Serveur d'exploration sur les dispositifs haptiques

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

Routes to embodiment

Identifieur interne : 000357 ( Pmc/Checkpoint ); précédent : 000356; suivant : 000358

Routes to embodiment

Auteurs : Anita Körner [Allemagne] ; Sascha Topolinski [Allemagne] ; Fritz Strack [Allemagne]

Source :

RBID : PMC:4488596

Abstract

Research on embodiment is rich in impressive demonstrations but somewhat poor in comprehensive explanations. Although some moderators and driving mechanisms have been identified, a comprehensive conceptual account of how bodily states or dynamics influence behavior is still missing. Here, we attempt to integrate current knowledge by describing three basic psychological mechanisms: direct state induction, which influences how humans feel or process information, unmediated by any other cognitive mechanism; modal priming, which changes the accessibility of concepts associated with a bodily state; sensorimotor simulation, which affects the ease with which congruent and incongruent actions are performed. We argue that the joint impact of these mechanisms can account for most existing embodiment effects. Additionally, we summarize empirical tests for distinguishing these mechanisms and suggest a guideline for future research about the mechanisms underlying embodiment effects.


Url:
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00940
PubMed: 26191033
PubMed Central: 4488596


Affiliations:


Links toward previous steps (curation, corpus...)


Links to Exploration step

PMC:4488596

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI>
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">Routes to embodiment</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Korner, Anita" sort="Korner, Anita" uniqKey="Korner A" first="Anita" last="Körner">Anita Körner</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="aff1">
<institution>Department of Psychology II, University of Würzburg</institution>
,
<country>Würzburg, Germany</country>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea></wicri:regionArea>
<wicri:regionArea># see nlm:aff region in country</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Topolinski, Sascha" sort="Topolinski, Sascha" uniqKey="Topolinski S" first="Sascha" last="Topolinski">Sascha Topolinski</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="aff2">
<institution>Social and Economic Cognition, Department of Psychology, University of Cologne</institution>
,
<country>Cologne, Germany</country>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea></wicri:regionArea>
<wicri:regionArea># see nlm:aff region in country</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Strack, Fritz" sort="Strack, Fritz" uniqKey="Strack F" first="Fritz" last="Strack">Fritz Strack</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="aff1">
<institution>Department of Psychology II, University of Würzburg</institution>
,
<country>Würzburg, Germany</country>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea></wicri:regionArea>
<wicri:regionArea># see nlm:aff region in country</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">PMC</idno>
<idno type="pmid">26191033</idno>
<idno type="pmc">4488596</idno>
<idno type="url">http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4488596</idno>
<idno type="RBID">PMC:4488596</idno>
<idno type="doi">10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00940</idno>
<date when="2015">2015</date>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Corpus">000201</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Curation">000201</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Checkpoint">000357</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title xml:lang="en" level="a" type="main">Routes to embodiment</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Korner, Anita" sort="Korner, Anita" uniqKey="Korner A" first="Anita" last="Körner">Anita Körner</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="aff1">
<institution>Department of Psychology II, University of Würzburg</institution>
,
<country>Würzburg, Germany</country>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea></wicri:regionArea>
<wicri:regionArea># see nlm:aff region in country</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Topolinski, Sascha" sort="Topolinski, Sascha" uniqKey="Topolinski S" first="Sascha" last="Topolinski">Sascha Topolinski</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="aff2">
<institution>Social and Economic Cognition, Department of Psychology, University of Cologne</institution>
,
<country>Cologne, Germany</country>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea></wicri:regionArea>
<wicri:regionArea># see nlm:aff region in country</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Strack, Fritz" sort="Strack, Fritz" uniqKey="Strack F" first="Fritz" last="Strack">Fritz Strack</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="aff1">
<institution>Department of Psychology II, University of Würzburg</institution>
,
<country>Würzburg, Germany</country>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea></wicri:regionArea>
<wicri:regionArea># see nlm:aff region in country</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<series>
<title level="j">Frontiers in Psychology</title>
<idno type="eISSN">1664-1078</idno>
<imprint>
<date when="2015">2015</date>
</imprint>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">
<p>Research on embodiment is rich in impressive demonstrations but somewhat poor in comprehensive explanations. Although some moderators and driving mechanisms have been identified, a comprehensive conceptual account of how bodily states or dynamics influence behavior is still missing. Here, we attempt to integrate current knowledge by describing three basic psychological mechanisms:
<italic>direct state induction</italic>
, which influences how humans feel or process information, unmediated by any other cognitive mechanism;
<italic>modal priming</italic>
, which changes the accessibility of concepts associated with a bodily state;
<italic>sensorimotor simulation</italic>
, which affects the ease with which congruent and incongruent actions are performed. We argue that the joint impact of these mechanisms can account for most existing embodiment effects. Additionally, we summarize empirical tests for distinguishing these mechanisms and suggest a guideline for future research about the mechanisms underlying embodiment effects.</p>
</div>
</front>
<back>
<div1 type="bibliography">
<listBibl>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ackerman, J" uniqKey="Ackerman J">J. Ackerman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nocera, C" uniqKey="Nocera C">C. Nocera</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bargh, J A" uniqKey="Bargh J">J. A. Bargh</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Adam, H" uniqKey="Adam H">H. Adam</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Galinsky, A D" uniqKey="Galinsky A">A. D. Galinsky</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Alban, M W" uniqKey="Alban M">M. W. Alban</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kelley, C M" uniqKey="Kelley C">C. M. Kelley</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Arrighi, R" uniqKey="Arrighi R">R. Arrighi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cartocci, G" uniqKey="Cartocci G">G. Cartocci</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Burr, D" uniqKey="Burr D">D. Burr</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bargh, J" uniqKey="Bargh J">J. Bargh</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chen, M" uniqKey="Chen M">M. Chen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Burrows, L" uniqKey="Burrows L">L. Burrows</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bargh, J A" uniqKey="Bargh J">J. A. Bargh</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schwader, K L" uniqKey="Schwader K">K. L. Schwader</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hailey, S E" uniqKey="Hailey S">S. E. Hailey</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dyer, R L" uniqKey="Dyer R">R. L. Dyer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Boothby, E J" uniqKey="Boothby E">E. J. Boothby</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, L" uniqKey="Barsalou L">L. Barsalou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, L" uniqKey="Barsalou L">L. Barsalou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, L" uniqKey="Barsalou L">L. Barsalou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, L" uniqKey="Barsalou L">L. Barsalou</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Niedenthal, P" uniqKey="Niedenthal P">P. Niedenthal</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barbey, A" uniqKey="Barbey A">A. Barbey</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ruppert, J" uniqKey="Ruppert J">J. Ruppert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Beilock, S" uniqKey="Beilock S">S. Beilock</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Holt, L" uniqKey="Holt L">L. Holt</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Beilock, S" uniqKey="Beilock S">S. Beilock</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lyons, I" uniqKey="Lyons I">I. Lyons</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mattarella Micke, A" uniqKey="Mattarella Micke A">A. Mattarella-Micke</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nusbaum, H" uniqKey="Nusbaum H">H. Nusbaum</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Small, S" uniqKey="Small S">S. Small</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bhalla, M" uniqKey="Bhalla M">M. Bhalla</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Proffitt, D R" uniqKey="Proffitt D">D. R. Proffitt</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bless, H" uniqKey="Bless H">H. Bless</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schwarz, N" uniqKey="Schwarz N">N. Schwarz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Borghi, A M" uniqKey="Borghi A">A. M. Borghi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cimatti, F" uniqKey="Cimatti F">F. Cimatti</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Boroditsky, L" uniqKey="Boroditsky L">L. Boroditsky</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Boroditsky, L" uniqKey="Boroditsky L">L. Boroditsky</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Boroditsky, L" uniqKey="Boroditsky L">L. Boroditsky</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gaby, A" uniqKey="Gaby A">A. Gaby</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Boroditsky, L" uniqKey="Boroditsky L">L. Boroditsky</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ramscar, M" uniqKey="Ramscar M">M. Ramscar</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Buccino, G" uniqKey="Buccino G">G. Buccino</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Binkofski, F" uniqKey="Binkofski F">F. Binkofski</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fink, G R" uniqKey="Fink G">G. R. Fink</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fadiga, L" uniqKey="Fadiga L">L. Fadiga</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fogassi, L" uniqKey="Fogassi L">L. Fogassi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gallese, V" uniqKey="Gallese V">V. Gallese</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cacioppo, J T" uniqKey="Cacioppo J">J. T. Cacioppo</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Priester, J R" uniqKey="Priester J">J. R. Priester</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Berntson, G G" uniqKey="Berntson G">G. G. Berntson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Campos, J J" uniqKey="Campos J">J. J. Campos</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Anderson, D I" uniqKey="Anderson D">D. I. Anderson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barbu Roth, M A" uniqKey="Barbu Roth M">M. A. Barbu-Roth</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hubbard, E M" uniqKey="Hubbard E">E. M. Hubbard</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hertenstein, M J" uniqKey="Hertenstein M">M. J. Hertenstein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Witherington, D" uniqKey="Witherington D">D. Witherington</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Casasanto, D" uniqKey="Casasanto D">D. Casasanto</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Casasanto, D" uniqKey="Casasanto D">D. Casasanto</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Casasanto, D" uniqKey="Casasanto D">D. Casasanto</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Boroditsky, L" uniqKey="Boroditsky L">L. Boroditsky</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Casasanto, D" uniqKey="Casasanto D">D. Casasanto</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chrysikou, E" uniqKey="Chrysikou E">E. Chrysikou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Casasanto, D" uniqKey="Casasanto D">D. Casasanto</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dijkstra, K" uniqKey="Dijkstra K">K. Dijkstra</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Casile, A" uniqKey="Casile A">A. Casile</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Giese, M A" uniqKey="Giese M">M. A. Giese</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Centerbar, D B" uniqKey="Centerbar D">D. B. Centerbar</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Clore, G L" uniqKey="Clore G">G. L. Clore</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cesario, J" uniqKey="Cesario J">J. Cesario</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcdonald, M M" uniqKey="Mcdonald M">M. M. McDonald</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chandler, J" uniqKey="Chandler J">J. Chandler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Reinhard, D" uniqKey="Reinhard D">D. Reinhard</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schwarz, N" uniqKey="Schwarz N">N. Schwarz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chandler, J" uniqKey="Chandler J">J. Chandler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schwarz, N" uniqKey="Schwarz N">N. Schwarz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chemero, A" uniqKey="Chemero A">A. Chemero</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chen, M" uniqKey="Chen M">M. Chen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bargh, J A" uniqKey="Bargh J">J. A. Bargh</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Clark, A" uniqKey="Clark A">A. Clark</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cohen, D" uniqKey="Cohen D">D. Cohen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leung, A" uniqKey="Leung A">A. Leung</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dewall, C N" uniqKey="Dewall C">C. N. DeWall</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bushman, B J" uniqKey="Bushman B">B. J. Bushman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dimmock, J A" uniqKey="Dimmock J">J. A. Dimmock</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jackson, B" uniqKey="Jackson B">B. Jackson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Clarke, D" uniqKey="Clarke D">D. Clarke</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Eder, A B" uniqKey="Eder A">A. B. Eder</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rothermund, K" uniqKey="Rothermund K">K. Rothermund</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Elder, R S" uniqKey="Elder R">R. S. Elder</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Krishna, A" uniqKey="Krishna A">A. Krishna</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fadiga, L" uniqKey="Fadiga L">L. Fadiga</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Craighero, L" uniqKey="Craighero L">L. Craighero</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Buccino, G" uniqKey="Buccino G">G. Buccino</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rizzolatti, G" uniqKey="Rizzolatti G">G. Rizzolatti</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fayant, M P" uniqKey="Fayant M">M.-P. Fayant</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Muller, D" uniqKey="Muller D">D. Muller</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nurra, C" uniqKey="Nurra C">C. Nurra</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Alexopoulos, T" uniqKey="Alexopoulos T">T. Alexopoulos</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Palluel Germain, R" uniqKey="Palluel Germain R">R. Palluel-Germain</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fischer, M H" uniqKey="Fischer M">M. H. Fischer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zwaan, R A" uniqKey="Zwaan R">R. A. Zwaan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Flusberg, S J" uniqKey="Flusberg S">S. J. Flusberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thibodeau, P H" uniqKey="Thibodeau P">P. H. Thibodeau</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sternberg, D A" uniqKey="Sternberg D">D. A. Sternberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Glick, J J" uniqKey="Glick J">J. J. Glick</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Foroni, F" uniqKey="Foroni F">F. Foroni</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Semin, G" uniqKey="Semin G">G. Semin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Foroni, F" uniqKey="Foroni F">F. Foroni</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Semin, G R" uniqKey="Semin G">G. R. Semin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Foroni, F" uniqKey="Foroni F">F. Foroni</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Semin, G R" uniqKey="Semin G">G. R. Semin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Forster, J" uniqKey="Forster J">J. Förster</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Forster, J" uniqKey="Forster J">J. Förster</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Liberman, N" uniqKey="Liberman N">N. Liberman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Friedman, R S" uniqKey="Friedman R">R. S. Friedman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Forster, J" uniqKey="Forster J">J. Förster</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Strack, F" uniqKey="Strack F">F. Strack</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Friedman, R S" uniqKey="Friedman R">R. S. Friedman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Forster, J" uniqKey="Forster J">J. Förster</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gallese, V" uniqKey="Gallese V">V. Gallese</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gallese, V" uniqKey="Gallese V">V. Gallese</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Glenberg, A M" uniqKey="Glenberg A">A. M. Glenberg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Glenberg, A M" uniqKey="Glenberg A">A. M. Glenberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kaschak, M" uniqKey="Kaschak M">M. Kaschak</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Glenberg, A M" uniqKey="Glenberg A">A. M. Glenberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Witt, J K" uniqKey="Witt J">J. K. Witt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Metcalfe, J" uniqKey="Metcalfe J">J. Metcalfe</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Goldman, A I" uniqKey="Goldman A">A. I. Goldman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Vignemont, F" uniqKey="De Vignemont F">F. de Vignemont</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gollwitzer, P M" uniqKey="Gollwitzer P">P. M. Gollwitzer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hamilton, A" uniqKey="Hamilton A">A. Hamilton</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wolpert, D" uniqKey="Wolpert D">D. Wolpert</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Frith, U" uniqKey="Frith U">U. Frith</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Havas, D A" uniqKey="Havas D">D. A. Havas</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Glenberg, A" uniqKey="Glenberg A">A. Glenberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gutowski, K" uniqKey="Gutowski K">K. Gutowski</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lucarelli, M" uniqKey="Lucarelli M">M. Lucarelli</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Davidson, R" uniqKey="Davidson R">R. Davidson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Havas, D A" uniqKey="Havas D">D. A. Havas</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Glenberg, A" uniqKey="Glenberg A">A. Glenberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rinck, M" uniqKey="Rinck M">M. Rinck</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Havas, D A" uniqKey="Havas D">D. A. Havas</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Matheson, J" uniqKey="Matheson J">J. Matheson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="H Fner, M" uniqKey="H Fner M">M. Häfner</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Huang, L" uniqKey="Huang L">L. Huang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Galinsky, A D" uniqKey="Galinsky A">A. D. Galinsky</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gruenfeld, D H" uniqKey="Gruenfeld D">D. H. Gruenfeld</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Guillory, L E" uniqKey="Guillory L">L. E. Guillory</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ijzerman, H" uniqKey="Ijzerman H">H. IJzerman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Semin, G R" uniqKey="Semin G">G. R. Semin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jacobs, A" uniqKey="Jacobs A">A. Jacobs</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shiffrar, M" uniqKey="Shiffrar M">M. Shiffrar</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="James, W" uniqKey="James W">W. James</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jones, A" uniqKey="Jones A">A. Jones</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fitness, J" uniqKey="Fitness J">J. Fitness</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jostmann, N" uniqKey="Jostmann N">N. Jostmann</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lakens, D" uniqKey="Lakens