Serveur d'exploration sur les dispositifs haptiques

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

Ambiguity in Tactile Apparent Motion Perception

Identifieur interne : 000167 ( Pmc/Checkpoint ); précédent : 000166; suivant : 000168

Ambiguity in Tactile Apparent Motion Perception

Auteurs : Emanuela Liaci [Allemagne] ; Michael Bach [Allemagne] ; Ludger Tebartz Van Elst [Allemagne] ; Sven P. Heinrich [Allemagne] ; Jürgen Kornmeier [Allemagne]

Source :

RBID : PMC:4865082

Abstract

Background

In von Schiller’s Stroboscopic Alternative Motion (SAM) stimulus two visually presented diagonal dot pairs, located on the corners of an imaginary rectangle, alternate with each other and induce either horizontal, vertical or, rarely, rotational motion percepts. SAM motion perception can be described by a psychometric function of the dot aspect ratio (“AR”, i.e. the relation between vertical and horizontal dot distances). Further, with equal horizontal and vertical dot distances (AR = 1) perception is biased towards vertical motion. In a series of five experiments, we presented tactile SAM versions and studied the role of AR and of different reference frames for the perception of tactile apparent motion.

Methods

We presented tactile SAM stimuli and varied the ARs, while participants reported the perceived motion directions. Pairs of vibration stimulators were attached to the participants’ forearms and stimulator distances were varied within and between forearms. We compared straight and rotated forearm conditions with each other in order to disentangle the roles of exogenous and endogenous reference frames.

Results

Increasing the tactile SAM’s AR biased perception towards vertical motion, but the effect was weak compared to the visual modality. We found no horizontal disambiguation, even for very small tactile ARs. A forearm rotation by 90° kept the vertical bias, even though it was now coupled with small ARs. A 45° rotation condition with crossed forearms, however, evoked a strong horizontal motion bias.

Discussion

Existing approaches to explain the visual SAM bias fail to explain the current tactile results. Particularly puzzling is the strong horizontal bias in the crossed-forearm conditions. In the case of tactile apparent motion, there seem to be no fixed priority rule for perceptual disambiguation. Rather the weighting of available evidence seems to depend on the degree of stimulus ambiguity, the current situation and on the perceptual strategy of the individual observer.


Url:
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0152736
PubMed: 27171276
PubMed Central: 4865082


Affiliations:


Links toward previous steps (curation, corpus...)


