 1897</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B24">Kohler, 1955</xref>
) and by recent spaceflight studies (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B9">Bock et al., 2010</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B19">Gaveau et al., 2011</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B32">Mulavara et al., 2012</xref>
). Since the model in
<bold>Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F1">1C</xref>
</bold>
is mainly based on findings about long-term adaptation, it most likely represents the τ
<sub>2</sub>
component. Little is known about the characteristics of the τ
<sub>1</sub>
component, except that it acts in parallel rather than in series to τ
<sub>2</sub>
(
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B28">Lee and Schweighofer, 2009</xref>
), requires working-memory resources (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B3">Anguera et al., 2010</xref>
), is context-independent (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B28">Lee and Schweighofer, 2009</xref>
) and exhibits its own distinctive directional tuning (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B10">Bock and Schmitz, 2011</xref>
). It still is unknown whether axis inversion and scaling also proceeds along multiple time scales.</p>
<p>The second neglected aspect is the contribution of strategies. Exposure to a distortion initiates not only the adaptive recalibration of sensorimotor pathways, but also the use of workaround strategies such as cognitive reinterpretations of sensory signals, anticipations, associative stimulus–response learning, postural changes, and error-based corrections (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B34">Redding and Wallace, 1996</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B30">McNay and Willingham, 1998</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B14">Clower and Boussaoud, 2000</xref>
). These strategies are thought to be situation-specific and short-lived, and thus to modify performance during exposure to a distortion, but not after removal of the distortion or after transfer to a new movement type. Evidence for the role of strategies is therefore largely based on the dissociated effects of higher-order mental functions on subjects’ performance
<italic>during</italic>
but not
<italic>after</italic>
exposure, e.g., the effects of aging (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B30">McNay and Willingham, 1998</xref>
;
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B6">Bock, 2005</xref>
), emotional state (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B7">Bock, 2010</xref>
), and explicit knowledge (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B50">Werner and Bock, 2007</xref>
).</p>
<p>Summing up,
<bold>Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F1">1C</xref>
</bold>
presents a model for the slow component of adaptive recalibration that accounts for a wide range of seemingly contradictory behavioral phenomena: compulsory versus partial versus null transfer, common mechanism for multiple distortions versus multiple mechanisms for one distortion, presence versus absence of direction-selectivity, and eye–arm transfer versus multiple adaptation of a single arm. Additional experiments are needed to verify the model, determine its parameter values, and possibly add further functional details.</p>
<sec>
<title>Conflict of Interest Statement</title>
<p>The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.</p>
</sec>
</body>
<back>
<ref-list>
<title>REFERENCES</title>
<ref id="B1">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Abeele</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input: different mechanisms for small versus large rotations.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Exp. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>140</volume>
<fpage>407</fpage>
<lpage>410</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11685393</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B2">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Abeele</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Transfer of sensorimotor adaptation between different movement categories.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Exp. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>148</volume>
<fpage>128</fpage>
<lpage>132</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12478403</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B3">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Anguera</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Reuter-Lorenz</surname>
<given-names>P. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Willingham</surname>
<given-names>D. T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Seidler</surname>
<given-names>R. D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Contributions of spatial working memory to visuomotor learning.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Cogn. Neurosci.</italic>
</source>
<volume>22</volume>
<fpage>1917</fpage>
<lpage>1930</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19803691</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B4">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1992</year>
).
<article-title>Adaptation of aimed arm movements to sensorimotor discordance: evidence for direction-independent gain control.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Behav. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>51</volume>
<fpage>41</fpage>
<lpage>50</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">1482544</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B5">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Sensorimotor adaptation to visual distortions with different kinematic coupling.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Exp. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>151</volume>
<fpage>557</fpage>
<lpage>560</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12856094</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B6">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Components of sensorimotor adaptation in young and elderly subjects.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Exp. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>160</volume>
<fpage>259</fpage>
<lpage>263</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15565436</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B7">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Sensorimotor adaptation is influenced by background music.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Exp. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>203</volume>
<fpage>737</fpage>
<lpage>741</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20480363</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B8">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Abeele</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Eversheim</surname>
<given-names>U.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Human adaptation to rotated vision: interplay of a continuous and a discrete process.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Exp. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>152</volume>
<fpage>528</fpage>
<lpage>532</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14513189</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B9">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bloomberg</surname>
<given-names>J. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Weigelt</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Cognitive demand of human sensorimotor performance during an extended space mission: a dual-task study.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Aviat. Space Environ. Med.</italic>
</source>
<volume>81</volume>
<fpage>819</fpage>
<lpage>824</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20824987</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B10">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schmitz</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Adaptation to rotated visual feedback depends on the number and spread of target directions.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Exp. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>209</volume>
<fpage>409</fpage>
<lpage>413</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21279631</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B11">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thomas</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<article-title>“Sensorimotor adaptation to visual and mechanical perturbations is governed by a common mechanism.”</article-title>
<publisher-name>FENS Meeting</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>Wien</publisher-loc>
<issue>1999</issue>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B12">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Worringham</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thomas</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Concurrent adaptations of left and right arms to opposite visual distortions.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Exp. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>162</volume>
<fpage>513</fpage>
<lpage>519</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15754178</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B13">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Carl</surname>
<given-names>J. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gellman</surname>
<given-names>R. S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1986</year>
).