D">D. Lakens</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, T" uniqKey="Schubert T">T. Schubert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Klatzky, R L" uniqKey="Klatzky R">R. L. Klatzky</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pellegrino, J W" uniqKey="Pellegrino J">J. W. Pellegrino</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mccloskey, B P" uniqKey="Mccloskey B">B. P. McCloskey</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Doherty, S" uniqKey="Doherty S">S. Doherty</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Koch, S" uniqKey="Koch S">S. Koch</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Holland, R W" uniqKey="Holland R">R. W. Holland</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Knippenberg, A" uniqKey="Van Knippenberg A">A. van Knippenberg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kouchaki, M" uniqKey="Kouchaki M">M. Kouchaki</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gino, F" uniqKey="Gino F">F. Gino</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jami, A" uniqKey="Jami A">A. Jami</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Krieglmeyer, R" uniqKey="Krieglmeyer R">R. Krieglmeyer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Deutsch, R" uniqKey="Deutsch R">R. Deutsch</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Houwer, J" uniqKey="De Houwer J">J. De Houwer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Raedt, R" uniqKey="De Raedt R">R. De Raedt</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lakens, D" uniqKey="Lakens D">D. Lakens</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lakoff, G" uniqKey="Lakoff G">G. Lakoff</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnson, M" uniqKey="Johnson M">M. Johnson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lakoff, G" uniqKey="Lakoff G">G. Lakoff</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnson, M" uniqKey="Johnson M">M. Johnson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Landau, M J" uniqKey="Landau M">M. J. Landau</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meier, B P" uniqKey="Meier B">B. P. Meier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Keefer, L A" uniqKey="Keefer L">L. A. Keefer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leder, H" uniqKey="Leder H">H. Leder</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="B R, S" uniqKey="B R S">S. Bär</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Topolinski, S" uniqKey="Topolinski S">S. Topolinski</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lee, S W S" uniqKey="Lee S">S. W. S. Lee</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schwarz, N" uniqKey="Schwarz N">N. Schwarz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lee, S W S" uniqKey="Lee S">S. W. S. Lee</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schwarz, N" uniqKey="Schwarz N">N. Schwarz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lewis, M B" uniqKey="Lewis M">M. B. Lewis</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bowler, P J" uniqKey="Bowler P">P. J. Bowler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Loersch, C" uniqKey="Loersch C">C. Loersch</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Payne, B K" uniqKey="Payne B">B. K. Payne</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Louwerse, M M" uniqKey="Louwerse M">M. M. Louwerse</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jeuniaux, P" uniqKey="Jeuniaux P">P. Jeuniaux</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Machery, E" uniqKey="Machery E">E. Machery</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mahon, B" uniqKey="Mahon B">B. Mahon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Caramazza, A" uniqKey="Caramazza A">A. Caramazza</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Markman, A B" uniqKey="Markman A">A. B. Markman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brendl, C M" uniqKey="Brendl C">C. M. Brendl</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meier, B P" uniqKey="Meier B">B. P. Meier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schnall, S" uniqKey="Schnall S">S. Schnall</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schwarz, N" uniqKey="Schwarz N">N. Schwarz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bargh, J A" uniqKey="Bargh J">J. A. Bargh</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Michael, J" uniqKey="Michael J">J. Michael</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sandberg, K" uniqKey="Sandberg K">K. Sandberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Skewes, J" uniqKey="Skewes J">J. Skewes</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wolf, T" uniqKey="Wolf T">T. Wolf</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Blicher, J" uniqKey="Blicher J">J. Blicher</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Overgaard, M" uniqKey="Overgaard M">M. Overgaard</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Moors, A" uniqKey="Moors A">A. Moors</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Houwer, J" uniqKey="De Houwer J">J. De Houwer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mussweiler, T" uniqKey="Mussweiler T">T. Mussweiler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Neumann, R" uniqKey="Neumann R">R. Neumann</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Forster, J" uniqKey="Forster J">J. Förster</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Strack, F" uniqKey="Strack F">F. Strack</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Niedenthal, P" uniqKey="Niedenthal P">P. Niedenthal</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Niedenthal, P" uniqKey="Niedenthal P">P. Niedenthal</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, L" uniqKey="Barsalou L">L. Barsalou</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Winkielman, P" uniqKey="Winkielman P">P. Winkielman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Krauth Gruber, S" uniqKey="Krauth Gruber S">S. Krauth-Gruber</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ric, F" uniqKey="Ric F">F. Ric</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Niedenthal, P" uniqKey="Niedenthal P">P. Niedenthal</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brauer, M" uniqKey="Brauer M">M. Brauer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Halberstadt, J" uniqKey="Halberstadt J">J. Halberstadt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Innes Ker, " uniqKey="Innes Ker ">Å. Innes-Ker</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Niedenthal, P" uniqKey="Niedenthal P">P. Niedenthal</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Winkielman, P" uniqKey="Winkielman P">P. Winkielman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mondillon, L" uniqKey="Mondillon L">L. Mondillon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vermeulen, N" uniqKey="Vermeulen N">N. Vermeulen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nussinson, R" uniqKey="Nussinson R">R. Nussinson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Seibt, B" uniqKey="Seibt B">B. Seibt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="H Fner, M" uniqKey="H Fner M">M. Häfner</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Strack, F" uniqKey="Strack F">F. Strack</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Oberman, L" uniqKey="Oberman L">L. Oberman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Winkielman, P" uniqKey="Winkielman P">P. Winkielman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ramachandran, V" uniqKey="Ramachandran V">V. Ramachandran</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Papeo, L" uniqKey="Papeo L">L. Papeo</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vallesi, A" uniqKey="Vallesi A">A. Vallesi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Isaja, A" uniqKey="Isaja A">A. Isaja</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rumiati, R I" uniqKey="Rumiati R">R. I. Rumiati</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Park, L E" uniqKey="Park L">L. E. Park</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Streamer, L" uniqKey="Streamer L">L. Streamer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Huang, L" uniqKey="Huang L">L. Huang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Galinsky, A D" uniqKey="Galinsky A">A. D. Galinsky</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pazzaglia, M" uniqKey="Pazzaglia M">M. Pazzaglia</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pizzamiglio, L" uniqKey="Pizzamiglio L">L. Pizzamiglio</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pes, E" uniqKey="Pes E">E. Pes</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aglioti, S M" uniqKey="Aglioti S">S. M. Aglioti</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pecher, D" uniqKey="Pecher D">D. Pecher</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Winkielman, P" uniqKey="Winkielman P">P. Winkielman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pecher, D" uniqKey="Pecher D">D. Pecher</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zeelenberg, R" uniqKey="Zeelenberg R">R. Zeelenberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, L W" uniqKey="Barsalou L">L. W. Barsalou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Prinz, W" uniqKey="Prinz W">W. Prinz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Proctor, R W" uniqKey="Proctor R">R. W. Proctor</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cho, Y S" uniqKey="Cho Y">Y. S. Cho</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Proffitt, D R" uniqKey="Proffitt D">D. R. Proffitt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stefanucci, J" uniqKey="Stefanucci J">J. Stefanucci</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Banton, T" uniqKey="Banton T">T. Banton</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Epstein, W" uniqKey="Epstein W">W. Epstein</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Reed, C L" uniqKey="Reed C">C. L. Reed</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcgoldrick, J E" uniqKey="Mcgoldrick J">J. E. McGoldrick</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rizzolatti, G" uniqKey="Rizzolatti G">G. Rizzolatti</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Craighero, L" uniqKey="Craighero L">L. Craighero</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Russell, J A" uniqKey="Russell J">J. A. Russell</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sato, M" uniqKey="Sato M">M. Sato</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mengarelli, M" uniqKey="Mengarelli M">M. Mengarelli</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Riggio, L" uniqKey="Riggio L">L. Riggio</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gallese, V" uniqKey="Gallese V">V. Gallese</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Buccino, G" uniqKey="Buccino G">G. Buccino</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schneider, I" uniqKey="Schneider I">I. Schneider</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rutjens, B" uniqKey="Rutjens B">B. Rutjens</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jostmann, N" uniqKey="Jostmann N">N. Jostmann</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lakens, D" uniqKey="Lakens D">D. Lakens</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, T" uniqKey="Schubert T">T. Schubert</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Semin, G" uniqKey="Semin G">G. Semin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schwarz, N" uniqKey="Schwarz N">N. Schwarz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schwarz, N" uniqKey="Schwarz N">N. Schwarz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bless, H" uniqKey="Bless H">H. Bless</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schwarz, N" uniqKey="Schwarz N">N. Schwarz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Servay, W" uniqKey="Servay W">W. Servay</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kumpf, M" uniqKey="Kumpf M">M. Kumpf</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Seibt, B" uniqKey="Seibt B">B. Seibt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Neumann, R" uniqKey="Neumann R">R. Neumann</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nussinson, R" uniqKey="Nussinson R">R. Nussinson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Strack, F" uniqKey="Strack F">F. Strack</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shen, H" uniqKey="Shen H">H. Shen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sengupta, J" uniqKey="Sengupta J">J. Sengupta</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shtyrov, Y" uniqKey="Shtyrov Y">Y. Shtyrov</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Butorina, A" uniqKey="Butorina A">A. Butorina</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nikolaeva, A" uniqKey="Nikolaeva A">A. Nikolaeva</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stroganova, T" uniqKey="Stroganova T">T. Stroganova</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, E R" uniqKey="Smith E">E. R. Smith</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Branscombe, N R" uniqKey="Branscombe N">N. R. Branscombe</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, E R" uniqKey="Smith E">E. R. Smith</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Decoster, J" uniqKey="Decoster J">J. DeCoster</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, E R" uniqKey="Smith E">E. R. Smith</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Semin, G R" uniqKey="Semin G">G. R. Semin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Solarz, A K" uniqKey="Solarz A">A. K. Solarz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Solomon, K O" uniqKey="Solomon K">K. O. Solomon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barsalou, L W" uniqKey="Barsalou L">L. W. Barsalou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sommerville, J A" uniqKey="Sommerville J">J. A. Sommerville</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Woodward, A L" uniqKey="Woodward A">A. L. Woodward</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Needham, A" uniqKey="Needham A">A. Needham</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stanfield, R" uniqKey="Stanfield R">R. Stanfield</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zwaan, R" uniqKey="Zwaan R">R. Zwaan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Strack, F" uniqKey="Strack F">F. Strack</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Deutsch, R" uniqKey="Deutsch R">R. Deutsch</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Strack, F" uniqKey="Strack F">F. Strack</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Martin, L" uniqKey="Martin L">L. Martin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stepper, S" uniqKey="Stepper S">S. Stepper</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Topolinski, S" uniqKey="Topolinski S">S. Topolinski</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Topolinski, S" uniqKey="Topolinski S">S. Topolinski</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Strack, F" uniqKey="Strack F">F. Strack</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tucker, M" uniqKey="Tucker M">M. Tucker</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ellis, R" uniqKey="Ellis R">R. Ellis</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Urgesi, C" uniqKey="Urgesi C">C. Urgesi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Candidi, M" uniqKey="Candidi M">M. Candidi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ionta, S" uniqKey="Ionta S">S. Ionta</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aglioti, S M" uniqKey="Aglioti S">S. M. Aglioti</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wheeler, S C" uniqKey="Wheeler S">S. C. Wheeler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Demarree, K G" uniqKey="Demarree K">K. G. DeMarree</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Petty, R E" uniqKey="Petty R">R. E. Petty</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Willems, R M" uniqKey="Willems R">R. M. Willems</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Casasanto, D" uniqKey="Casasanto D">D. Casasanto</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Willems, R M" uniqKey="Willems R">R. M. Willems</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Francken, J C" uniqKey="Francken J">J. C. Francken</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Williams, L" uniqKey="Williams L">L. Williams</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Huang, J" uniqKey="Huang J">J. Huang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bargh, J" uniqKey="Bargh J">J. Bargh</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wilson, M" uniqKey="Wilson M">M. Wilson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wollmer, M A" uniqKey="Wollmer M">M. A. Wollmer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Boer, C" uniqKey="De Boer C">C. de Boer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kalak, N" uniqKey="Kalak N">N. Kalak</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Beck, J" uniqKey="Beck J">J. Beck</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gotz, T" uniqKey="Gotz T">T. Götz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schmidt, T" uniqKey="Schmidt T">T. Schmidt</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Xu, A J" uniqKey="Xu A">A. J. Xu</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zwick, R" uniqKey="Zwick R">R. Zwick</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schwarz, N" uniqKey="Schwarz N">N. Schwarz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yan, Z" uniqKey="Yan Z">Z. Yan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ding, D" uniqKey="Ding D">D. Ding</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yan, L" uniqKey="Yan L">L. Yan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zajonc, R" uniqKey="Zajonc R">R. Zajonc</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Markus, H" uniqKey="Markus H">H. Markus</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zhang, M" uniqKey="Zhang M">M. Zhang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Li, X" uniqKey="Li X">X. Li</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zhong, C" uniqKey="Zhong C">C. Zhong</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Liljenquist, K" uniqKey="Liljenquist K">K. Liljenquist</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zwaan, R A" uniqKey="Zwaan R">R. A. Zwaan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Taylor, L J" uniqKey="Taylor L">L. J. Taylor</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zwickel, J" uniqKey="Zwickel J">J. Zwickel</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Prinz, W" uniqKey="Prinz W">W. Prinz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
</listBibl>
</div1>
</back>
</TEI>
<pmc article-type="research-article">
<pmc-dir>properties open_access</pmc-dir>
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="nlm-ta">Front Psychol</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="iso-abbrev">Front Psychol</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="publisher-id">Front. Psychol.</journal-id>
<journal-title-group>
<journal-title>Frontiers in Psychology</journal-title>
</journal-title-group>
<issn pub-type="epub">1664-1078</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Frontiers Media S.A.</publisher-name>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmid">26191033</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmc">4488596</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00940</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Hypothesis & Theory</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>Routes to embodiment</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Körner</surname>
<given-names>Anita</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff1">
<sup>1</sup>
</xref>
<xref ref-type="author-notes" rid="fn001">
<sup>*</sup>
</xref>
<uri xlink:type="simple" xlink:href="http://loop.frontiersin.org/people/224329"></uri>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Topolinski</surname>
<given-names>Sascha</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff2">
<sup>2</sup>
</xref>
<uri xlink:type="simple" xlink:href="http://loop.frontiersin.org/people/11199"></uri>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Strack</surname>
<given-names>Fritz</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff1">
<sup>1</sup>
</xref>
<uri xlink:type="simple" xlink:href="http://loop.frontiersin.org/people/140954"></uri>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<aff id="aff1">
<sup>1</sup>
<institution>Department of Psychology II, University of Würzburg</institution>
,
<country>Würzburg, Germany</country>
</aff>
<aff id="aff2">
<sup>2</sup>
<institution>Social and Economic Cognition, Department of Psychology, University of Cologne</institution>
,
<country>Cologne, Germany</country>
</aff>
<author-notes>
<fn fn-type="edited-by">
<p>Edited by:
<italic>Anna M. Borghi, Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies – University of Bologna, Italy</italic>
</p>
</fn>
<fn fn-type="edited-by">
<p>Reviewed by:
<italic>Roel M. Willems, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Netherlands; Fausto Caruana, Italian Institute of Technology, Italy</italic>
</p>
</fn>
<corresp id="fn001">*Correspondence:
<italic>Anita Körner, Department of Psychology II, University of Würzburg, Roentgenring 10, 97070 Würzburg, Germany,
<email xlink:type="simple">anita.koerner@uni-wuerzburg.de</email>
</italic>
</corresp>
<fn fn-type="other" id="fn002">
<p>This article was submitted to Cognition, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology.</p>
</fn>
</author-notes>
<pub-date pub-type="epub">
<day>02</day>
<month>7</month>
<year>2015</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="collection">
<year>2015</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>6</volume>
<elocation-id>940</elocation-id>
<history>
<date date-type="received">
<day>10</day>
<month>4</month>
<year>2015</year>
</date>
<date date-type="accepted">
<day>22</day>