Links to Exploration step

PMC:4865082

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI>
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">Ambiguity in Tactile Apparent Motion Perception</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Liaci, Emanuela" sort="Liaci, Emanuela" uniqKey="Liaci E" first="Emanuela" last="Liaci">Emanuela Liaci</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff001">
<addr-line>Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology and Mental Health, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology and Mental Health, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff002">
<addr-line>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff003">
<addr-line>Clinic for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Clinic for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff004">
<addr-line>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bach, Michael" sort="Bach, Michael" uniqKey="Bach M" first="Michael" last="Bach">Michael Bach</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff002">
<addr-line>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff004">
<addr-line>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tebartz Van Elst, Ludger" sort="Tebartz Van Elst, Ludger" uniqKey="Tebartz Van Elst L" first="Ludger" last="Tebartz Van Elst">Ludger Tebartz Van Elst</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff003">
<addr-line>Clinic for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Clinic for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff004">
<addr-line>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Heinrich, Sven P" sort="Heinrich, Sven P" uniqKey="Heinrich S" first="Sven P." last="Heinrich">Sven P. Heinrich</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff002">
<addr-line>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff004">
<addr-line>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kornmeier, Jurgen" sort="Kornmeier, Jurgen" uniqKey="Kornmeier J" first="Jürgen" last="Kornmeier">Jürgen Kornmeier</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff001">
<addr-line>Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology and Mental Health, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology and Mental Health, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff002">
<addr-line>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff004">
<addr-line>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">PMC</idno>
<idno type="pmid">27171276</idno>
<idno type="pmc">4865082</idno>
<idno type="url">http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4865082</idno>
<idno type="RBID">PMC:4865082</idno>
<idno type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0152736</idno>
<date when="2016">2016</date>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Corpus">000565</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Curation">000565</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Checkpoint">000167</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title xml:lang="en" level="a" type="main">Ambiguity in Tactile Apparent Motion Perception</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Liaci, Emanuela" sort="Liaci, Emanuela" uniqKey="Liaci E" first="Emanuela" last="Liaci">Emanuela Liaci</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff001">
<addr-line>Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology and Mental Health, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology and Mental Health, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff002">
<addr-line>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff003">
<addr-line>Clinic for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Clinic for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff004">
<addr-line>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bach, Michael" sort="Bach, Michael" uniqKey="Bach M" first="Michael" last="Bach">Michael Bach</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff002">
<addr-line>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff004">
<addr-line>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tebartz Van Elst, Ludger" sort="Tebartz Van Elst, Ludger" uniqKey="Tebartz Van Elst L" first="Ludger" last="Tebartz Van Elst">Ludger Tebartz Van Elst</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff003">
<addr-line>Clinic for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Clinic for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff004">
<addr-line>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Heinrich, Sven P" sort="Heinrich, Sven P" uniqKey="Heinrich S" first="Sven P." last="Heinrich">Sven P. Heinrich</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff002">
<addr-line>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff004">
<addr-line>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kornmeier, Jurgen" sort="Kornmeier, Jurgen" uniqKey="Kornmeier J" first="Jürgen" last="Kornmeier">Jürgen Kornmeier</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff001">
<addr-line>Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology and Mental Health, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology and Mental Health, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff002">
<addr-line>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="3">
<nlm:aff id="aff004">
<addr-line>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg</wicri:regionArea>
<placeName>
<region type="land" nuts="1">Bade-Wurtemberg</region>
<region type="district" nuts="2">District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</region>
<settlement type="city">Fribourg-en-Brisgau</settlement>
</placeName>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<series>
<title level="j">PLoS ONE</title>
<idno type="eISSN">1932-6203</idno>
<imprint>
<date when="2016">2016</date>
</imprint>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">
<sec id="sec001">
<title>Background</title>
<p>In von Schiller’s Stroboscopic Alternative Motion (SAM) stimulus two visually presented diagonal dot pairs, located on the corners of an imaginary rectangle, alternate with each other and induce either horizontal, vertical or, rarely, rotational motion percepts. SAM motion perception can be described by a psychometric function of the dot aspect ratio (“AR”, i.e. the relation between vertical and horizontal dot distances). Further, with equal horizontal and vertical dot distances (AR = 1) perception is biased towards vertical motion. In a series of five experiments, we presented tactile SAM versions and studied the role of AR and of different reference frames for the perception of tactile apparent motion.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec002">
<title>Methods</title>
<p>We presented tactile SAM stimuli and varied the ARs, while participants reported the perceived motion directions. Pairs of vibration stimulators were attached to the participants’ forearms and stimulator distances were varied within and between forearms. We compared straight and rotated forearm conditions with each other in order to disentangle the roles of exogenous and endogenous reference frames.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec003">
<title>Results</title>
<p>Increasing the tactile SAM’s AR biased perception towards vertical motion, but the effect was weak compared to the visual modality. We found no horizontal disambiguation, even for very small tactile ARs. A forearm rotation by 90° kept the vertical bias, even though it was now coupled with small ARs. A 45° rotation condition with crossed forearms, however, evoked a strong horizontal motion bias.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec004">
<title>Discussion</title>
<p>Existing approaches to explain the visual SAM bias fail to explain the current tactile results. Particularly puzzling is the strong horizontal bias in the crossed-forearm conditions. In the case of tactile apparent motion, there seem to be no fixed priority rule for perceptual disambiguation. Rather the weighting of available evidence seems to depend on the degree of stimulus ambiguity, the current situation and on the perceptual strategy of the individual observer.</p>
</sec>
</div>
</front>
<back>
<div1 type="bibliography">