<article-title>“Adaptive responses in human smooth pursuit,” in</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Adaptive Processes in Visual and Oculomotor Systems</italic>
</source>
<role>eds</role>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Keller</surname>
<given-names>E. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zee</surname>
<given-names>D. S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-name>Oxford:</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>Pergamon Press</publisher-loc>
)
<fpage>335</fpage>
<lpage>339</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B14">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Clower</surname>
<given-names>D. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Boussaoud</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Selective use of perceptual recalibration versus visuomotor skill acquisition.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Neurophysiol.</italic>
</source>
<volume>84</volume>
<fpage>2703</fpage>
<lpage>2708</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11068013</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B15">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cotti</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Guillaume</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Alahyane</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pélisson</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Vercher</surname>
<given-names>J.-L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Adaptation of voluntary saccades, but not of reactive saccades, transfers to hand pointing movements.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Neurophysiol.</italic>
</source>
<volume>98</volume>
<fpage>602</fpage>
<lpage>612</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17553949</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B16">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cunningham</surname>
<given-names>H. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1989</year>
).
<article-title>Aiming error under transformed spatial mappings suggests a structure for visual-motor maps.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.</italic>
</source>
<volume>15</volume>
<fpage>493</fpage>
<lpage>506</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">2527958</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B17">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cunningham</surname>
<given-names>H. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Welch</surname>
<given-names>R. B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>Multiple concurrent visual-motor mappings: implication for models of adaptation.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Exp. Psychol.</italic>
</source>
<volume>20</volume>
<fpage>987</fpage>
<lpage>999</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B18">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Deubel</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>Separate adaptive mechanisms for the control of reactive and volitional saccadic eye movements.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Vis. Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>35</volume>
<fpage>3529</fpage>
<lpage>3540</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8560817</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B19">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gaveau</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Paizis</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Berret</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pozzo</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Papaxanthis</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Sensorimotor adaptation of point-to-point arm movements after spaceflight: the role of internal representation of gravity force in trajectory planning.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Neurophysiol.</italic>
</source>
<volume>106</volume>
<fpage>620</fpage>
<lpage>629</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21562193</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B20">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gentilucci</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Daprati</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Toni</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Chieffi</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Saetti</surname>
<given-names>M. C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>Unconscious updating of grasp motor program.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Exp. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>105</volume>
<fpage>291</fpage>
<lpage>303</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">7498382</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B21">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ghahramani</surname>
<given-names>Z.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wolpert</surname>
<given-names>D. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1997</year>
).
<article-title>Modular decomposition in visuomotor learning.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Nature</italic>
</source>
<volume>386</volume>
<fpage>392</fpage>
<lpage>395</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">9121554</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B22">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Grigorova</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ilieva</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schmitz</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Directional adaptation of reactive saccades and of hand pointing movements is not independent.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Motor Behav.</italic>
</source>
<volume>45</volume>
<fpage>101</fpage>
<lpage>106</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B23">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kagerer</surname>
<given-names>F. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Contreras-Vidal</surname>
<given-names>J. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Adaptation of sound localization induced by rotated visual feedback in reaching movements.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Exp. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>193</volume>
<fpage>315</fpage>
<lpage>321</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19048242</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B24">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kohler</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1955</year>
).