<month>6</month>
<year>2015</year>
</date>
</history>
<permissions>
<copyright-statement>Copyright © 2015 Körner, Topolinski and Strack.</copyright-statement>
<copyright-year>2015</copyright-year>
<copyright-holder>Körner, Topolinski and Strack</copyright-holder>
<license xlink:href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/">
<license-p>This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.</license-p>
</license>
</permissions>
<abstract>
<p>Research on embodiment is rich in impressive demonstrations but somewhat poor in comprehensive explanations. Although some moderators and driving mechanisms have been identified, a comprehensive conceptual account of how bodily states or dynamics influence behavior is still missing. Here, we attempt to integrate current knowledge by describing three basic psychological mechanisms:
<italic>direct state induction</italic>
, which influences how humans feel or process information, unmediated by any other cognitive mechanism;
<italic>modal priming</italic>
, which changes the accessibility of concepts associated with a bodily state;
<italic>sensorimotor simulation</italic>
, which affects the ease with which congruent and incongruent actions are performed. We argue that the joint impact of these mechanisms can account for most existing embodiment effects. Additionally, we summarize empirical tests for distinguishing these mechanisms and suggest a guideline for future research about the mechanisms underlying embodiment effects.</p>
</abstract>
<kwd-group>
<kwd>embodied cognition</kwd>
<kwd>grounded cognition</kwd>
<kwd>simulation</kwd>
<kwd>priming</kwd>
<kwd>metaphors</kwd>
</kwd-group>
<funding-group>
<award-group>
<funding-source id="cn001">German Research Foundation (DFG)</funding-source>
</award-group>
<award-group>
<funding-source id="cn002">University of Wuerzburg</funding-source>
</award-group>
<award-group>
<funding-source id="cn003">German Research Foundation</funding-source>
<award-id rid="cn003">DFG, TO 705/1-1</award-id>
<award-id rid="cn003">Str 264/26-1</award-id>
</award-group>
</funding-group>
<counts>
<fig-count count="0"></fig-count>
<table-count count="0"></table-count>
<equation-count count="0"></equation-count>
<ref-count count="143"></ref-count>
<page-count count="10"></page-count>
<word-count count="9038"></word-count>
</counts>
</article-meta>
</front>
<body>
<sec sec-type="intro">
<title>Introduction</title>
<p>Carrying weight makes hills seem steeper and distances seem greater (
<xref rid="B13" ref-type="bibr">Bhalla and Proffitt, 1999</xref>
;
<xref rid="B105" ref-type="bibr">Proffitt et al., 2003</xref>
). Additionally it influences judgments about observed weight lifting in others (
<xref rid="B59" ref-type="bibr">Hamilton et al., 2004</xref>
); but also estimates of a topic’s importance (
<xref rid="B69" ref-type="bibr">Jostmann et al., 2009</xref>
;
<xref rid="B31" ref-type="bibr">Chandler et al., 2012</xref>
), of one’s guilt (
<xref rid="B72" ref-type="bibr">Kouchaki et al., 2014</xref>
), and of one’s success in learning (
<xref rid="B3" ref-type="bibr">Alban and Kelley, 2013</xref>
). These findings highlight the body’s role in information processing, termed embodiment. Conceptually, however, this impressively diverse set of effects is poorly integrated (
<xref rid="B111" ref-type="bibr">Schubert and Semin, 2009</xref>
;
<xref rid="B9" ref-type="bibr">Barsalou, 2010</xref>
;
<xref rid="B87" ref-type="bibr">Meier et al., 2012</xref>
;
<xref rid="B133" ref-type="bibr">Willems and Francken, 2012</xref>
;
<xref rid="B56" ref-type="bibr">Glenberg et al., 2013</xref>
).</p>
<p>Recently, some moderators and driving mechanisms of specific embodiment effects have been identified. For example, interoceptive sensitivity was found to moderate different embodiment effects (
<xref rid="B63" ref-type="bibr">Häfner, 2013</xref>
), and fishy smells’ impact on distrust was linked to a chain of semantic associations (
<xref rid="B80" ref-type="bibr">Lee and Schwarz, 2012</xref>
). However, a comprehensive conceptual account of how bodily states or dynamics influence human experience and behavior is still missing. Therefore, it is our goal to integrate current knowledge by describing three basic psychological mechanisms and illustrate them briefly with findings from the cognitive, social psychology, and neuroscience literature (for more extensive reviews, see, e.g.,
<xref rid="B8" ref-type="bibr">Barsalou, 2008</xref>
;
<xref rid="B77" ref-type="bibr">Landau et al., 2010</xref>
;
<xref rid="B56" ref-type="bibr">Glenberg et al., 2013</xref>
).</p>
<p>To start out, we adopt a phenomenological definition for embodiment. Specifically, we define embodiment as an effect where the body, its sensorimotor state, its morphology, or its mental representation play an instrumental role in information processing. This definition is agnostic about how such knowledge is represented (for more theory-driven accounts of how to define embodiment, see, e.g.,
<xref rid="B135" ref-type="bibr">Wilson, 2002</xref>
;
<xref rid="B57" ref-type="bibr">Goldman and de Vignemont, 2009</xref>
). Even though the question about how knowledge is grounded, and to what degree this grounding is based on the body, is a vital one, our classification of embodiment effects does not explicitly make use of knowledge representations. Therefore, we leave the question of knowledge representation to theories of embodied cognition. Accordingly, some of the effects we discuss do not provide evidence for stronger versions of embodied or grounded cognition hypotheses (e.g.,
<xref rid="B35" ref-type="bibr">Clark, 1999</xref>
;
<xref rid="B33" ref-type="bibr">Chemero, 2013</xref>
; see also
<xref rid="B135" ref-type="bibr">Wilson, 2002</xref>
;
<xref rid="B84" ref-type="bibr">Machery, 2007</xref>
) but can also be explained by amodal mechanisms (i.e., mechanisms that are completely independent of modality-specific systems). Indeed, we also discuss amodal mechanisms for embodiment effects. Our primary goal is to compare, and classify the effects and understand their underlying mechanisms. Hopefully, this will advance our understanding of the body’s role in information processing and shed light on the more fundamental mechanisms of cognition and the nature of the cognitive architecture.</p>
<p>Pursuing this goal, we suggest a classification for embodiment effects that is based on their underlying mechanisms. Specifically, we argue that sensations or actions can have three qualitatively different effects on the mind.</p>
<list list-type="simple">
<list-item>
<label></label>
<p>First, they can directly alter a person’s state of mind, feelings, or information processing (
<italic>direct state induction</italic>
).</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<label></label>
<p>Second, they can change how readily specific information comes to mind, thus influencing the mental contents instead of the mode of operation (
<italic>modal priming</italic>
).</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<label></label>
<p>Third, they can lead to compatibility effects with concurrent automatic simulations, changing, for example, fluency and preferences (
<italic>sensorimotor simulation</italic>
).</p>
</list-item>
</list>
<p>We classify all three mechanisms as automatic in the sense that they require little processing resources and can be initiated unintentionally (see
<xref rid="B89" ref-type="bibr">Moors and De Houwer, 2006</xref>
). However, this does not mean these mechanisms are uncontrollable or inflexible. On the contrary, we adopt a situated cognition perspective (
<xref rid="B120" ref-type="bibr">Smith and Semin, 2004</xref>
); thus, especially modal priming and sensorimotor simulation are generally influenced, among other things, by a person’s current goal state and by situational circumstances.</p>
<p>In the following, we sketch each route to embodiment and highlight its signature characteristics. We do not go into details about the mechanisms, as all of them have been proposed before and explained more fully than could be done here
<sup>
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="fn01">1</xref>
</sup>
. Instead, we highlight and compare the mechanisms and their features. Our key thesis is that each mechanism has distinct features that cannot easily be explained by any of the other mechanisms. This enables us to provide procedures to test their respective contributions for a given empirical effect.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Mechanism 1: Direct State Induction</title>
<p>Some bodily states directly induce affective or non-affective feelings and are not mediated by any kind of higher cognitive mechanism such as attributions or inferences (
<xref rid="B10" ref-type="bibr">Barsalou et al., 2003</xref>
;
<xref rid="B91" ref-type="bibr">Neumann et al., 2003</xref>
;
<xref rid="B92" ref-type="bibr">Niedenthal, 2007</xref>
). For example, keeping muscle tension associated with increasing or decreasing distance may induce a motivational orientation of avoidance or approach, respectively (
<xref rid="B21" ref-type="bibr">Cacioppo et al., 1993</xref>
;
<xref rid="B125" ref-type="bibr">Strack and Deutsch, 2004</xref>
). Similarly, disabling the corrugator muscle (by an injection of Botulinum Toxin A) may reduce negative affect and depressive symptoms (
<xref rid="B81" ref-type="bibr">Lewis and Bowler, 2009</xref>
;
<xref rid="B136" ref-type="bibr">Wollmer et al., 2012</xref>
). Thus, with facial configuration directly influencing emotional experiences, direct state induction is akin to William James’s theory of emotion elicitation (
<xref rid="B67" ref-type="bibr">James, 1884</xref>
) and to some later theories of emotion, like facial feedback theory and hard interface theory (
<xref rid="B139" ref-type="bibr">Zajonc and Markus, 1988</xref>
). Accordingly, this route to embodiment should generally be stable, in terms of both context influences and temporal dynamics.</p>
<p>The resulting psychological state can, in turn, also influence judgment or behavior. For example, distance changing muscle contractions were found to influence diverse judgments and behaviors, such as valence judgments (
<xref rid="B21" ref-type="bibr">Cacioppo et al., 1993</xref>
;
<xref rid="B29" ref-type="bibr">Centerbar and Clore, 2006</xref>
), the recognition of valenced information (
<xref rid="B50" ref-type="bibr">Förster and Strack, 1996</xref>
), the amount of consumed food (
<xref rid="B48" ref-type="bibr">Förster, 2003</xref>
), tendencies to judge others as similar or dissimilar to oneself (
<xref rid="B96" ref-type="bibr">Nussinson et al., 2010</xref>
), and the recruitment of different cognitive resources (
<xref rid="B51" ref-type="bibr">Friedman and Förster, 2005</xref>
;
<xref rid="B71" ref-type="bibr">Koch et al., 2008</xref>
).</p>
<p>Direct state induction is similar to procedural or mindset priming. Both are content free (
<xref rid="B58" ref-type="bibr">Gollwitzer, 1990</xref>
;
<xref rid="B49" ref-type="bibr">Förster et al., 2009</xref>
), which implies that the activation refers not to distinct concepts but to the information processing style, influencing cognitive functions in unrelated domains. Moreover, it does not matter how the altered state or mindset came about. The same motivational state may influence cognitive processes in the same manner, regardless if the state was elicited by a cognitive or an embodied induction. For instance, an avoidance orientation should result in a tendency to evaluate neutral stimuli as negative and to adopt a narrow attentional scope—no matter if it was induced by walking backward (
<xref rid="B42" ref-type="bibr">Fayant et al., 2011</xref>
), by arm extension (
<xref rid="B21" ref-type="bibr">Cacioppo et al., 1993</xref>
), or by performing a task of guiding a mouse through a maze to avoid a bird of prey (
<xref rid="B51" ref-type="bibr">Friedman and Förster, 2005</xref>
).</p>
<p>Even though direct state induction works largely independently of contextual and situational factors, the elicited state may be prevented from influencing behavior, for instance by re-attributing the feeling (
<xref rid="B112" ref-type="bibr">Schwarz, 2011</xref>
). Thus, like physical arousal’s influence on a judgment can be moderated by attributing it to an incidental source (e.g., a pill) instead of one’s reaction to the stimulus (
<xref rid="B114" ref-type="bibr">Schwarz et al., 1985</xref>
), the influence of bodily induced feelings on behavior can be counteracted. Therefore, finding high context sensitivity for an embodiment manipulation does not rule out direct state induction as the responsible mechanism. Instead, it is crucial to distinguish between the induced state and the resulting behavior. While the former is assumed to follow directly from the embodiment manipulation, the latter is highly flexible and context sensitive. Thus, direct state induction alters a person’s state or mindset. But, how this state and mindset is used depends on a number of factors besides the state itself.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Mechanism 2: Modal Priming</title>
<p>In modal priming, sensorimotor states activate (often abstract) concepts. For example, handling a rough (vs. smooth) object leads to judging social interactions to be less coordinated (
<xref rid="B1" ref-type="bibr">Ackerman et al., 2010</xref>
). Similarly, holding a heavy (vs. light) object heightens the perceived significance of an object or the weight of a topic (
<xref rid="B69" ref-type="bibr">Jostmann et al., 2009</xref>
;
<xref rid="B110" ref-type="bibr">Schneider et al., 2011</xref>
). This mechanism presupposes that representations contain modality-specific aspects (e.g., movements or bodily, facial, or sensory states). Activating the bodily states partially activates the associated semantic concepts (e.g., by means of spreading activation in a multi-modal associative store; see
<xref rid="B119" ref-type="bibr">Smith and DeCoster, 2000</xref>
;
<xref rid="B125" ref-type="bibr">Strack and Deutsch, 2004</xref>
; for a connectionist model for a modal priming effect, see
<xref rid="B44" ref-type="bibr">Flusberg et al., 2010</xref>
); and, in turn, the activated semantic concepts can influence behavior.</p>
<p>Modal priming effects have gained much attention through reports of surprising associations. Instead of being semantically associated, the bodily states or actions and the abstract concepts are often connected via conceptual metaphors (
<xref rid="B75" ref-type="bibr">Lakoff and Johnson, 1980</xref>
,
<xref rid="B76" ref-type="bibr">1999</xref>
;
<xref rid="B77" ref-type="bibr">Landau et al., 2010</xref>
). For example, a faint smell of fish decreases trust in social interactions (
<xref rid="B80" ref-type="bibr">Lee and Schwarz, 2012</xref>
). Yet, the connection between fish and trust consists solely in the metaphoric expression of “something smelling fishy” for untrustworthiness.</p>
<p>Apart from its distinct mode of activation, modal priming operates similar to priming in general (
<xref rid="B80" ref-type="bibr">Lee and Schwarz, 2012</xref>
;
<xref rid="B87" ref-type="bibr">Meier et al., 2012</xref>
). Specifically, the bodily manipulation increases the accessibility of associated concepts making them more likely to be used in subsequent tasks (
<xref rid="B82" ref-type="bibr">Loersch and Payne, 2011</xref>
). Importantly, accessible concepts can be used to either construct a target or a standard of comparison (
<xref rid="B113" ref-type="bibr">Schwarz and Bless, 1992</xref>
;
<xref rid="B14" ref-type="bibr">Bless and Schwarz, 2010</xref>
), leading to assimilation or contrast effects, respectively. Also, they can be discarded altogether when they are attributed to a source that is irrelevant for the task at hand (see also
<xref rid="B131" ref-type="bibr">Wheeler et al., 2007</xref>
, for a similar account).</p>
<p>Like other kinds of priming, modal priming effects should in general be bidirectional. Thus, while a fishy smell activates the concept of suspiciousness, inducing suspicion also lowers the sensory threshold to detect fishy smells (
<xref rid="B80" ref-type="bibr">Lee and Schwarz, 2012</xref>
). Similarly, walking slowly activates the elderly concept (
<xref rid="B90" ref-type="bibr">Mussweiler, 2006</xref>
), while, conversely, activating the elderly concept decreases walking speed (
<xref rid="B5" ref-type="bibr">Bargh et al., 1996</xref>
). Although there is convincing evidence for the unidirectionality of some effects (e.g.,
<xref rid="B16" ref-type="bibr">Boroditsky, 2000</xref>
;
<xref rid="B25" ref-type="bibr">Casasanto and Boroditsky, 2008</xref>
), the majority of metaphoric effects have been shown to work bidirectionally. Similarly, for priming in general, bidirectionality is the rule rather than the exception.</p>
<p>In fact, a similar analogy holds for
<italic>modal</italic>
and
<italic>semantic priming</italic>
to the previously described analogy of
<italic>direct state induction</italic>
and
<italic>mindset priming</italic>
. As long as a concept is activated and attributed to one’s own judgment about a currently perceived stimulus, similar effects result whether the concept is activated semantically or physically (
<xref rid="B65" ref-type="bibr">IJzerman and Semin, 2010</xref>
; see also
<xref rid="B37" ref-type="bibr">DeWall and Bushman, 2009</xref>
;
<xref rid="B38" ref-type="bibr">Dimmock et al., 2013</xref>
). From this analogy follow some properties of
<italic>modal priming</italic>
and
<italic>direct state induction</italic>
:
<italic>modal priming</italic>
influences specific associated concepts while
<italic>direct state induction</italic>
affects a broader range of behaviors. Moreover, the time course should differ. While content that is activated through priming gets quickly deactivated, altered information processing tends to last longer after the manipulation (
<xref rid="B118" ref-type="bibr">Smith and Branscombe, 1987</xref>
).</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Mechanism 3: Sensorimotor Simulation</title>
<p>Perceiving a stimulus automatically triggers the simulation of reenacting or interacting with it (
<xref rid="B7" ref-type="bibr">Barsalou, 1999</xref>
,
<xref rid="B8" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
). For instance, seeing objects that afford handling evokes the simulation of grasping (
<xref rid="B129" ref-type="bibr">Tucker and Ellis, 1998</xref>
), reading words elicits the simulation of pronunciation (
<xref rid="B41" ref-type="bibr">Fadiga et al., 2002</xref>
;
<xref rid="B128" ref-type="bibr">Topolinski and Strack, 2009</xref>
) and reading sentences leads to a multi-sensory simulation of the experiential content (
<xref rid="B124" ref-type="bibr">Stanfield and Zwaan, 2001</xref>
;
<xref rid="B55" ref-type="bibr">Glenberg and Kaschak, 2002</xref>
;
<xref rid="B102" ref-type="bibr">Pecher et al., 2003</xref>
;
<xref rid="B43" ref-type="bibr">Fischer and Zwaan, 2008</xref>
). This automatic simulation is very similar to the action or sensation itself—even employing the same brain regions (
<xref rid="B52" ref-type="bibr">Gallese, 2007</xref>
). Taking this notion farther, mental representation might essentially be “the reenactment of previous experiences” (
<xref rid="B101" ref-type="bibr">Pecher and Winkielman, 2013</xref>
, p. 396).</p>
<p>As a result, sensations or actions are facilitated if they are congruent with the simulated sensations or actions, while incompatible sensations or actions are hampered. For example, pressing the right (or left) key in response to an image of a cup with its handle on the right (or left) side is facilitated compared to the mismatching assignment (
<xref rid="B129" ref-type="bibr">Tucker and Ellis, 1998</xref>
).</p>
<p>Simulation is important in social interactions. Understanding actions in others involves activity in brain regions involved in performing these very actions. Indeed, a network of brain regions (sometimes referred to as mirror-neuron system) is activated both when an action is performed and when it is observed in others (
<xref rid="B20" ref-type="bibr">Buccino et al., 2001</xref>
;
<xref rid="B107" ref-type="bibr">Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004</xref>
; cf.
<xref rid="B103" ref-type="bibr">Prinz, 1990</xref>
). Evidence for a causal role of those brain regions in action understanding comes from lesion and interference studies (e.g.,
<xref rid="B130" ref-type="bibr">Urgesi et al., 2007</xref>
;
<xref rid="B100" ref-type="bibr">Pazzaglia et al., 2008</xref>
). For example, temporarily impairing participants’ hand (vs. lip) area of the premotor cortex (by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation) increases error rates in understanding pantomimed hand (vs. lip) actions (
<xref rid="B88" ref-type="bibr">Michael et al., 2014</xref>
). Thus, in addition to its quality of reenacting previous experiences, simulation has an additional function in predicting other’s behavior (
<xref rid="B52" ref-type="bibr">Gallese, 2007</xref>
,
<xref rid="B53" ref-type="bibr">2009</xref>
; see also
<xref rid="B15" ref-type="bibr">Borghi and Cimatti, 2010</xref>
).</p>
<p>Simulation has also been shown to play a causal role in various other mental faculties, such as processing emotion (
<xref rid="B97" ref-type="bibr">Oberman et al., 2007</xref>
;
<xref rid="B45" ref-type="bibr">Foroni and Semin, 2009</xref>
,
<xref rid="B46" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
,
<xref rid="B47" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
;
<xref rid="B95" ref-type="bibr">Niedenthal et al., 2009</xref>
) or representing meaning (
<xref rid="B70" ref-type="bibr">Klatzky et al., 1989</xref>
;
<xref rid="B142" ref-type="bibr">Zwaan and Taylor, 2006</xref>
)—even the meaning of abstract concepts (e.g., information transfer,
<xref rid="B55" ref-type="bibr">Glenberg and Kaschak, 2002</xref>
). Moreover, preference and esthetic appreciation also relies on sensorimotor simulation, specifically its fluency (
<xref rid="B11" ref-type="bibr">Beilock and Holt, 2007</xref>
;
<xref rid="B128" ref-type="bibr">Topolinski and Strack, 2009</xref>
).</p>
<p>Automatic simulation depends on previous experience and skills (