<listBibl>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kornmeier, J" uniqKey="Kornmeier J">J Kornmeier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mayer, G" uniqKey="Mayer G">G Mayer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Necker, La" uniqKey="Necker L">LA Necker</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ammons, Rb" uniqKey="Ammons R">RB Ammons</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ulrich, P" uniqKey="Ulrich P">P Ulrich</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schiller, Pv" uniqKey="Schiller P">PV Schiller</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Boring, Eg" uniqKey="Boring E">EG Boring</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Blake, R" uniqKey="Blake R">R Blake</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Logothetis, Nk" uniqKey="Logothetis N">NK Logothetis</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="O Hea, Rp" uniqKey="O Hea R">RP O’Shea</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kornmeier, J" uniqKey="Kornmeier J">J Kornmeier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Roeber, U" uniqKey="Roeber U">U Roeber</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ward, Lm" uniqKey="Ward L">LM Ward</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brascamp, Jw" uniqKey="Brascamp J">JW Brascamp</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Ee, R" uniqKey="Van Ee R">R van Ee</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pestman, Wr" uniqKey="Pestman W">WR Pestman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Den Berg, Av" uniqKey="Van Den Berg A">AV van den Berg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wernery, J" uniqKey="Wernery J">J Wernery</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Atmanspacher, H" uniqKey="Atmanspacher H">H Atmanspacher</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kornmeier, J" uniqKey="Kornmeier J">J Kornmeier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Candia, V" uniqKey="Candia V">V Candia</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Folkers, G" uniqKey="Folkers G">G Folkers</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wittmann, M" uniqKey="Wittmann M">M Wittmann</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kornmeier, J" uniqKey="Kornmeier J">J Kornmeier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hein, Cm" uniqKey="Hein C">CM Hein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bach, M" uniqKey="Bach M">M Bach</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leopold, Da" uniqKey="Leopold D">DA Leopold</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Logothetis, Nk" uniqKey="Logothetis N">NK Logothetis</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Miller, Ga" uniqKey="Miller G">GA Miller</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Radilova, J" uniqKey="Radilova J">J Radilová</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Poppel, E" uniqKey="Poppel E">E Pöppel</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ilmberger, J" uniqKey="Ilmberger J">J Ilmberger</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zhou, W" uniqKey="Zhou W">W Zhou</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chen, D" uniqKey="Chen D">D Chen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Harrar, V" uniqKey="Harrar V">V Harrar</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Harris, Lr" uniqKey="Harris L">LR Harris</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pikler, J" uniqKey="Pikler J">J Pikler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hein, E" uniqKey="Hein E">E Hein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Moore, Cm" uniqKey="Moore C">CM Moore</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Carter, O" uniqKey="Carter O">O Carter</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Konkle, T" uniqKey="Konkle T">T Konkle</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wang, Q" uniqKey="Wang Q">Q Wang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hayward, V" uniqKey="Hayward V">V Hayward</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Moore, C" uniqKey="Moore C">C Moore</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kruse, P" uniqKey="Kruse P">P Kruse</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Carmesin, Ho" uniqKey="Carmesin H">HO Carmesin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pahlke, L" uniqKey="Pahlke L">L Pahlke</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Struber, D" uniqKey="Struber D">D Struber</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stadler, M" uniqKey="Stadler M">M Stadler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bukowski, L" uniqKey="Bukowski L">L Bukowski</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hock, H" uniqKey="Hock H">H Hock</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Huisman, A" uniqKey="Huisman A">A Huisman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nichols, D" uniqKey="Nichols D">D Nichols</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rivera, M" uniqKey="Rivera M">M Rivera</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chaudhuri, A" uniqKey="Chaudhuri A">A Chaudhuri</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Glaser, Da" uniqKey="Glaser D">DA Glaser</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gengerelli, Ja" uniqKey="Gengerelli J">JA Gengerelli</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Conrad, V" uniqKey="Conrad V">V Conrad</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vitello, Mp" uniqKey="Vitello M">MP Vitello</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Noppeney, U" uniqKey="Noppeney U">U Noppeney</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vitello, Mp" uniqKey="Vitello M">MP Vitello</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bach, M" uniqKey="Bach M">M Bach</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bach, M" uniqKey="Bach M">M Bach</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct></biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Edgington, Es" uniqKey="Edgington E">ES Edgington</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Onghena, P" uniqKey="Onghena P">P Onghena</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Holm, S" uniqKey="Holm S">S Holm</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Alais, D" uniqKey="Alais D">D Alais</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Burr, D" uniqKey="Burr D">D Burr</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hock, Hs" uniqKey="Hock H">HS Hock</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kelso, Ja" uniqKey="Kelso J">JA Kelso</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schoner, G" uniqKey="Schoner G">G Schoner</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gepshtein, S" uniqKey="Gepshtein S">S Gepshtein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kubovy, M" uniqKey="Kubovy M">M Kubovy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Overvliet, Ke" uniqKey="Overvliet K">KE Overvliet</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Krampe, Rt" uniqKey="Krampe R">RT Krampe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wagemans, J" uniqKey="Wagemans J">J Wagemans</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct></biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Maloney, Lt" uniqKey="Maloney L">LT Maloney</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Martello, Mfd" uniqKey="Martello M">MFD Martello</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sahm, C" uniqKey="Sahm C">C Sahm</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Spillmann, L" uniqKey="Spillmann L">L Spillmann</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schwartz, Jl" uniqKey="Schwartz J">JL Schwartz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grimault, N" uniqKey="Grimault N">N Grimault</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hupe, Jm" uniqKey="Hupe J">JM Hupe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Moore, Bc" uniqKey="Moore B">BC Moore</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pressnitzer, D" uniqKey="Pressnitzer D">D Pressnitzer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Long, Gm" uniqKey="Long G">GM Long</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Toppino, Tc" uniqKey="Toppino T">TC Toppino</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kornmeier, J" uniqKey="Kornmeier J">J Kornmeier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bach, M" uniqKey="Bach M">M Bach</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Genc, E" uniqKey="Genc E">E Genç</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bergmann, J" uniqKey="Bergmann J">J Bergmann</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Singer, W" uniqKey="Singer W">W Singer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kohler, A" uniqKey="Kohler A">A Kohler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Struber, D" uniqKey="Struber D">D Strüber</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rach, S" uniqKey="Rach S">S Rach</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Trautmann Lengsfeld, Sa" uniqKey="Trautmann Lengsfeld S">SA Trautmann-Lengsfeld</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Engel, Ak" uniqKey="Engel A">AK Engel</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Herrmann, Cs" uniqKey="Herrmann C">CS Herrmann</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Eickhoff, Sb" uniqKey="Eickhoff S">SB Eickhoff</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grefkes, C" uniqKey="Grefkes C">C Grefkes</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fink, Gr" uniqKey="Fink G">GR Fink</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zilles, K" uniqKey="Zilles K">K Zilles</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Heed, T" uniqKey="Heed T">T Heed</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Roder, B" uniqKey="Roder B">B Röder</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Murray, Mm" uniqKey="Murray M">MM Murray</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wallace, Mt" uniqKey="Wallace M">MT Wallace</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Clement, G" uniqKey="Clement G">G Clément</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Demel, M" uniqKey="Demel M">M Demel</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Clement, G" uniqKey="Clement G">G Clément</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wood, Sj" uniqKey="Wood S">SJ Wood</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Von Helmholtz, H" uniqKey="Von Helmholtz H">H von Helmholtz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kersten, D" uniqKey="Kersten D">D Kersten</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mamassian, P" uniqKey="Mamassian P">P Mamassian</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yuille, A" uniqKey="Yuille A">A Yuille</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