<article-title>Experiments with prolonged optical distortions.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Acta Psychol.</italic>
</source>
<volume>11</volume>
<fpage>176</fpage>
<lpage>178</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B25">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Krakauer</surname>
<given-names>J. W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pine</surname>
<given-names>Z. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ghilardi</surname>
<given-names>M.-F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ghez</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Learning of visuomotor transformations for vectorial planning of reaching trajectories.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Neurosci.</italic>
</source>
<volume>20</volume>
<fpage>8916</fpage>
<lpage>8924</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11102502</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B26">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lackner</surname>
<given-names>J. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>DiZio</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>Rapid adaptation to coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Neurophysiol.</italic>
</source>
<volume>72</volume>
<fpage>299</fpage>
<lpage>313</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">7965013</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B27">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lazar</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>van Laer</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1968</year>
).
<article-title>Adaptation to displaced vision after experience with lesser displacements.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Percept. Motor Skills</italic>
</source>
<volume>26</volume>
<fpage>579</fpage>
<lpage>582</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">5654890</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B28">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lee</surname>
<given-names>J. Y.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schweighofer</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Dual adaptation supports a parallel architecture of motor memory.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Neurosci.</italic>
</source>
<volume>29</volume>
<fpage>10396</fpage>
<lpage>10404</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19692614</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B29">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>McLaughlin</surname>
<given-names>S. C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1967</year>
).
<article-title>Parametric adjustment in saccadic eye movements.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Percept. Psychophys.</italic>
</source>
<volume>2</volume>
<fpage>359</fpage>
<lpage>362</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B30">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>McNay</surname>
<given-names>E. C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Willingham</surname>
<given-names>D. B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1998</year>
).
<article-title>Deficit in learning of a motor skill requiring strategy, but not of perceptual motor recalibration, with aging.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Learn. Mem.</italic>
</source>
<volume>4</volume>
<fpage>411</fpage>
<lpage>420</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10701880</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B31">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Mikaelian</surname>
<given-names>H. H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1974</year>
).
<article-title>Adaptation to displaced hearing: a nonproprioceptive change.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Exp. Psychol.</italic>
</source>
<volume>103</volume>
<fpage>326</fpage>
<lpage>330</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B32">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Mulavara</surname>
<given-names>A. P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ruttley</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cohen</surname>
<given-names>H. S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Peters</surname>
<given-names>B. T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Miller</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brady</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Vestibular-somatosensory convergence in head movement control during locomotion after long-duration space flight.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Vestib. Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>22</volume>
<fpage>153</fpage>
<lpage>166</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23000615</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B33">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Prablanc</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tzavaras</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jeannerod</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1975</year>
).
<article-title>Adaptation of the two arms to opposite prism displacements.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Q. J. Exp. Psychol.</italic>
</source>
<volume>27</volume>
<fpage>667</fpage>
<lpage>671</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B34">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Redding</surname>
<given-names>G. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wallace</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1996</year>
).
<article-title>Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.</italic>
</source>
<volume>22</volume>
<fpage>379</fpage>
<lpage>394</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8934851</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B35">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Roby-Brami</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Burnod</surname>
<given-names>Y.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>Learning a new visuomotor transformation: error correction and generalization.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Cogn. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>2</volume>
<fpage>229</fpage>
<lpage>242</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B36">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schmidt</surname>
<given-names>R. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1975</year>
).
<article-title>A schema theory of motor learning.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Psychol. Rev.</italic>
</source>
<volume>82</volume>
<fpage>225</fpage>
<lpage>260</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B37">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Seidler</surname>
<given-names>R. D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bloomberg</surname>
<given-names>J. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Stelmach</surname>
<given-names>G. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Context-dependent arm pointing adaptation.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Behav. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>119</volume>
<fpage>155</fpage>
<lpage>166</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11165331</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B38">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Shadmehr</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mussa-Ivaldi</surname>
<given-names>F. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Neurosci.</italic>
</source>
<volume>14</volume>
<fpage>3208</fpage>
<lpage>3224</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8182467</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B39">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>M. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ghazizadeh</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Shadmehr</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Interacting adaptive processes with different timescales underlie short-term motor learning.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>PLoS Biol.</italic>
</source>
<volume>4:</volume>
<issue>e179</issue>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pbio.0040179</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B40">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Snoddy</surname>
<given-names>G. S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1926</year>
).
<article-title>Learning and stability.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Appl. Psychol.</italic>
</source>
<volume>10</volume>
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>36</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B41">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Stratton</surname>
<given-names>G. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1897</year>
).