<xref rid="B12" ref-type="bibr">Beilock et al., 2008</xref>
). Accordingly, participants trained in a specific movement are better than untrained participants at visually recognizing similar movements (
<xref rid="B28" ref-type="bibr">Casile and Giese, 2006</xref>
). Similarly, participants with severe spinal-cord injury are impaired in detecting biological motion (
<xref rid="B4" ref-type="bibr">Arrighi et al., 2011</xref>
). Moreover, young children learn about other people’s goal-directed object manipulations by interacting with objects themselves (
<xref rid="B123" ref-type="bibr">Sommerville et al., 2005</xref>
; see also
<xref rid="B22" ref-type="bibr">Campos et al., 2000</xref>
).</p>
<p>A unique feature of the simulation mechanism is that it also works offline, that is, in absence of the particular bodily state or action that is simulated (
<xref rid="B135" ref-type="bibr">Wilson, 2002</xref>
; see also
<xref rid="B93" ref-type="bibr">Niedenthal et al., 2005</xref>
). A stimulus is processed and elicits a simulation even without any particular sensorimotor state; this simulation—its ease or the associated sensorimotor activity—affects information processing, judgment, and behavior. From this property it follows that a bodily manipulation can be used to both facilitate and interfere with the simulation (e.g.,
<xref rid="B11" ref-type="bibr">Beilock and Holt, 2007</xref>
;
<xref rid="B40" ref-type="bibr">Elder and Krishna, 2012</xref>
).</p>
<p>Moreover, simulation is more flexible in how and when it takes place than the other two mechanisms. Simulation occurs automatic, unintentionally, fast, and even without attending to the stimulus eliciting the simulation (
<xref rid="B117" ref-type="bibr">Shtyrov et al., 2014</xref>
). However, it can be blocked by a concurrent task using the same sensorimotor resources (e.g., chewing blocks covert pronunciation,
<xref rid="B128" ref-type="bibr">Topolinski and Strack, 2009</xref>
) and can even be prevented by performing some tasks on the stimuli (
<xref rid="B122" ref-type="bibr">Solomon and Barsalou, 2004</xref>
;
<xref rid="B109" ref-type="bibr">Sato et al., 2008</xref>
;
<xref rid="B95" ref-type="bibr">Niedenthal et al., 2009</xref>
;
<xref rid="B98" ref-type="bibr">Papeo et al., 2009</xref>
; for an overview, see also
<xref rid="B132" ref-type="bibr">Willems and Casasanto, 2011</xref>
). As yet, it is not entirely clear which conditions prevent simulation, and whether these conditions are independent of simulation contents (e.g., emotions or actions), but it seems that simulation is a default mechanism that occurs unless it is prevented.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Mixed Forms</title>
<p>We do not argue that every embodiment effect is exclusively driven by one of these three routes. Instead, joint influences are rather the rule than the exception. For instance, there seem to be two driving mechanisms for embodied cleansing. Embodied cleansing refers to the effect that physical cleansing one’s hands reduces the impact of previous experiences (e.g., washing one’s hands after a transgression reduces guilty feelings,
<xref rid="B141" ref-type="bibr">Zhong and Liljenquist, 2006</xref>
). On the one hand, several studies found semantic associations between (im)morality and (un)cleanness. Specifically, activating the concept of immorality leads to higher accessibility of cleaning-related words (
<xref rid="B141" ref-type="bibr">Zhong and Liljenquist, 2006</xref>
;
<xref rid="B68" ref-type="bibr">Jones and Fitness, 2008</xref>
;
<xref rid="B138" ref-type="bibr">Yan et al., 2011</xref>
), which points to modal priming. On the other hand, cleaning influences a broad range of cognitive processes in domains beyond morality, called the “clean slate effect” (
<xref rid="B79" ref-type="bibr">Lee and Schwarz, 2010</xref>
;
<xref rid="B137" ref-type="bibr">Xu et al., 2012</xref>
); and diversity of consequences is a signature feature of direct state induction. A similar case can be made for the influence of weight on importance judgments. Here, both modal priming (
<xref rid="B140" ref-type="bibr">Zhang and Li, 2012</xref>
) and sensorimotor simulation (
<xref rid="B63" ref-type="bibr">Häfner, 2013</xref>
) have been argued to contribute to the effect.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>The Role of Conscious Inferences</title>
<p>Inferential processes may also play a role in combination with the three automatic mechanisms discussed thus far. Specifically, the bodily state or action can be consciously perceived and categorized, such that inferences can be drawn from it, which may contribute to the observed effect. One such inferential process could be self-perception. Typically,
<italic>self-perception</italic>
has been ruled out in embodiment by the use of unobtrusive bodily manipulations and elaborate debriefings.</p>
<p>However, inferential processes can also influence the
<italic>accessibility</italic>
of concepts. A conscious classification of an action that is inherently ambiguous can be necessary for the activation of a concept, and thus for the occurrence of modal priming. For example, clothes make the man—depending on man’s interpretation of the clothes: Wearing a white coat increases participants’ performance in attention-related tasks when the coat is introduced as a doctor’s coat compared to a painter’s coat (
<xref rid="B2" ref-type="bibr">Adam and Galinsky, 2012</xref>
). This is another instance of a mixed effect as both the conscious label and actual wearing of the coat are necessary for the effect to occur (
<xref rid="B2" ref-type="bibr">Adam and Galinsky, 2012</xref>
; see also
<xref rid="B19" ref-type="bibr">Boroditsky and Ramscar, 2002</xref>
;
<xref rid="B27" ref-type="bibr">Casasanto and Dijkstra, 2010</xref>
, for related notions about the influence of a movement’s subjective meaning).</p>
<p>Conscious inferences can also influence or even reverse compatibility effects of sensorimotor simulation. In general, changing distance leads to a compatibility effect with the valence of a stimulus (i.e., moving toward a positive stimulus and away from a negative one are faster than
<italic>vice versa</italic>
; e.g.,
<xref rid="B121" ref-type="bibr">Solarz, 1960</xref>
;
<xref rid="B34" ref-type="bibr">Chen and Bargh, 1999</xref>
). However, giving a different meaning to the movement can reverse the effect (
<xref rid="B86" ref-type="bibr">Markman and Brendl, 2005</xref>
;
<xref rid="B39" ref-type="bibr">Eder and Rothermund, 2008</xref>
;
<xref rid="B115" ref-type="bibr">Seibt et al., 2008</xref>
). Interestingly, when combining re-categorization of the movement and automatic tendencies to increase or decrease distance, both influence movement speed depending on the valence of the word (
<xref rid="B73" ref-type="bibr">Krieglmeyer et al., 2010</xref>
). So it seems that both automatic distance regulation and the situational meaning of a movement produce compatibility effects between a word’s valence and movement.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Disentangling the Routes to Embodiment</title>
<p>The current topology of three routes to embodiment does not only parsimoniously cover the bulk of existing embodiment effects with a few basic principles, it also provides empirical tests of their respective contributions to a given phenomenon. Let us illustrate this with the well-known pen manipulation (
<xref rid="B126" ref-type="bibr">Strack et al., 1988</xref>
), where participants hold a pencil either between their protruded lips with their teeth (while not touching it with the lips) or with their lips. This procedure was meant to either facilitate or inhibit the contraction of the zygomaticus muscle which is used in smiling. At the same time, participants were prevented from categorizing their facial action as a smile and draw inferences from it. While the smile facilitation condition yielded higher funniness ratings of cartoons compared to a neutral control condition, inhibited smiling caused lower funniness ratings (
<xref rid="B126" ref-type="bibr">Strack et al., 1988</xref>
).</p>
<p>The pen manipulation could reasonably be based on all three proposed mechanisms. First, operating through direct state induction, smiling might improve mood. Yet, when assessing participants’ affective state, generally no influence of the pen manipulation can be detected (
<xref rid="B126" ref-type="bibr">Strack et al., 1988</xref>
;
<xref rid="B94" ref-type="bibr">Niedenthal et al., 2001</xref>
). Second, operating through modal priming, smiling could activate fun-related concepts which could in turn be used as judgmental cues in the funniness ratings. However, this is also unlikely because holding a pen between one’s teeth does not lead to faster responses for positively valenced words in a lexical decision task (
<xref rid="B61" ref-type="bibr">Havas et al., 2007</xref>
). Third and most likely, automatic simulations of subtle smiling could be triggered when evaluating funniness (
<xref rid="B45" ref-type="bibr">Foroni and Semin, 2009</xref>
). These smiling simulations could be influenced by concurrent facilitated or inhibited smiling as a result of holding a pen. And indeed, facilitated or inhibited sensorimotor simulations seem to lead to respective effects on evaluations (
<xref rid="B62" ref-type="bibr">Havas and Matheson, 2013</xref>
). Moreover, only sensorimotor simulation explains the decrease in funniness ratings by an inhibition of smiling compared to a no-interference control condition. Thus, the facial posture’s influence on funniness ratings seems to involve sensorimotor simulation.</p>
<p>In the following we outline tests for each causal route, exploiting its respective procedural features such as breadth of effects or arbitrariness of associations.</p>
<sec>
<title>Probing Direct State Induction</title>
<p>Direct state induction requires the activation of a global psychological state, emotion, or mindset, which allows for the following tests.</p>
<sec>
<title>Presence of Induced State</title>
<p>As its name says, the test of direct state induction is assessing the presence of the induced state. Recurring to the smile facilitation example, smiling could improve positive mood which could lead to cartoons being judged funnier. But as the effect on cartoon judgment has been repeatedly observed without concurrent mood change, this mechanism is unlikely. Of course, assessing the focal state depends on the measure’s sensitivity—for instance, smile induction might elicit affect too subtle to be caught by mood reports—and agreement on what the state might be. If, however, the proposed state does vary with the manipulation, and if this variation mediates the effect of the manipulation on the dependent measure, then direct state induction seems responsible for producing the embodiment effect at hand.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Diversity of Consequences</title>
<p>Usually, states induced by embodiment are global psychological states, such as emotions or motivational orientations, prompting a broad variety of cognitive, affective, and behavioral consequences. Thus, the scope of impact should usually be broader for direct state induction than for the other routes—particularly concerning consequences in content domains that are not connected to the bodily manipulation. For instance, the link between arm muscle contraction and performance in the Stroop task (
<xref rid="B71" ref-type="bibr">Koch et al., 2008</xref>
) cannot be reconstructed by semantic associations or sensorimotor simulation.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Universal State Induction</title>
<p>As direct state induction does not rest on stored semantic or linguistic associations, inducing a certain sensorimotor state should invariably result in a similar state change—across languages, cultures, and other factors that shape semantic memory. Thus, cross-cultural replications of the same effect speak in favor of direct state induction. In contrast, particularly modal priming, often involving culturally idiosyncratic language metaphors, should substantially be modulated by culture (see the next section).</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Probing Modal Priming</title>
<p>Modal priming generally conforms to the rules of priming more generally, which allows for the following tests.</p>
<sec>
<title>Activation of Concepts Instead of Global States</title>
<p>In modal priming, embodiment manipulations invariably activate associated concepts. Indeed, this concept activation is thought to mediate any effects. Therefore, the most direct test is to measure concept accessibility, for instance, via a lexical decision or word stem completion task (
<xref rid="B80" ref-type="bibr">Lee and Schwarz, 2012</xref>
). If the concepts supposedly mediating an embodiment effect are not activated by the manipulation, modal priming is an unlikely mechanism—as in the case of no heightened accessibility of positive valence words by the pen manipulation (
<xref rid="B61" ref-type="bibr">Havas et al., 2007</xref>
). In contrast to the rather broad affective, cognitive, and behavioral consequences following direct state induction (see the preceding section), concept activation should be relatively more narrow and specific and always traceable through associative links.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Dependence on Associative Structures</title>
<p>As any other priming form, modal priming depends on the architecture of the associative network and thus on cultural, linguistic, biographic, and many other factors shaping this associative structure. Therefore, for example, cultural practices, linguistic metaphors, expertise, and interindividual differences should influence the presence and direction of modal priming effects. This is, for instance, the case for the mental representation of time which varies with linguistic metaphors in a culture (
<xref rid="B17" ref-type="bibr">Boroditsky, 2001</xref>
;
<xref rid="B18" ref-type="bibr">Boroditsky and Gaby, 2010</xref>
).</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Arbitrariness and Flexibility of Associations</title>
<p>One unique feature of modal associations is that they can be arbitrary. Although for many associations an ecological connection seems plausible, this is not necessary. Arbitrary cultural conventions, for instance, showing hostility by extending one’s middle finger (
<xref rid="B32" ref-type="bibr">Chandler and Schwarz, 2009</xref>
), can form strong associations via learning and thus yield embodiment effects. Furthermore, these associations can be transformed via learning, so that experimental conditioning of associations should alter embodiment effects that rest upon modal priming. In contrast, the bodily conditions in direct state inductions are rather phylogenetically shaped and rigid states; and their relations to psychological effects are hard-wired and less flexible, such as emotions and their affective and cognitive syndromes (
<xref rid="B108" ref-type="bibr">Russell, 2003</xref>
).</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Probing Sensorimotor Simulation</title>
<p>Simulation takes place automatically, in the absence of the sensation or action that is simulated, which allows for the following tests.</p>
<sec>
<title>Interference</title>
<p>The unique feature of sensorimotor simulation is its susceptibility to interference and complete blockade (e.g.,
<xref rid="B11" ref-type="bibr">Beilock and Holt, 2007</xref>
;
<xref rid="B45" ref-type="bibr">Foroni and Semin, 2009</xref>
;
<xref rid="B128" ref-type="bibr">Topolinski and Strack, 2009</xref>
). Thus, to test simulation, a concurrent task that engages the same sensorimotor resources as the simulation should interfere with the simulation. If an effect relies partly on simulation, the effect should be diminished under such interventions. For example, participants with blocked frowning muscles are slower to read negative emotional sentences (
<xref rid="B60" ref-type="bibr">Havas et al., 2010</xref>
).</p>
<p>Note that a facilitated simulation effect is not as strong support for simulation as an inhibited simulation effect. Both effects would be expected when simulation contributes to an effect, but facilitation could also be explained by modal priming while inhibition could not. Having all three conditions in the same experiment (i.e., facilitation, inhibition, and a neutral condition, where simulation is neither facilitated nor inhibited) constitutes a strong test for simulation. Even more so, as alternative explanations (e.g., distraction) are ruled out if the manipulations for interference and facilitation are similar.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Fluency</title>
<p>Another—and, where feasible, particularly neat—test for simulation consists in altering the fluency of the simulation. Participants trained in a specific movement are better than control participants at simulating that movement, and consequently profit at related tasks (
<xref rid="B28" ref-type="bibr">Casile and Giese, 2006</xref>
;
<xref rid="B127" ref-type="bibr">Topolinski, 2010</xref>
;
<xref rid="B78" ref-type="bibr">Leder et al., 2013</xref>
). Additionally, simulation fluency can be altered by “untraining” a more fluent action. For example, people generally prefer objects according to their handedness—right-handers prefer objects on their right side, and left-handers on their left side (
<xref rid="B23" ref-type="bibr">Casasanto, 2009</xref>
;
<xref rid="B40" ref-type="bibr">Elder and Krishna, 2012</xref>
;
<xref rid="B116" ref-type="bibr">Shen and Sengupta, 2012</xref>
). However, performing movements where the dominant hand is made relatively clumsy reverses this fluency, which in turn reverses preferences (
<xref rid="B26" ref-type="bibr">Casasanto and Chrysikou, 2011</xref>
). This kind of temporarily altering fluency by training has the advantage that no manipulation has to be present when the effect is measured. Moreover, for disfluency training, priming accounts would rather make the opposite prediction from simulation. The training increases frequency of prior concept activation and thereby should lead to an increase in accessibility, while sensorimotor simulation predicts a decreased embodiment effect as a result of disfluency training.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Further Tests</title>
<sec>
<title>Conditions for Contrast Effects</title>
<p>Up to now, most published embodiment effects show an assimilative pattern. However, in sensorimotor simulation, specific differences in timing (
<xref rid="B106" ref-type="bibr">Reed and McGoldrick, 2007</xref>
) or similarity between current and simulated sensorimotor state (
<xref rid="B66" ref-type="bibr">Jacobs and Shiffrar, 2005</xref>
) can change facilitation into interference and can thereby lead to contrast effects (see also
<xref rid="B143" ref-type="bibr">Zwickel and Prinz, 2012</xref>
). In modal priming, though up to now not empirically tested, experimental manipulations that influence whether the activated knowledge is used to construct the target or the standard of comparison might play a greater role in reversing assimilation into contrast effects (see the mechanism description). Third, in direct state induction, contrast effects should never occur in the induced state—unless brought about by additionally working inferential processes.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Bidirectionality</title>
<p>Turning the independent variable in embodiment studies, the bodily state, into the dependent variable and testing whether the converse effect holds as well, has sometimes been suggested as a test for sensorimotor simulation against modal priming (e.g.,
<xref rid="B110" ref-type="bibr">Schneider et al., 2011</xref>
). However, as outlined above, most modal priming effects are bidirectional—as are effects driven by direct state induction and sensorimotor simulation. Thus, bidirectionality is no distinguishing characteristic for the presently proposed mechanisms.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec sec-type="conclusions">
<title>Conclusion</title>
<p>We have proposed three mechanisms of how the human body, its actions and states, can influence human cognition and behavior—each with distinct properties and distinct consequences. Crucially, we argue that across psychological subdisciplines, these mechanisms and their joint impact can account for the bulk of existing embodiment effects
<sup>
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="fn02">2</xref>
</sup>
.</p>
<p>On the other hand, classifying embodiment effects with respect to subject matter or semantic category seems less suitable when causal mechanisms are of interest, because effects within the same subject domain may be caused by different mechanisms. For example, approach–avoidance effects can be differentially caused, either because they are directly induced by a motivational orientation or because they are (in)compatible with a sensorimotor simulation. Thus, for embodiment to become a truly useful integrative framework for psychology (
<xref rid="B111" ref-type="bibr">Schubert and Semin, 2009</xref>
;
<xref rid="B54" ref-type="bibr">Glenberg, 2010</xref>
), it is necessary to understand its underlying mechanisms. We believe our framework is a valuable contribution that can be tested and refined in the future.</p>
<p>Additionally, we have derived empirical standards for how the working of each of these mechanisms can be tested for a given effect, and how the specific contributions of a joint impact of these routes can be dissociated. With that we aim to provide a guideline for future research targeted at understanding the operating mechanisms. A systematically spelled-out distinction of underlying processes with tests to distinguish the processes is vital when trying to resolve debates about what might cause a certain embodiment effect. For example, the current controversy about the effects of expanded posture on power or power-related states or behavior (see
<xref rid="B64" ref-type="bibr">Huang et al., 2011</xref>
;