</listBibl>
</div1>
</back>
</TEI>
<pmc article-type="research-article">
<pmc-dir>properties open_access</pmc-dir>
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="nlm-ta">PLoS One</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="iso-abbrev">PLoS ONE</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="publisher-id">plos</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="pmc">plosone</journal-id>
<journal-title-group>
<journal-title>PLoS ONE</journal-title>
</journal-title-group>
<issn pub-type="epub">1932-6203</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Public Library of Science</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>San Francisco, CA USA</publisher-loc>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmid">27171276</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmc">4865082</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0152736</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="publisher-id">PONE-D-15-20743</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Research Article</subject>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Neuroscience</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Cognitive Science</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Cognitive Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Perception</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Cognitive Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Perception</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Social Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Cognitive Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Perception</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Neuroscience</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Sensory Perception</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Sensory Perception</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Social Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Sensory Perception</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Neuroscience</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Sensory Perception</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Vision</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Sensory Perception</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Vision</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Social Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Sensory Perception</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Vision</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Anatomy</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Musculoskeletal System</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Limbs (Anatomy)</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Arms</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Forearms</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Medicine and Health Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Anatomy</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Musculoskeletal System</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Limbs (Anatomy)</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Arms</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Forearms</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Physical Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Physics</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Classical Mechanics</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Vibration</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Physical Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Mathematics</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Geometry</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Aspect Ratio</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Physiology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Sensory Physiology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Somatosensory System</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Tactile Sensation</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Medicine and Health Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Physiology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Sensory Physiology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Somatosensory System</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Tactile Sensation</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Neuroscience</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Sensory Systems</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Somatosensory System</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Tactile Sensation</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Anatomy</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Musculoskeletal System</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Limbs (Anatomy)</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Arms</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Medicine and Health Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Anatomy</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Musculoskeletal System</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Limbs (Anatomy)</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Arms</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>Ambiguity in Tactile Apparent Motion Perception</article-title>
<alt-title alt-title-type="running-head">Ambiguity in Tactile Apparent Motion Perception</alt-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Liaci</surname>
<given-names>Emanuela</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff001">
<sup>1</sup>
</xref>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff002">
<sup>2</sup>
</xref>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff003">
<sup>3</sup>
</xref>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff004">
<sup>4</sup>
</xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bach</surname>
<given-names>Michael</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff002">
<sup>2</sup>
</xref>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff004">
<sup>4</sup>
</xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tebartz van Elst</surname>
<given-names>Ludger</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff003">
<sup>3</sup>
</xref>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff004">
<sup>4</sup>
</xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Heinrich</surname>
<given-names>Sven P.</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff002">
<sup>2</sup>
</xref>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff004">
<sup>4</sup>
</xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kornmeier</surname>
<given-names>Jürgen</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff001">
<sup>1</sup>
</xref>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff002">
<sup>2</sup>
</xref>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff004">
<sup>4</sup>
</xref>
<xref ref-type="corresp" rid="cor001">*</xref>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<aff id="aff001">
<label>1</label>
<addr-line>Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology and Mental Health, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</aff>
<aff id="aff002">
<label>2</label>
<addr-line>Eye Center, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</aff>
<aff id="aff003">
<label>3</label>
<addr-line>Clinic for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</aff>
<aff id="aff004">
<label>4</label>
<addr-line>Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany</addr-line>
</aff>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Herzog</surname>
<given-names>Michael H.</given-names>
</name>