<article-title>Vision without inversion of the retinal image.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Psychol. Rev.</italic>
</source>
<volume>4</volume>
<fpage>341</fpage>
<lpage>481</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B42">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tanaka</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sejnowski</surname>
<given-names>T. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Krakauer</surname>
<given-names>J. W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Adaptation to visuomotor rotation through interaction between posterior parietal and motor cortical areas.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Neurophysiol.</italic>
</source>
<volume>102</volume>
<fpage>2921</fpage>
<lpage>2932</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19741098</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B43">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Thomas</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Is sensorimotor adaptation to position- and velocity-dependent visual distortions based on distinct adaptive processes?</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Hum. Mov. Sci.</italic>
</source>
<volume>29</volume>
<fpage>179</fpage>
<lpage>186</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20338653</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B44">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Thomas</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Concurrent adaptation to four different visual rotations.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Exp. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>221</volume>
<fpage>85</fpage>
<lpage>91</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22777101</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B45">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wada</surname>
<given-names>Y.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kawabata</surname>
<given-names>Y.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kotosaka</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Yamamoto</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kitazawa</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kawato</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Acquisition and contextual switching of multiple internal models for different viscous force fields.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Neurosci. Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>46</volume>
<fpage>319</fpage>
<lpage>331</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12804793</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B46">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wang</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sainburg</surname>
<given-names>R. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Mechanisms underlying interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Exp. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>149</volume>
<fpage>520</fpage>
<lpage>526</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12677333</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B47">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wang</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sainburg</surname>
<given-names>R. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Adaptation to visuomotor rotations remaps movement vectors, not final positions.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Neurosci.</italic>
</source>
<volume>25</volume>
<fpage>4024</fpage>
<lpage>4030</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15843604</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B48">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Weigelt</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Adaptation of grasping responses to distorted object size and orientation.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Exp. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>181</volume>
<fpage>139</fpage>
<lpage>146</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17333005</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B49">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Weigelt</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Adaptation of the precision grip orientation to a visual–haptic mismatch.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Exp. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>201</volume>
<fpage>621</fpage>
<lpage>630</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20012539</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B50">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Werner</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Effects of variable practice and declarative knowledge on sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual feedback.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Exp. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>178</volume>
<fpage>554</fpage>
<lpage>559</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17361424</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B51">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Werner</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bock</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Mechanisms for visuomotor adaptation to left–right reversed vision.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Hum. Mov. Sci.</italic>
</source>
<volume>29</volume>
<fpage>172</fpage>
<lpage>178</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20304517</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B52">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wigmore</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tong</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Flanagan</surname>
<given-names>J. R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Visuomotor rotations of varying size and direction compete for single internal model in working memory.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.</italic>
</source>
<volume>28</volume>
<fpage>447</fpage>
<lpage>457</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11999865</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B53">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wolpert</surname>
<given-names>D. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kawato</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1998</year>
).
<article-title>Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Neural Netw.</italic>
</source>
<volume>11</volume>
<fpage>1317</fpage>
<lpage>1329</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12662752</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B54">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Woolley</surname>
<given-names>D. G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tresilian</surname>
<given-names>J. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Carson</surname>
<given-names>R. G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rick</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Dual adaptation to two opposing visuomotor rotations when each is associated with different regions of workspace.</article-title>
<source>
<italic>Exp. Brain Res.</italic>
</source>
<volume>179</volume>
<fpage>155</fpage>
<lpage>165</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17119942</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</pmc>
<affiliations>
<list></list>
<tree>
<noCountry>
<name sortKey="Bock, Otmar" sort="Bock, Otmar" uniqKey="Bock O" first="Otmar" last="Bock">Otmar Bock</name>
</noCountry>
</tree>
</affiliations>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Ticri/CIDE/explor/HapticV1/Data/Pmc/Checkpoint
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 001388 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Checkpoint/biblio.hfd -nk 001388 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Ticri/CIDE
   |area=    HapticV1
   |flux=    Pmc
   |étape=   Checkpoint
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     PMC:3596763
   |texte=   Basic principles of sensorimotor adaptation to different distortions with different effectors and movement types: a review and synthesis of behavioral findings
}}

Pour générer des pages wiki

HfdIndexSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Checkpoint/RBID.i   -Sk "pubmed:23503204" \
       | HfdSelect -Kh $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Checkpoint/biblio.hfd   \
       | NlmPubMed2Wicri -a HapticV1 

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.23.
Data generation: Mon Jun 13 01:09:46 2016. Site generation: Wed Mar 6 09:54:07 2024