<xref rid="B30" ref-type="bibr">Cesario and McDonald, 2013</xref>
;
<xref rid="B99" ref-type="bibr">Park et al., 2013</xref>
) could profit from clear statements about what is considered the basic state affected by an expanded posture, and what constitutes decisive tests for distinguishing the mechanism.</p>
<p>Recent theoretical work has advanced distinctions of embodiment effects similar to ours. Specifically our first two mechanisms somewhat resemble
<xref rid="B36" ref-type="bibr">Cohen and Leung’s (2009)</xref>
pre-wired (direct state induction) and totem (modal priming) embodiments, respectively. Both pre-wired embodiment and direct state induction are seen as stable and universal. Similarly, both totem embodiments and modal priming rely heavily on learning. However, direct state induction is a narrower category than pre-wired embodiment, while modal priming is broader and can include non-arbitrary associations. Moreover, our conceptualization seems to be more comprehensive, encompassing both purely cognitive and sensorimotor simulation effects. Moreover, by focusing on the mechanisms, our framework is naturally more explicit about cognitive processes.</p>
<p>Other research programs are more distantly related to our approach. For example, simulation effects can be split into online and offline effects (
<xref rid="B93" ref-type="bibr">Niedenthal et al., 2005</xref>
). This distinction can help elucidating the many different effects that simulation can have on cognition and behavior, but essentially rests on one mechanism. Other approaches investigate the phylogenetic and ontogenetic precursors of embodiment (e.g.,
<xref rid="B134" ref-type="bibr">Williams et al., 2009</xref>
;
<xref rid="B6" ref-type="bibr">Bargh et al., 2012</xref>
;
<xref rid="B24" ref-type="bibr">Casasanto, 2014</xref>
). While certainly an intriguing question, it does not necessarily advance our knowledge of the underlying mechanisms, as there is no one-to-one mapping of the developmental course and operating mechanism. Similarly, investigating where the boundaries of embodiment are, which functions are embodied, and to what degree (e.g.,
<xref rid="B85" ref-type="bibr">Mahon and Caramazza, 2008</xref>
;
<xref rid="B83" ref-type="bibr">Louwerse and Jeuniaux, 2010</xref>
), will certainly advance our understanding of information processing, but it is an endeavor quite distinct from distinguishing different embodiment mechanisms when they are operating.</p>
<p>To conclude, we have proposed a comprehensive framework consisting of three distinct mechanisms that explain how the body may shape the mind. With this distinction and our emphasis on empirical tests for the proposed mechanisms, we hope to stimulate further research on the mechanisms that underlie the surprising phenomena of embodiment.</p>
<sec>
<title>Conflict of Interest Statement</title>
<p>The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
</body>
<back>
<ack>
<title>Acknowledgments</title>
<p>This publication was funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG) and the University of Wuerzburg in the funding programme Open Access Publishing. Sascha Topolinski was supported by the German Research Foundation (DFG, TO 705/1-1, Str 264/26-1).</p>
</ack>
<fn-group>
<fn id="fn01">
<label>1</label>
<p>Indeed, the mechanisms have even been proposed in this or similar form to explain the effects we use as illustrations for the mechanisms, though sometimes without any empirical support for the working of the proposed mechanism.</p>
</fn>
<fn id="fn02">
<label>2</label>
<p>One mechanism that we did not mention relies on polarity correspondence (
<xref rid="B104" ref-type="bibr">Proctor and Cho, 2006</xref>
;
<xref rid="B74" ref-type="bibr">Lakens, 2012</xref>
). That is, some congruency effects between physical and psychological dimensions can be explained convincingly solely by polarity differences instead of invoking priming or simulation.</p>
</fn>
</fn-group>
<ref-list>
<title>References</title>
<ref id="B1">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ackerman</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nocera</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bargh</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Incidental haptic sensations influence social judgments and decisions</article-title>
.
<source>Science</source>
<volume>328</volume>
,
<fpage>1712</fpage>
<lpage>1715</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1126/science.1189993</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20576894</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B2">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Adam</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Galinsky</surname>
<given-names>A. D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Enclothed cognition</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>48</volume>
,
<fpage>918</fpage>
<lpage>925</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jesp.2012.02.008</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B3">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Alban</surname>
<given-names>M. W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kelley</surname>
<given-names>C. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Embodiment meets metamemory: weight as a cue for metacognitive judgments</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn.</source>
<volume>39</volume>
,
<fpage>1628</fpage>
<lpage>1634</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0032420</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23565793</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B4">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Arrighi</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cartocci</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Burr</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Reduced perceptual sensitivity for biological motion in paraplegia patients</article-title>
.
<source>Curr. Biol.</source>
<volume>21</volume>
,
<fpage>R910</fpage>
<lpage>R911</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.cub.2011.09.048</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22115454</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B5">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bargh</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Chen</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Burrows</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1996</year>
).
<article-title>Automaticity of social behavior: direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action</article-title>
.
<source>J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>71</volume>
,
<fpage>230</fpage>
<lpage>244</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8765481</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B6">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bargh</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schwader</surname>
<given-names>K. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hailey</surname>
<given-names>S. E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dyer</surname>
<given-names>R. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Boothby</surname>
<given-names>E. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Automaticity in social-cognitive processes</article-title>
.
<source>Trends Cogn. Sci.</source>
<volume>16</volume>
,
<fpage>593</fpage>
<lpage>605</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.002</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23127330</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B7">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<article-title>Perceptual symbol systems</article-title>
.
<source>Behav. Brain Sci.</source>
<volume>22</volume>
,
<fpage>577</fpage>
<lpage>660</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1017/S0140525X99002149</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11301525</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B8">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Grounded cognition</article-title>
.
<source>Annu. Rev. Psychol.</source>
<volume>59</volume>
,
<fpage>617</fpage>
<lpage>645</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17705682</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B9">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Grounded cognition: past, present, and future</article-title>
.
<source>Top. Cogn. Sci.</source>
<volume>2</volume>
,
<fpage>716</fpage>
<lpage>724</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01115.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">25164052</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B10">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Niedenthal</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Barbey</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ruppert</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>“Social embodiment,”</article-title>
in
<source>Psychology of Learning and Motivation</source>
,
<volume>Vol. 43</volume>
, ed.
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Ross</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>San Diego, CA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Academic Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>43</fpage>
<lpage>92</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B11">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Beilock</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Holt</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Embodied preference judgments</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>18</volume>
,
<fpage>51</fpage>
<lpage>57</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01848.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17362378</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B12">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Beilock</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lyons</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mattarella-Micke</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nusbaum</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Small</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Sports experience changes the neural processing of action language</article-title>
.
<source>Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.</source>
<volume>105</volume>
,
<fpage>13269</fpage>
<lpage>13273</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1073/pnas.0803424105</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18765806</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B13">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bhalla</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Proffitt</surname>
<given-names>D. R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<article-title>Visual–motor recalibration in geographical slant perception</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.</source>
<volume>25</volume>
,
<fpage>1076</fpage>
<lpage>1096</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0096-1523.25.4.1076</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10464946</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B14">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bless</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schwarz</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Mental construal and the emergence of assimilation and contrast effects: the inclusion/exclusion model</article-title>
.
<source>Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>42</volume>
,
<fpage>319</fpage>
<lpage>373</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0065-2601(10)42006-7</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B15">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Borghi</surname>
<given-names>A. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cimatti</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Embodied cognition and beyond: acting and sensing the body</article-title>
.
<source>Neuropsychologia</source>
<volume>48</volume>
,
<fpage>763</fpage>
<lpage>773</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.029</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19913041</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B16">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Boroditsky</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors</article-title>
.
<source>Cognition</source>
<volume>75</volume>
,
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>28</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00073-6</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10815775</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B17">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Boroditsky</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers’ conceptions of time</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Psychol.</source>
<volume>43</volume>
,
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>22</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1006/cogp.2001.0748</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11487292</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B18">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Boroditsky</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gaby</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Remembrances of times east: absolute spatial representations of time in an Australian aboriginal community</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>21</volume>
,
<fpage>1635</fpage>
<lpage>1639</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0956797610386621</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20959511</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B19">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Boroditsky</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ramscar</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>The roles of body and mind in abstract thought</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>13</volume>
,
<fpage>185</fpage>
<lpage>189</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/1467-9280.00434</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11934006</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B20">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Buccino</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Binkofski</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fink</surname>
<given-names>G. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fadiga</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fogassi</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gallese</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study</article-title>
.
<source>Eur. J. Neurosci.</source>
<volume>13</volume>
,
<fpage>400</fpage>
<lpage>404</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1460-9568.2001.01385.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11168545</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B21">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cacioppo</surname>
<given-names>J. T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Priester</surname>
<given-names>J. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Berntson</surname>
<given-names>G. G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1993</year>
).
<article-title>Rudimentary determinants of attitudes: II. Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes</article-title>
.
<source>J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>65</volume>
,
<fpage>5</fpage>
<lpage>17</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.5</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8355142</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B22">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Campos</surname>
<given-names>J. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Anderson</surname>
<given-names>D. I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Barbu-Roth</surname>
<given-names>M. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hubbard</surname>
<given-names>E. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hertenstein</surname>
<given-names>M. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Witherington</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Travel broadens the mind</article-title>
.
<source>Infancy</source>
<volume>1</volume>
,
<fpage>149</fpage>
<lpage>219</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1207/S15327078IN0102_1</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B23">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Casasanto</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Embodiment of abstract concepts: good and bad in right- and left-handers</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.</source>
<volume>138</volume>
,
<fpage>351</fpage>
<lpage>367</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0015854</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19653795</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B24">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Casasanto</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2014</year>
).
<article-title>“Experiential origins of mental metaphors: Language, culture, and body,”</article-title>
in
<source>The Power of Metaphor: Examining its Influence on Social Life</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Landau</surname>
<given-names>M. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Robinson</surname>
<given-names>M. D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Meier</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Washington, DC</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>American Psychological Association</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>249</fpage>
<lpage>268</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/14278-011</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B25">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Casasanto</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Boroditsky</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Time in the mind: using space to think about time</article-title>
.
<source>Cognition</source>
<volume>106</volume>
,
<fpage>579</fpage>
<lpage>593</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.004</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17509553</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B26">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Casasanto</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Chrysikou</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>When left is “right”: motor fluency shapes abstract concepts</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>22</volume>
,
<fpage>419</fpage>
<lpage>422</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0956797611401755</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21389336</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B27">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Casasanto</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dijkstra</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Motor action and emotional memory</article-title>
.
<source>Cognition</source>
<volume>115</volume>
,
<fpage>179</fpage>
<lpage>185</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.cognition.2009.11.002</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20096831</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B28">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Casile</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Giese</surname>
<given-names>M. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Nonvisual motor training influences biological motion perception</article-title>
.
<source>Curr. Biol.</source>
<volume>16</volume>
,
<fpage>69</fpage>
<lpage>74</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.cub.2005.10.071</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16401424</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B29">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Centerbar</surname>
<given-names>D. B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Clore</surname>
<given-names>G. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Do approach-avoidance actions create attitudes?</article-title>
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>17</volume>
,
<fpage>22</fpage>
<lpage>29</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01660.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16371140</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B30">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cesario</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McDonald</surname>
<given-names>M. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Bodies in context: power poses as a computation of action possibility</article-title>
.
<source>Soc. Cogn.</source>
<volume>31</volume>
,
<fpage>260</fpage>
<lpage>274</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1521/soco.2013.31.2.260</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B31">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Chandler</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Reinhard</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schwarz</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>To judge a book by its weight you need to know its content: knowledge moderates the use of embodied cues</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>48</volume>
,
<fpage>948</fpage>
<lpage>952</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jesp.2012.03.003</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B32">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Chandler</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schwarz</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>How extending your middle finger affects your perception of others: learned movements influence concept accessibility</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>45</volume>
,
<fpage>123</fpage>