<role>Editor</role>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="edit1"></xref>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<aff id="edit1">
<addr-line>Ecole Polytechnique Federale de Lausanne, SWITZERLAND</addr-line>
</aff>
<author-notes>
<fn fn-type="conflict" id="coi001">
<p>
<bold>Competing Interests: </bold>
The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.</p>
</fn>
<fn fn-type="con" id="contrib001">
<p>Conceived and designed the experiments: EL SH JK. Performed the experiments: EL JK. Analyzed the data: EL MB LTvE SH JK. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: EL SH JK. Wrote the paper: EL MB LTvE SH JK.</p>
</fn>
<corresp id="cor001">* E-mail:
<email>juergen.kornmeier@uni-freiburg.de</email>
</corresp>
</author-notes>
<pub-date pub-type="epub">
<day>12</day>
<month>5</month>
<year>2016</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="collection">
<year>2016</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>11</volume>
<issue>5</issue>
<elocation-id>e0152736</elocation-id>
<history>
<date date-type="received">
<day>13</day>
<month>5</month>
<year>2015</year>
</date>
<date date-type="accepted">
<day>19</day>
<month>3</month>
<year>2016</year>
</date>
</history>
<permissions>
<copyright-statement>© 2016 Liaci et al</copyright-statement>
<copyright-year>2016</copyright-year>
<copyright-holder>Liaci et al</copyright-holder>
<license xlink:href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/">
<license-p>This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/">Creative Commons Attribution License</ext-link>
, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.</license-p>
</license>
</permissions>
<self-uri content-type="pdf" xlink:href="pone.0152736.pdf"></self-uri>
<abstract>
<sec id="sec001">
<title>Background</title>
<p>In von Schiller’s Stroboscopic Alternative Motion (SAM) stimulus two visually presented diagonal dot pairs, located on the corners of an imaginary rectangle, alternate with each other and induce either horizontal, vertical or, rarely, rotational motion percepts. SAM motion perception can be described by a psychometric function of the dot aspect ratio (“AR”, i.e. the relation between vertical and horizontal dot distances). Further, with equal horizontal and vertical dot distances (AR = 1) perception is biased towards vertical motion. In a series of five experiments, we presented tactile SAM versions and studied the role of AR and of different reference frames for the perception of tactile apparent motion.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec002">
<title>Methods</title>
<p>We presented tactile SAM stimuli and varied the ARs, while participants reported the perceived motion directions. Pairs of vibration stimulators were attached to the participants’ forearms and stimulator distances were varied within and between forearms. We compared straight and rotated forearm conditions with each other in order to disentangle the roles of exogenous and endogenous reference frames.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec003">
<title>Results</title>
<p>Increasing the tactile SAM’s AR biased perception towards vertical motion, but the effect was weak compared to the visual modality. We found no horizontal disambiguation, even for very small tactile ARs. A forearm rotation by 90° kept the vertical bias, even though it was now coupled with small ARs. A 45° rotation condition with crossed forearms, however, evoked a strong horizontal motion bias.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec004">
<title>Discussion</title>
<p>Existing approaches to explain the visual SAM bias fail to explain the current tactile results. Particularly puzzling is the strong horizontal bias in the crossed-forearm conditions. In the case of tactile apparent motion, there seem to be no fixed priority rule for perceptual disambiguation. Rather the weighting of available evidence seems to depend on the degree of stimulus ambiguity, the current situation and on the perceptual strategy of the individual observer.</p>
</sec>
</abstract>
<funding-group>
<award-group id="award001">
<funding-source>
<institution-wrap>
<institution-id institution-id-type="funder-id">http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100001659</institution-id>
<institution>Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft</institution>
</institution-wrap>
</funding-source>
<award-id>KO 4764/1-1, TE 280/8-1</award-id>
<principal-award-recipient>
<name>
<surname>Kornmeier</surname>
<given-names>Jürgen</given-names>
</name>
</principal-award-recipient>
</award-group>
<funding-statement>This work was funded by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) (
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.dfg.de">http://www.dfg.de</ext-link>
) (projects: KO 4764/1-1 and TE 280/8-1). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.</funding-statement>
</funding-group>
<counts>
<fig-count count="5"></fig-count>
<table-count count="4"></table-count>
<page-count count="22"></page-count>
</counts>
<custom-meta-group>
<custom-meta id="data-availability">
<meta-name>Data Availability</meta-name>
<meta-value>All relevant data are within the paper.</meta-value>
</custom-meta>
</custom-meta-group>
</article-meta>
<notes>
<title>Data Availability</title>
<p>All relevant data are within the paper.</p>
</notes>
</front>
<body>
<sec sec-type="intro" id="sec005">
<title>Introduction</title>
<p>The capacity of our sensory organs is restricted and thus the availability of the information from the world around us. Our perceptual system needs to make sense of this incomplete and to varying degrees ambiguous information in order to construct the most coherent interpretations (e.g. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref001" ref-type="bibr">1</xref>
]). Ambiguous figures, like the famous Necker cube [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref002" ref-type="bibr">2</xref>
], are extreme cases of sensory ambiguity. The corresponding percepts are only temporarily stable and alternate between different but equally probable interpretations, even though the stimulus, and thus the retinal input, remain constant. Multistability can be evoked by a variety of ambiguous figures, involving depth ambiguity e.g. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref002" ref-type="bibr">2</xref>
], figure/ground organization (e.g., Rubin’s face/vase figure [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref003" ref-type="bibr">3</xref>
]), motion ambiguity (e.g., von Schiller’s Stroboscopic Alternative Motion stimulus, “SAM” [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref004" ref-type="bibr">4</xref>
]), or even semantic ambiguity (e.g., Boring's Old/Young woman [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref005" ref-type="bibr">5</xref>
]). Another class of multistable perception is binocular rivalry, when different images are presented to the two eyes and perception alternates spontaneously between the two images [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref006" ref-type="bibr">6</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref007" ref-type="bibr">7</xref>
]. All cases of multistability share some common features: (
<italic>i</italic>
) perceptual alternations show a stochastic pattern and can be modeled by gamma distributions [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref008" ref-type="bibr">8</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref009" ref-type="bibr">9</xref>
] and (
<italic>ii</italic>
) perceptual alternations can be influenced volitionally, e.g. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref010" ref-type="bibr">10</xref>
]. Several authors have thus postulated that sensory ambiguity is processed at high-level/cognitive units (e.g., the seminal review [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref011" ref-type="bibr">11</xref>
]). Multistable perception has been studied predominantly in the visual modality. However, very similar effects have been described in other sensory modalities, like audition (e.g. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref012" ref-type="bibr">12</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref013" ref-type="bibr">13</xref>
]) and olfaction (e.g. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref014" ref-type="bibr">14</xref>
]). Similarity of ambiguity features across sensory modalities may further support this high-level/cognitive explanations, mentioned above.</p>
<p>Studies of ambiguity in the tactile modality used tactile apparent motion stimulus variants. Harrar and Harris [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref015" ref-type="bibr">15</xref>
] applied a tactile version of the visual “Pikler-Ternus illusion” [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref016" ref-type="bibr">16</xref>
]: The sequential presentation of frames composed of three dots can be perceived as either group or element motion, as a function of the inter stimulus interval between frames (for a demonstration see
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://michaelbach.de/ot/mot-Ternus/">Ternus</ext-link>
). Like the visual Ternus, the tactile variant was ambiguous with spontaneous alternations between group and element motion, however, with lower rates of perceptual alternations (e.g. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref017" ref-type="bibr">17</xref>
]).</p>
<p>Von Schiller’s ambiguous stroboscopic alternative motion (SAM) stimulus [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref004" ref-type="bibr">4</xref>
], (also known as the “apparent motion quartet” [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref018" ref-type="bibr">18</xref>
]), is another apparent motion stimulus that can be realized both in the visual and tactile modalities and will be used in the present study. The visual SAM consists of two diagonal pairs of dots alternately presented at the corners of an invisible rectangle. The alternating appearance of the dot pairs integrates into a coherent percept of apparent motion, whereby we perceive two dots moving in opposite directions. The perceived motion direction is a function of the rectangle’s
<italic>aspect ratio (“AR”)</italic>
, i.e. the ratio between horizontal and vertical distances of the four dots. Observers perceive horizontal motion with small horizontal and large vertical dot distance (AR << 1), and vice versa. In rare cases observers also report rightwards or leftwards rotation, e.g. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref019" ref-type="bibr">19</xref>
]. At a certain AR perception becomes unstable and spontaneously changes between motion directions (for an online demonstration see
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://michaelbach.de/ot/mot_sam/">SAM</ext-link>
). In particular, the dependence of the perceptual outcome on the AR can be modeled by a psychometric function, e.g. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref020" ref-type="bibr">20</xref>
]. Interesting in this context is a vertical perceptual bias of the SAM stimulus: At an AR = 1 (when vertical and horizontal dot distances are equal) we would expect parity of percepts but typically find a bias towards vertical motion [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref021" ref-type="bibr">21</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref022" ref-type="bibr">22</xref>
].</p>
<p>In the tactile version of the SAM stimulus the observers are stimulated with pulses of four vibrotactile stimulators. Carter et al. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref018" ref-type="bibr">18</xref>
] have demonstrated perceptual instability during tactile SAM stimulation on the finger tips. In contrast to the visual modality, however, changing the AR of the tactile stimulators had no significant perceptual disambiguation effect in their study. A major difference between the visual and tactile stimulation is that the spatial location of a tactile stimulus can be mapped to a somatotopic/skin-based
<italic>endogenous reference frame</italic>
, in addition to a space-based/world-based
<italic>exogenous reference frame</italic>
, shared by both vision and touch. In the endogenous reference frame, the stimuli are encoded in relation to one’s own body surface, whereas the exogenous reference frame refers to the external (world) representation of stimuli. Conflicting information from the two reference frames might explain the missing disambiguation for extreme AR values in the tactile SAM.</p>
<p>An interesting variant of the tactile SAM occurs when the two pairs of vibrotactile stimulators are placed at corresponding fingers of the left and right hand [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref023" ref-type="bibr">23</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref024" ref-type="bibr">24</xref>
]. A change of the within-finger distance of the vibrotactile SAM stimulators then entails a change in both endogenous and exogenous reference frames, whereas a change of the between-limb distance changes the position of the stimulators only in the exogenous reference frame. This might allow disentangling the influences of the exogenous and the endogenous reference frames on tactile motion perception. Conrad et al. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref023" ref-type="bibr">23</xref>
] attached two coin-sized vibrators, with small LEDs on top, to each index finger of their participants. Across four different conditions they either used unimodal visual or tactile stimulation and either spatially congruent or incongruent bimodal stimulation with three different finger distances. Overall, they found more stable motion perception in the tactile modality and with congruent bimodal stimulation compared to pure visual stimulation. Furthermore, the perceived motion direction was a function of finger distance with “horizontal”/ between-finger motion for small finger distances (AR = 0.5), “vertical”/ within-finger motion for larger finger distances (AR = 2) and most unstable percepts with AR = 1 (i.e. equal horizontal and vertical stimulator distance). The disambiguated tactile SAM stimuli (small and large finger distances), however, produced less clear perceptual biases (≈ 60%) than the visual SAM stimulus (close to 100% as reported in [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref020" ref-type="bibr">20</xref>
]). A similar pattern of results was found in Vitello [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref024" ref-type="bibr">24</xref>
], who also used tactile stimulators on the index fingers and varied finger distances between five different AR values (0.25, 0.5, 1, 2 and 4) in the tactile SAM stimulus. He found an increasing bias towards between-finger (horizontal) motion perception with smaller between-finger distances, but no clear bias towards within-finger (vertical) motion for larger between-finger distances. Further, the amount of tactile bias in Vitello’s data again did not exceed 60% even with more extreme ARs, cf. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref023" ref-type="bibr">23</xref>
]. Vitello’s tactile data displayed a linear relation between finger distance and perceptual outcome, which is in contrast to the sigmoidal shape observed in the visual modality [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref020" ref-type="bibr">20</xref>
].</p>
<p>In summary, apparent motion stimuli, e.g. von Schiller’s SAM, are well suited to compare perceptual processing of visual with tactile ambiguity, because highly similar stimuli can be used in the two modalities. Studies from the visual modality show clear stimulus disambiguation and perceptual stabilization of horizontal and vertical motion for small and large SAM ARs respectively and perceptual instabilities at intermediate ARs, together with a strong bias towards vertical motion at an AR = 1. The few previous studies with a tactile variant of the SAM stimulus provide a less clear picture. Changing the AR of the tactile SAM had only a weak stimulus disambiguation effect, compared to the visual modality, without the expected sigmoidal shape and no clear vertical bias at AR = 1. Ambiguity seems to be processed differently in visual and tactile modalities, which argues against the postulated role of central high-level factors beyond sensory modalities during ambiguity resolution, e.g. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref011" ref-type="bibr">11</xref>
].</p>
<p>In the present study we focus on two aspects:</p>
<p>(1) One potential explanation for the difference in influence of AR on motion disambiguation may be a different spatial scaling in the tactile compared to the visual modalities. In order to test this, we applied tactile SAM stimuli with a larger range of ARs than previous studies. Tactile perceptual disambiguation close to 100% at more extreme values of ARs (larger and smaller) would point to overall cross modal similarities in motion disambiguation but different spatial scales for motion gestalt integration.</p>
<p>(2) Another potential explanation for the difference in influence of AR on motion disambiguation between the two modalities may be an interplay of exogenous with the endogenous reference frames during the processing of tactile information while only one (exogenous) reference frame is available in the visual modality. In a series of experiments we thus varied the mapping between exogenous and endogenous reference frames by forearm rotation and studied how this affects the disambiguation of ambiguous tactile motion information.</p>
<p>Sensory information is to varying degrees ambiguous. Its disambiguation is a major challenge of our perceptual system, even though we do not notice this in our every day experience. The present study aims to further our understanding of modality-specific and modality-independent aspects of sensory disambiguation, and in particular how information from different reference frames are integrated.</p>
</sec>