<lpage>128</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jesp.2008.06.012</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B33">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Chemero</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Radical embodied cognitive science</article-title>
.
<source>Rev. Gen. Psychol.</source>
<volume>17</volume>
,
<fpage>145</fpage>
<lpage>150</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0032923</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B34">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Chen</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bargh</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<article-title>Consequences of automatic evaluation: immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus</article-title>
.
<source>Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull.</source>
<volume>25</volume>
,
<fpage>215</fpage>
<lpage>224</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0146167299025002007</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B35">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Clark</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<article-title>An embodied cognitive science?</article-title>
<source>Trends Cogn. Sci.</source>
<volume>3</volume>
,
<fpage>345</fpage>
<lpage>351</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01361-3</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10461197</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B36">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cohen</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Leung</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>The hard embodiment of culture</article-title>
.
<source>Eur. J. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>39</volume>
,
<fpage>1278</fpage>
<lpage>1289</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1002/ejsp.671</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B37">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>DeWall</surname>
<given-names>C. N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bushman</surname>
<given-names>B. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Hot under the collar in a lukewarm environment: words associated with hot temperature increase aggressive thoughts and hostile perceptions</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>45</volume>
,
<fpage>1045</fpage>
<lpage>1047</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jesp.2009.05.003</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B38">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Dimmock</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jackson</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Clarke</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>“Warm interactions” and “Cold streaks”: the influence of temperature word primes on interpersonal warmth and competence-related outcomes in a dyadic sport task</article-title>
.
<source>Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol.</source>
<volume>2</volume>
,
<fpage>102</fpage>
<lpage>116</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0030191</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B39">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Eder</surname>
<given-names>A. B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rothermund</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>When do motor behaviors (mis)match affective stimuli? An evaluative coding view of approach and avoidance reactions</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.</source>
<volume>137</volume>
,
<fpage>262</fpage>
<lpage>281</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.262</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18473659</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B40">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Elder</surname>
<given-names>R. S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Krishna</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>The “visual depiction effect” in advertising: facilitating embodied mental simulation through product orientation</article-title>
.
<source>J. Consum. Res.</source>
<volume>38</volume>
,
<fpage>988</fpage>
<lpage>1003</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1086/661531</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B41">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fadiga</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Craighero</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Buccino</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rizzolatti</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: a TMS study</article-title>
.
<source>Eur. J. Neurosci.</source>
<volume>15</volume>
,
<fpage>399</fpage>
<lpage>402</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01874.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11849307</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B42">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fayant</surname>
<given-names>M.-P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Muller</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nurra</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Alexopoulos</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Palluel-Germain</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Moving forward is not only a metaphor: approach and avoidance lead to self-evaluative assimilation and contrast</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>47</volume>
,
<fpage>241</fpage>
<lpage>245</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jesp.2010.07.013</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B43">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fischer</surname>
<given-names>M. H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zwaan</surname>
<given-names>R. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Embodied language: a review of the role of the motor system in language comprehension</article-title>
.
<source>Q. J. Exp. Psychol.</source>
<volume>61</volume>
,
<fpage>825</fpage>
<lpage>850</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/17470210701623605</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18470815</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B44">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Flusberg</surname>
<given-names>S. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thibodeau</surname>
<given-names>P. H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sternberg</surname>
<given-names>D. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Glick</surname>
<given-names>J. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>A connectionist approach to embodied conceptual metaphor</article-title>
.
<source>Front. Psychol.</source>
<volume>1</volume>
:
<fpage>197</fpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00197</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21833256</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B45">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Foroni</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Semin</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Language that puts you in touch with your bodily feelings. The multimodal responsiveness of affective expressions</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>20</volume>
,
<fpage>974</fpage>
<lpage>980</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02400.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19594858</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B46">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Foroni</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Semin</surname>
<given-names>G. R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>When does mimicry affect evaluative judgment?</article-title>
<source>Emotion</source>
<volume>11</volume>
,
<fpage>687</fpage>
<lpage>690</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0023163</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21668115</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B47">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Foroni</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Semin</surname>
<given-names>G. R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Not all implicit measures of attitudes are created equal: evidence from an embodiment perspective</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>48</volume>
,
<fpage>424</fpage>
<lpage>427</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jesp.2011.08.015</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B48">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Förster</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>The influence of approach and avoidance motor actions on food intake</article-title>
.
<source>Eur. J. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>33</volume>
,
<fpage>339</fpage>
<lpage>350</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1002/ejsp.150</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B49">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Förster</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Liberman</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Friedman</surname>
<given-names>R. S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>“What do we prime? On distinguishing between semantic priming, procedural priming, and goal priming,”</article-title>
in
<source>Oxford Handbook of Human Action. Social Cognition and Social Neuroscience</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Morsella</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bargh</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gollwitzer</surname>
<given-names>P. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>173</fpage>
<lpage>193</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B50">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Förster</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Strack</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1996</year>
).
<article-title>Influence of overt head movements on memory for valenced words: a case of conceptual-motor compatibility</article-title>
.
<source>J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>71</volume>
,
<fpage>421</fpage>
<lpage>430</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.71.3.421</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8831156</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B51">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Friedman</surname>
<given-names>R. S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Förster</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>The influence of approach and avoidance cues on attentional flexibility</article-title>
.
<source>Motiv. Emot.</source>
<volume>29</volume>
,
<fpage>69</fpage>
<lpage>81</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s11031-005-7954-4</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B52">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gallese</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Before and below ‘Theory of Mind’: embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition</article-title>
.
<source>Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.</source>
<volume>362</volume>
,
<fpage>659</fpage>
<lpage>669</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1098/rstb.2006.2002</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17301027</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B53">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gallese</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Motor abstraction: a neuroscientific account of how action goals and intentions are mapped and understood</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Res.</source>
<volume>73</volume>
,
<fpage>486</fpage>
<lpage>498</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s00426-009-0232-4</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19381683</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B54">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Glenberg</surname>
<given-names>A. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Embodiment as a unifying perspective for psychology</article-title>
.
<source>Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci.</source>
<volume>1</volume>
,
<fpage>586</fpage>
<lpage>596</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1002/wcs.55</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B55">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Glenberg</surname>
<given-names>A. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kaschak</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Grounding language in action</article-title>
.
<source>Psychon. Bull. Rev.</source>
<volume>9</volume>
,
<fpage>558</fpage>
<lpage>565</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3758/BF03196313</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12412897</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B56">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Glenberg</surname>
<given-names>A. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Witt</surname>
<given-names>J. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Metcalfe</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>From the revolution to embodiment: 25 years of cognitive psychology</article-title>
.
<source>Perspect. Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>8</volume>
,
<fpage>573</fpage>
<lpage>585</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/1745691613498098</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B57">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Goldman</surname>
<given-names>A. I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>de Vignemont</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Is social cognition embodied?</article-title>
<source>Trends Cogn. Sci.</source>
<volume>13</volume>
,
<fpage>154</fpage>
<lpage>159</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.007</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19269881</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B58">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gollwitzer</surname>
<given-names>P. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<article-title>“Action phases and mind-sets,”</article-title>
in
<source>Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of Social Behavior</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Higgins</surname>
<given-names>E. T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sorrentino</surname>
<given-names>R. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Guilford Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>53</fpage>
<lpage>92</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B59">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hamilton</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wolpert</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Frith</surname>
<given-names>U.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Your own action influences how you perceive another person’s action</article-title>
.
<source>Curr. Biol.</source>
<volume>14</volume>
,
<fpage>493</fpage>
<lpage>498</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.cub.2004.03.007</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15043814</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B60">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Havas</surname>
<given-names>D. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Glenberg</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gutowski</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lucarelli</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Davidson</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Cosmetic use of botulinum toxin-A affects processing of emotional language</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>21</volume>
,
<fpage>895</fpage>
<lpage>900</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0956797610374742</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20548056</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B61">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Havas</surname>
<given-names>D. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Glenberg</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rinck</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Emotion simulation during language comprehension</article-title>
.
<source>Psychon. Bull. Rev.</source>
<volume>14</volume>
,
<fpage>436</fpage>
<lpage>441</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3758/BF03194085</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17874584</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B62">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Havas</surname>
<given-names>D. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Matheson</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>The functional role of the periphery in emotional language comprehension</article-title>
.
<source>Front. Psychol.</source>
<volume>4</volume>
:
<fpage>294</fpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00294</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23750145</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B63">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Häfner</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>When body and mind are talking: interoception moderates embodied cognition</article-title>
.
<source>Exp. Psychol.</source>
<volume>60</volume>
,
<fpage>255</fpage>
<lpage>259</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1027/1618-3169/a000194</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23548985</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B64">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Huang</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Galinsky</surname>
<given-names>A. D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gruenfeld</surname>
<given-names>D. H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Guillory</surname>
<given-names>L. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Powerful postures versus powerful roles: which is the proximate correlate of thought and behavior?</article-title>
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>22</volume>
,
<fpage>95</fpage>
<lpage>102</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0956797610391912</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21149853</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B65">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>IJzerman</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Semin</surname>
<given-names>G. R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Temperature perceptions as a ground for social proximity</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>46</volume>
,
<fpage>867</fpage>
<lpage>873</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jesp.2010.07.015</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B66">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Jacobs</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Shiffrar</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Walking perception by walking observers</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.</source>
<volume>31</volume>
,
<fpage>157</fpage>
<lpage>169</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.157</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15709870</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B67">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>James</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1884</year>
).
<article-title>What is an emotion?</article-title>
<source>Mind</source>
<volume>9</volume>
,
<fpage>188</fpage>
<lpage>205</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1093/mind/os-IX.34.188</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B68">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Jones</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fitness</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Moral hypervigilance: the influence of disgust sensitivity in the moral domain</article-title>
.
<source>Emotion</source>
<volume>8</volume>
,
<fpage>613</fpage>
<lpage>627</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0013435</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18837611</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B69">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Jostmann</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lakens</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Weight as an embodiment of importance</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>20</volume>
,
<fpage>1169</fpage>
<lpage>1174</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02426.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19686292</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B70">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Klatzky</surname>
<given-names>R. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pellegrino</surname>
<given-names>J. W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McCloskey</surname>
<given-names>B. P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Doherty</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1989</year>
).
<article-title>Can you squeeze a tomato? The role of motor representations in semantic sensibility judgments</article-title>
.
<source>J. Mem. Lang.</source>
<volume>28</volume>
,
<fpage>56</fpage>
<lpage>77</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0749-596X(89)90028-4</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B71">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Koch</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Holland</surname>
<given-names>R. W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>van Knippenberg</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Regulating cognitive control through approach-avoidance motor actions</article-title>
.
<source>Cognition</source>
<volume>109</volume>
,
<fpage>133</fpage>