<sec sec-type="materials|methods" id="sec006">
<title>Methods</title>
<sec id="sec007">
<title>Participants</title>
<p>All participants were naive as to the specific experimental question and gave their written informed consent. None of them reported any history of neurological disease. Visual acuity was tested with the Freiburg Visual Acuity Test [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref025" ref-type="bibr">25</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref026" ref-type="bibr">26</xref>
] and all participants had a normal or corrected-to-normal vision. The study, consisting of five experiments, was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Freiburg and performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref027" ref-type="bibr">27</xref>
].</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec008">
<title>Experiments</title>
<p>The present study consisted of five experiments. In Experiments 1 and 2 we applied a tactile variant of the visual SAM to the participants’ forearms, placed straight parallel on the table in front. We studied tactile SAM motion perception as a function of a large range of aspect ratios. In Experiment 3 we compared two configurations of tactile SAM perception, one with straight parallel and one with forearms rotated by 90°, in order to disentangle the contribution from endogenous and exogenous reference frames. In Experiments 4 and 5 we rotated the participants forearms by 45°, resulting in crossed forearms, and compared the perceptual results from two different tactile SAM stimulation protocols. Experimental details are provided in the respective sections.</p>
<sec id="sec009">
<title>Vertical visual vs. “vertical” tactile motion</title>
<p>In the visual modality perceived “vertical motion” was defined as motion between the upper and lower right (or left) corners of the imaginary rectangle, e.g. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref019" ref-type="bibr">19</xref>
]. In the tactile modality we adopted the definitions from previous studies on the tactile SAM variants of perceived “vertical motion” as motion in depth to and away from the observer’s body along the arms that were positioned on a table in front of the participants [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref023" ref-type="bibr">23</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref024" ref-type="bibr">24</xref>
] (see also
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g001">Fig 1</xref>
). This can be motivated by the following considerations: At the earliest step of vision, a three dimensional world is projected onto two-dimensional retinae. As a consequence, up and down motion as well as motion to and away from the body are projected onto the same axis. The depth axis in the tactile experiment and the vertical axis in typical visual SAM experiments may thus be represented in the same way. Despite this, the results of the present experiments have to be regarded under the caveat that vertical visual motion is located in another 3D plane than vertical tactile motion.</p>
<fig id="pone.0152736.g001" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0152736.g001</object-id>
<label>Fig 1</label>
<caption>
<title>Overview of the tactile Experiments.</title>
<p>Schematic illustration of the stimulus setups, hypothetical motion percepts and their description with respect to the two different reference frames (RF).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0152736.g001"></graphic>
</fig>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="sec010">
<title>Data analysis</title>
<p>We calculated generalized linear regression analyses with a binomial family function and a logit link function in order to test relations between the direction of motion perception (dependent variable) and horizontal and vertical stimulator distances (independent variables).</p>
<p>We applied non-parametric statistical permutation tests [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref028" ref-type="bibr">28</xref>
] analogous to classical t-tests in order to test deviations of motion perception from chance level (0.5). The basic idea of permutation tests is to generate reference distributions out of the measured data by randomizing their relation to experimental conditions. Correction for multiple testing was applied with Holm’s variant of the Bonferroni correction [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref029" ref-type="bibr">29</xref>
]. The Holm-procedure starts by ordering the p-values from all executed tests. It then compares the smallest p-value with an alpha level corrected by the number n of individual tests, as suggested by Bonferroni. It continues by comparing the subsequent p-values with step-wise adjusted alphas (e.g. second smallest p-value compared to an alpha level corrected by n-1, etc.).</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="sec011">
<title>Experiment 1: Tactile Analogue of a Typical Visual SAM Experiment</title>
<sec id="sec012" sec-type="intro">
<title>Background</title>
<p>We applied a tactile version of the SAM stimulus with varying ARs and studied tactile apparent motion percepts as a function of AR.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec013">
<title>Participants</title>
<p>Nine participants (6 males, 3 females; age range = 20–27, mean age = 23 years) participated in Experiment 1. All participants were right-handed.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec014">
<title>Tactile SAM stimuli</title>
<p>For tactile SAM stimulation four SHIVR HEK-100 stimulator kits (Midé Technology Corp., Medford, MA, USA) were used. Each consisted of a piezoelectric tactor and the respective driver electronics, into which sequences of 2-V pulses were fed to produce vibrations. These voltage pulses were controlled through custom software based on Igor Pro (Wavemetrics, Inc., Lake Oswego, OR, USA) running on a Mac Mini computer. A single vibration burst consisted of 30 pulses at a frequency of 100 Hz, each lasting for 5 ms and being followed by a gap of 5 ms. Each stimulator had a diameter of 0.32 cm.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec015">
<title>Paradigm</title>
<p>The participants were seated with their arms on a table placed in front of them. The arms were oriented in parallel to each other in straight-ahead direction. Two stimulators were fixed at each of the observer’s forearm. The four stimulators were positioned in parallel within and between the arms at the four corners of an imaginary rectangle. We defined distances of stimulator locations in relation to the total forearm length of the individual participant in order to control for inter-individual variability in forearm length. The two stimulators of each stimulator-pair were placed at the center of the two forearms at a distance of 50% of the participants total forearm length and seven different between-forearm distances ranging from 20% to 200% of the participants total forearm length in steps of 33.33%. This resulted in 7 different tactile SAM stimuli, corresponding to the following ARs: 0, 0.67, 1.34, 2, 2.67, 3.34 and 4. All ARs of the tactile SAM stimuli were calculated with respect to the exogenous reference coordinates. An AR of 0 corresponds to a configuration where the two forearms touch each other. However, the stimulators from one forearm never touched the neighboring forearm and all participants had clear antiparallel motion percepts in this configuration.</p>
<p>All participants were blindfolded during the tactile SAM experiment because normally vision dominates the other senses in spatial processing [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref030" ref-type="bibr">30</xref>
]. In particular, Vitello [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref024" ref-type="bibr">24</xref>
] found visual influence on the perception of the ambiguous tactile SAM variant. In addition, our participants wore headphones during the experiment, listening to music in order to mask the vibration sound of the stimulators.</p>
<p>The tactile SAM stimuli were presented in a sequence of four frames. Each frame contained one pair of two diagonal vibrators pulsing for 300 ms and was followed by an inter-pulse interval of 200 ms per frame. Each vibrator pair was thus active twice within a sequence and was followed by a response interval of 3000 ms. During the latter, participants indicated horizontal or vertical motion percepts by pressing one of two different pedals of a three-pedal foot device (Scythe USB_3FS-2 triple foot switch). In the case of other percepts, the participants pressed a third pedal. Any pedal press terminated the response interval and started the next SAM sequence. Preliminary tests suggested that operating 5 keys of the foot device could potentially overburden the participants. Given the rare occurrence of rotational motion, we therefore decided to use only 3 switches. None of the participants reported any case of rotational motion perception in the tactile experiments. Whenever participants responded outside the response interval (i.e. too early or too late), this SAM sequence was repeated. The starting stimulator pair was randomized between sequences. Before executing the experiment, all participants were trained to associate each key with the corresponding tactile motion direction.</p>