<lpage>142</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.cognition.2008.07.014</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18835601</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B72">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kouchaki</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gino</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jami</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2014</year>
).
<article-title>The burden of guilt: heavy backpacks, light snacks, and enhanced morality</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.</source>
<volume>143</volume>
,
<fpage>414</fpage>
<lpage>424</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0031769</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23398182</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B73">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Krieglmeyer</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Deutsch</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>De Houwer</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>De Raedt</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Being moved valence activates approach-avoidance behavior independently of evaluation and approach-avoidance intentions</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>21</volume>
,
<fpage>607</fpage>
<lpage>613</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0956797610365131</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20424109</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B74">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lakens</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Polarity correspondence in metaphor congruency effects: structural overlap predicts categorization times for bipolar concepts presented in vertical space</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn.</source>
<volume>38</volume>
,
<fpage>726</fpage>
<lpage>736</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0024955</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21843022</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B75">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lakoff</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Johnson</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1980</year>
).
<source>Metaphors We Live By</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Chicago, IL</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Chicago press</publisher-name>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B76">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lakoff</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Johnson</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<source>Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and its Challenge to Western Thought</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Basic books</publisher-name>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B77">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Landau</surname>
<given-names>M. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Meier</surname>
<given-names>B. P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Keefer</surname>
<given-names>L. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>A metaphor-enriched social cognition</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Bull.</source>
<volume>136</volume>
,
<fpage>1045</fpage>
<lpage>1067</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0020970</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20822208</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B78">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Leder</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bär</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Topolinski</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Covert painting simulations influence aesthetic appreciation of artworks</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>23</volume>
,
<fpage>1479</fpage>
<lpage>1481</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0956797612452866</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23137968</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B79">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lee</surname>
<given-names>S. W. S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schwarz</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Washing away postdecisional dissonance</article-title>
.
<source>Science</source>
<volume>328</volume>
,
<fpage>709</fpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1126/science.1186799</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20448177</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B80">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lee</surname>
<given-names>S. W. S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schwarz</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Bidirectionality, mediation, and moderation of metaphorical effects: the embodiment of social suspicion and fishy smells</article-title>
.
<source>J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>103</volume>
,
<fpage>737</fpage>
<lpage>749</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0029708</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22905770</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B81">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lewis</surname>
<given-names>M. B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bowler</surname>
<given-names>P. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Botulinum toxin cosmetic therapy correlates with a more positive mood</article-title>
.
<source>J. Cosmet. Dermatol.</source>
<volume>8</volume>
,
<fpage>24</fpage>
<lpage>26</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1473-2165.2009.00419.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19250162</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B82">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Loersch</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Payne</surname>
<given-names>B. K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>The situated inference model: an integrative account of the effects of primes on perception, behavior, and motivation</article-title>
.
<source>Perspect. Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>6</volume>
,
<fpage>234</fpage>
<lpage>252</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/1745691611406921</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B83">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Louwerse</surname>
<given-names>M. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jeuniaux</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>The linguistic and embodied nature of conceptual processing</article-title>
.
<source>Cognition</source>
<volume>114</volume>
,
<fpage>96</fpage>
<lpage>104</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.002</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19818435</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B84">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Machery</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Concept empiricism: a methodological critique</article-title>
.
<source>Cognition</source>
<volume>104</volume>
,
<fpage>19</fpage>
<lpage>46</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.cognition.2006.05.002</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16814274</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B85">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Mahon</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Caramazza</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content</article-title>
.
<source>J. Physiol. Paris</source>
<volume>102</volume>
,
<fpage>59</fpage>
<lpage>70</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.004</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18448316</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B86">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Markman</surname>
<given-names>A. B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brendl</surname>
<given-names>C. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Constraining theories of embodied cognition</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>16</volume>
,
<fpage>6</fpage>
<lpage>10</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00772.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15660844</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B87">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Meier</surname>
<given-names>B. P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schnall</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schwarz</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bargh</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Embodiment in social psychology</article-title>
.
<source>Top. Cogn. Sci.</source>
<volume>4</volume>
,
<fpage>705</fpage>
<lpage>716</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01212.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22777820</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B88">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Michael</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sandberg</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Skewes</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wolf</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Blicher</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Overgaard</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
(
<year>2014</year>
).
<article-title>Continuous theta-burst stimulation demonstrates a causal role of premotor homunculus in action understanding</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>25</volume>
,
<fpage>963</fpage>
<lpage>972</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0956797613520608</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">24549297</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B89">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Moors</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>De Houwer</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Automaticity: a theoretical and conceptual analysis</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Bull.</source>
<volume>132</volume>
,
<fpage>297</fpage>
<lpage>326</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.297</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16536645</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B90">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Mussweiler</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Doing is for thinking! Stereotype activation by stereotypic movements</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>17</volume>
,
<fpage>17</fpage>
<lpage>21</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01659.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16371139</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B91">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Neumann</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Förster</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Strack</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>“Motor compatibility: the bidirectional link between behavior and evaluation,”</article-title>
in
<source>The Psychology of Evaluation: Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Musch</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Klauer</surname>
<given-names>K. C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Mahwah, NJ</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Lawrence Erlbaum Associates</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>371</fpage>
<lpage>391</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B92">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Niedenthal</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Embodying emotion</article-title>
.
<source>Science</source>
<volume>316</volume>
,
<fpage>1002</fpage>
<lpage>1005</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1126/science.1136930</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17510358</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B93">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Niedenthal</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Winkielman</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Krauth-Gruber</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ric</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion</article-title>
.
<source>Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev.</source>
<volume>9</volume>
,
<fpage>184</fpage>
<lpage>211</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1207/s15327957pspr0903_1</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16083360</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B94">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Niedenthal</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brauer</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Halberstadt</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Innes-Ker</surname>
<given-names>Å.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>When did her smile drop? Facial mimicry and the influences of emotional state on the detection of change in emotional expression</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Emot.</source>
<volume>15</volume>
,
<fpage>853</fpage>
<lpage>864</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/02699930143000194</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B95">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Niedenthal</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Winkielman</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mondillon</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Vermeulen</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Embodiment of emotion concepts</article-title>
.
<source>J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>96</volume>
,
<fpage>1120</fpage>
<lpage>1136</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0015574</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19469591</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B96">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Nussinson</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Seibt</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Häfner</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Strack</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Come a bit closer: approach motor actions lead to feeling similar and behavioral assimilation</article-title>
.
<source>Soc. Cogn.</source>
<volume>28</volume>
,
<fpage>40</fpage>
<lpage>58</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1521/soco.2010.28.1.40</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B97">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Oberman</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Winkielman</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ramachandran</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Face to face: blocking facial mimicry can selectively impair recognition of emotional expressions</article-title>
.
<source>Soc. Neurosci.</source>
<volume>2</volume>
,
<fpage>167</fpage>
<lpage>178</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/17470910701391943</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18633815</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B98">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Papeo</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Vallesi</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Isaja</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rumiati</surname>
<given-names>R. I.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Effects of TMS on different stages of motor and non-motor verb processing in the primary motor cortex</article-title>
.
<source>PLoS ONE</source>
<volume>4</volume>
:
<fpage>e4508</fpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0004508</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19240793</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B99">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Park</surname>
<given-names>L. E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Streamer</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Huang</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Galinsky</surname>
<given-names>A. D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Stand tall, but don’t put your feet up: universal and culturally-specific effects of expansive postures on power</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>49</volume>
,
<fpage>965</fpage>
<lpage>971</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jesp.2013.06.001</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B100">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Pazzaglia</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pizzamiglio</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pes</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Aglioti</surname>
<given-names>S. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>The sound of actions in apraxia</article-title>
.
<source>Curr. Biol.</source>
<volume>18</volume>
,
<fpage>1766</fpage>
<lpage>1772</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.cub.2008.09.061</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19013068</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B101">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Pecher</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Winkielman</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>“Grounded cognition and social interaction,”</article-title>
in
<source>Encyclopedia of Philosophy and the Social Sciences</source>
, ed.
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Kaldis</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Thousand Oaks, CA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Sage</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>396</fpage>
<lpage>397</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B102">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Pecher</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zeelenberg</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>L. W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Verifying different-modality properties for concepts produces switching costs</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>14</volume>
,
<fpage>119</fpage>
<lpage>124</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01429</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12661672</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B103">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Prinz</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<article-title>“A common-coding approach to perception and action,”</article-title>
in
<source>Relationships Between Perception and Action: Current Approaches</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Neumann</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Prinz</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Berlin</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Springer</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>167</fpage>
<lpage>201</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B104">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Proctor</surname>
<given-names>R. W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cho</surname>
<given-names>Y. S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Polarity correspondence: a general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Bull.</source>
<volume>132</volume>
,
<fpage>416</fpage>
<lpage>442</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.416</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16719568</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B105">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Proffitt</surname>
<given-names>D. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Stefanucci</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Banton</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Epstein</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>The role of effort in perceiving distance</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>14</volume>
,
<fpage>106</fpage>
<lpage>112</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01427</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12661670</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B106">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Reed</surname>
<given-names>C. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McGoldrick</surname>
<given-names>J. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Action during body perception: processing time affects self–other correspondences</article-title>
.
<source>Soc. Neurosci.</source>
<volume>2</volume>
,
<fpage>134</fpage>
<lpage>147</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/17470910701376811</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18633812</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B107">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rizzolatti</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Craighero</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>The mirror-neuron system</article-title>
.
<source>Annu. Rev. Neurosci.</source>
<volume>27</volume>
,
<fpage>169</fpage>
<lpage>192</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15217330</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B108">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Russell</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Rev.</source>
<volume>110</volume>
,
<fpage>145</fpage>
<lpage>172</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12529060</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B109">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sato</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mengarelli</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Riggio</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gallese</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Buccino</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Task related modulation of the motor system during language processing</article-title>
.
<source>Brain Lang.</source>
<volume>105</volume>
,
<fpage>83</fpage>
<lpage>90</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.bandl.2007.10.001</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18054379</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B110">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schneider</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rutjens</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jostmann</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lakens</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Weighty matters: importance literally feels heavy</article-title>
.
<source>Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci.</source>
<volume>2</volume>
,
<fpage>474</fpage>
<lpage>478</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/1948550610397895</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B111">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Semin</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Embodiment as a unifying perspective for psychology</article-title>
.
<source>Eur. J. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>39</volume>
,
<fpage>1135</fpage>
<lpage>1141</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1002/ejsp.670</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B112">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schwarz</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>“Feelings-as-information theory,”</article-title>
in
<source>Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Van Lange</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kruglanski</surname>
<given-names>A. W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Higgins</surname>
<given-names>E. T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Thousand Oaks, CA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Sage</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>289</fpage>
<lpage>308</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B113">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schwarz</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bless</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1992</year>
).
<article-title>“Constructing reality and its alternatives: an inclusion/exclusion model of assimilation and contrast effects in social judgment,”</article-title>
in
<source>The Construction of Social Judgments</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Martin</surname>
<given-names>L. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tesser</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Hillsdale, NJ</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Lawrence Erlbaum Associates</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>217</fpage>
<lpage>245</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B114">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schwarz</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Servay</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kumpf</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1985</year>
).
<article-title>Attribution of arousal as a mediator of the effectiveness of fear-arousing communications</article-title>
.
<source>J. Appl. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>15</volume>
,
<fpage>74</fpage>
<lpage>78</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1559-1816.1985.tb02343.x</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B115">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Seibt</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Neumann</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nussinson</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Strack</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Movement direction or change in distance? Self-and object-related approach–avoidance motions</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>44</volume>
,
<fpage>713</fpage>
<lpage>720</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jesp.2007.04.013</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B116">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Shen</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sengupta</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>If you can’t grab it, it won’t grab you: the effect of restricting the dominant hand on target evaluations</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>48</volume>
,
<fpage>525</fpage>
<lpage>529</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jesp.2011.11.003</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B117">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Shtyrov</surname>
<given-names>Y.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Butorina</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nikolaeva</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Stroganova</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2014</year>
).
<article-title>Automatic ultrarapid activation and inhibition of cortical motor systems in spoken word comprehension</article-title>
.
<source>Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.</source>
<volume>111</volume>
,
<fpage>E1918</fpage>
<lpage>E1923</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1073/pnas.1323158111</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">24753617</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B118">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>E. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Branscombe</surname>
<given-names>N. R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1987</year>
).
<article-title>Procedurally mediated social inferences: the case of category accessibility effects</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>23</volume>
,
<fpage>361</fpage>
<lpage>382</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0022-1031(87)90036-9</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B119">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>E. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>DeCoster</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems</article-title>
.
<source>Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev.</source>
<volume>4</volume>
,
<fpage>108</fpage>
<lpage>131</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1207/S15327957PSPR0402_01</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B120">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>E. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Semin</surname>
<given-names>G. R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Socially situated cognition: cognition in its social context</article-title>
.
<source>Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>36</volume>
,
<fpage>53</fpage>
<lpage>117</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0065-2601(04)36002-8</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B121">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Solarz</surname>
<given-names>A. K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1960</year>
).
<article-title>Latency of instrumental responses as a function of compatibility with the meaning of eliciting verbal signs</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Psychol.</source>
<volume>59</volume>
,
<fpage>239</fpage>
<lpage>245</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/h0047274</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">13832584</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B122">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Solomon</surname>
<given-names>K. O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Barsalou</surname>
<given-names>L. W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Perceptual simulation in property verification</article-title>
.
<source>Mem. Cogn.</source>
<volume>32</volume>
,
<fpage>244</fpage>
<lpage>259</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3758/BF03196856</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15190717</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B123">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sommerville</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Woodward</surname>
<given-names>A. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Needham</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Action experience alters 3-month-old infants’ perception of others’ actions</article-title>
.
<source>Cognition</source>
<volume>96</volume>
,
<fpage>B1</fpage>
<lpage>B11</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.cognition.2004.07.004</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15833301</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B124">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Stanfield</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zwaan</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>The effect of implied orientation derived from verbal context on picture recognition</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>12</volume>
,
<fpage>153</fpage>
<lpage>156</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/1467-9280.00326</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11340925</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B125">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Strack</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Deutsch</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior</article-title>
.
<source>Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev.</source>
<volume>8</volume>
,
<fpage>220</fpage>
<lpage>247</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15454347</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B126">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Strack</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Martin</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Stepper</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1988</year>
).
<article-title>Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: a nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis</article-title>
.
<source>J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>54</volume>
,
<fpage>768</fpage>
<lpage>777</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.768</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">3379579</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B127">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Topolinski</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Moving the eye of the beholder: motor components in vision determine aesthetic preference</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci.</source>
<volume>21</volume>
,
<fpage>1220</fpage>
<lpage>1224</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0956797610378308</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20639401</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B128">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Topolinski</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Strack</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Motormouth: mere exposure depends on stimulus-specific motor simulations</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn.</source>
<volume>35</volume>
,
<fpage>423</fpage>
<lpage>433</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0014504</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19271856</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B129">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tucker</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ellis</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1998</year>
).
<article-title>On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.</source>
<volume>24</volume>
,
<fpage>830</fpage>
<lpage>846</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.830</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">9627419</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B130">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Urgesi</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Candidi</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ionta</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Aglioti</surname>
<given-names>S. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Representation of body identity and body actions in extrastriate body area and ventral premotor cortex</article-title>
.
<source>Nat. Neurosci.</source>
<volume>10</volume>
,
<fpage>30</fpage>
<lpage>31</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1038/nn1815</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17159990</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B131">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wheeler</surname>
<given-names>S. C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>DeMarree</surname>
<given-names>K. G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Petty</surname>
<given-names>R. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Understanding the role of the self in prime-to-behavior effects: the active-self account</article-title>
.
<source>Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev.</source>
<volume>11</volume>
,
<fpage>234</fpage>
<lpage>261</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/1088868307302223</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18453463</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B132">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Willems</surname>
<given-names>R. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Casasanto</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Flexibility in embodied language understanding</article-title>
.
<source>Front. Psychol.</source>
<volume>2</volume>
:
<fpage>116</fpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00116</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21779264</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B133">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Willems</surname>
<given-names>R. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Francken</surname>
<given-names>J. C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Embodied cognition: taking the next step</article-title>
.
<source>Front. Psychol.</source>
<volume>3</volume>
:
<fpage>582</fpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00582</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23293620</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B134">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Williams</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Huang</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bargh</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>The scaffolded mind: higher mental processes are grounded in early experience of the physical world</article-title>
.
<source>Eur. J. Soc. Psychol.</source>
<volume>39</volume>
,
<fpage>1257</fpage>
<lpage>1267</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1002/ejsp.665</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20046813</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B135">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wilson</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Six views of embodied cognition</article-title>
.
<source>Psychon. Bull. Rev.</source>
<volume>9</volume>
,
<fpage>625</fpage>
<lpage>636</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3758/BF03196322</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12613670</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B136">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wollmer</surname>
<given-names>M. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>de Boer</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kalak</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Beck</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Götz</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schmidt</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Facing depression with botulinum toxin: a randomized controlled trial</article-title>
.
<source>J. Psychiatr. Res.</source>
<volume>46</volume>
,
<fpage>574</fpage>
<lpage>581</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.01.027</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22364892</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B137">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Xu</surname>
<given-names>A. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zwick</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schwarz</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Washing away your (good or bad) luck: physical cleansing affects risk-taking behavior</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.</source>
<volume>141</volume>
,
<fpage>26</fpage>
<lpage>30</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0023997</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21707206</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B138">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Yan</surname>
<given-names>Z.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ding</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Yan</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>To wash your body, or purify your soul: physical cleansing would strengthen the sense of high moral character</article-title>
.
<source>Psychology</source>
<volume>2</volume>
,
<fpage>992</fpage>
<lpage>997</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.4236/psych.2011.29149</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B139">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Zajonc</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Markus</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1988</year>
).
<article-title>“Affect and cognition: the hard interface,”</article-title>
in
<source>Emotions, Cognition, and Behavior</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Izard</surname>
<given-names>C. E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kagan</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zajonc</surname>
<given-names>R. B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Cambridge, UK</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>73</fpage>
<lpage>102</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B140">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Zhang</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Li</surname>
<given-names>X.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>From physical weight to psychological significance: the contribution of semantic activations</article-title>
.
<source>J. Consum. Res.</source>
<volume>38</volume>
,
<fpage>1063</fpage>
<lpage>1075</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1086/661768</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B141">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Zhong</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Liljenquist</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Washing away your sins: threatened morality and physical cleansing</article-title>
.
<source>Science</source>
<volume>313</volume>
,
<fpage>1451</fpage>
<lpage>1452</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1126/science.1130726</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16960010</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B142">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Zwaan</surname>
<given-names>R. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Taylor</surname>
<given-names>L. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Seeing, acting, understanding: motor resonance in language comprehension</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.</source>
<volume>135</volume>
,
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>11</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0096-3445.135.1.1</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16478313</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B143">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Zwickel</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Prinz</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Assimilation and contrast: the two sides of specific interference between action and perception</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Res.</source>
<volume>76</volume>
,
<fpage>171</fpage>
<lpage>182</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s00426-011-0338-3</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21556808</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</pmc>
<affiliations>
<list>
<country>
<li>Allemagne</li>
</country>
</list>
<tree>
<country name="Allemagne">
<noRegion>
<name sortKey="Korner, Anita" sort="Korner, Anita" uniqKey="Korner A" first="Anita" last="Körner">Anita Körner</name>
</noRegion>
<name sortKey="Strack, Fritz" sort="Strack, Fritz" uniqKey="Strack F" first="Fritz" last="Strack">Fritz Strack</name>
<name sortKey="Topolinski, Sascha" sort="Topolinski, Sascha" uniqKey="Topolinski S" first="Sascha" last="Topolinski">Sascha Topolinski</name>
</country>
</tree>
</affiliations>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Ticri/CIDE/explor/HapticV1/Data/Pmc/Checkpoint
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 000357 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Checkpoint/biblio.hfd -nk 000357 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Ticri/CIDE
   |area=    HapticV1
   |flux=    Pmc
   |étape=   Checkpoint
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     PMC:4488596
   |texte=   Routes to embodiment
}}

Pour générer des pages wiki

HfdIndexSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Checkpoint/RBID.i   -Sk "pubmed:26191033" \
       | HfdSelect -Kh $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Checkpoint/biblio.hfd   \
       | NlmPubMed2Wicri -a HapticV1 

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.23.
Data generation: Mon Jun 13 01:09:46 2016. Site generation: Wed Mar 6 09:54:07 2024