<p>Some participants reported perceiving dot-like objects jumping back and forth either along the forearms (within-forearm or vertical motion) or between the forearms (between-forearm or horizontal motion,
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g001">Fig 1</xref>
). Other participants reported perceptions of an imaginary rod, whose partial back and forth rotation induced tactile stimulations on the forearms. In this case they perceived the front end (and likewise the back end) of the rod oscillating either on one forearm (vertical motion) or between the forearms (horizontal motion).</p>
<p>One specific SAM sequence was repeated 20 times within one experimental block. AR and thus SAM stimulator attachment was changed between experimental blocks. The order of the 7 experimental blocks, each with a specific SAM stimulus, was randomized in the first half of the experiment. The blocks were repeated in reverse order in the second half of the experiment (ABBA scheme). This resulted in 40 presentations of each of the seven SAM stimuli (see
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g002">Fig 2</xref>
).</p>
<fig id="pone.0152736.g002" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0152736.g002</object-id>
<label>Fig 2</label>
<caption>
<title>Tactile SAM Paradigm.</title>
<p>Within each experimental block, 20 SAM sequences (Seq
<sub>i</sub>
) were presented for 4800 ms each and participants indicated their motion percepts at the end of each sequence. AR was kept constant within SAM sequences and blocks but changed randomly between blocks. The number of ARs, and thus the number of blocks, varied between experiments. Experimental blocks with specific SAM ARs occurred in random order in the first half of the experiment. This random order was reversed in the second half of the experiment resulting in 40 sequences per AR in total. Small red and white circles: Respectively active and inactive tactile stimulators.</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0152736.g002"></graphic>
</fig>
</sec>
<sec id="sec016" sec-type="results">
<title>Results</title>
<p>The mean percentage of percepts other than vertical or horizontal motion across conditions was 4 ± 2%.</p>
<p>The generalized linear model indicated no effect of horizontal and vertical stimulator distance on the direction of motion perception.</p>
<p>Individual randomization tests for difference of perceived motion direction from chance level indicated a bias towards vertical (i.e. within-forearm) motion perception with aspect ratios larger than one. After correction for multiple testing this bias remained significant for ARs of 2.0 and 3.34 (see
<xref ref-type="table" rid="pone.0152736.t001">Table 1</xref>
). However, this vertical bias was much weaker (roughly 70%, see
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g003">Fig 3</xref>
, grey trace) than the vertical bias for large ARs as reported from the visual modality (close to 100%, e.g. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref031" ref-type="bibr">31</xref>
]). We did not observe a bias towards horizontal motion perception, not even for the smallest horizontal stimulator distances (54% vertical motion percepts, p = 0.35 for the test against chance level). The statistical results from Experiment 1 are listed in
<xref ref-type="table" rid="pone.0152736.t001">Table 1</xref>
.</p>
<table-wrap id="pone.0152736.t001" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0152736.t001</object-id>
<label>Table 1</label>
<caption>
<title>Statistical Results from Experiment 1.</title>
</caption>
<alternatives>
<graphic id="pone.0152736.t001g" xlink:href="pone.0152736.t001"></graphic>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<colgroup span="1">
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
</colgroup>
<thead>
<tr>
<th align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Aspect Ratio (Horiz./between- forearm stimulator distance in % forearm-length)</th>
<th align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">p-Value</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.4 (20%)</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.35</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.67 (33%)</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.36</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">1.34 (67%)</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.035</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">2.0 (100%)</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.0055
<xref ref-type="table-fn" rid="t001fn002">*</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">2.67 (133%)</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.075</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">3.34 (167%)</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.0004
<xref ref-type="table-fn" rid="t001fn003">**</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">4.0 (200%)</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.014</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</alternatives>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="t001fn001">
<p>Significance level after correction for multiple testing:</p>
</fn>
<fn id="t001fn002">
<p>* p < 0.05;</p>
</fn>
<fn id="t001fn003">
<p>** p < 0.01</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
<fig id="pone.0152736.g003" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0152736.g003</object-id>
<label>Fig 3</label>
<caption>
<title>Results from Experiments 1–3.</title>
<p>Percentage of vertical motion percepts as functions of the stimulators’ AR. Symbols are grand means ± SEM. Within-forearm dot distances were 50% total forearm length (grey, dark blue and red traces) and 25% (light blue and red traces). The grey trace depicts results from Experiment 1, the red traces depict results from Experiment 2 and the blue traces results from Experiment 3. The horizontal dotted line indicates chance level, i.e. equal probability for vertical and horizontal motion percepts. In contrast to the visual SAM we here see very little influence of AR on perceptual dominance—in the visual modality this figure would resemble a psychometric function, running from bottom left to top right. Stars indicate statistical significance (* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01). [wfsd = within-forearm stimulator distance].</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0152736.g003"></graphic>
</fig>
</sec>
<sec id="sec017" sec-type="conclusions">
<title>Discussion</title>
<p>Perception of the tactile SAM stimuli showed the following similarities to the visual modality. First both the visual and the tactile SAM stimuli induced apparent motion perception. Second, this apparent motion perception was ambiguous in a certain range of stimulus parameters. Third, taking into account that during visual perception a three-dimensional world is projected onto two-dimensional retinae and labeling both the visual up and down motion and the tactile motion in depth away and towards the body as “vertical motion”, both modalities showed a perceptual bias towards vertical motion.</p>
<p>Despite these similarities there were also obvious differences between the visual and the tactile modalities. First, apparent motion perception in the tactile modality was overall less stable than in the visual modality—it stayed ambiguous even for large ARs (up to 1.3) and only became biased with ARs of 2 and larger (see
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g003">Fig 3</xref>
, grey trace). Second, we did not find any bias towards horizontal (between forearms) tactile motion perception, even for the smallest between-forearm stimulator distance when grouping by proximity would predict horizontal motion perception [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref032" ref-type="bibr">32</xref>
]. Similar deviations from the proximity rule in the tactile modality have been reported in other studies [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref018" ref-type="bibr">18</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref033" ref-type="bibr">33</xref>
]. This is in stark contrast with the visual modality, where even small ARs elicited close to 100% horizontal motion perception (e.g. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref020" ref-type="bibr">20</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref031" ref-type="bibr">31</xref>
]).</p>
<sec id="sec018">
<title>Possible explanations of extended perceptual ambiguity and the vertical motion bias in the tactile modality</title>
<p>As noted before, one obvious difference between modalities is the involvement of
<italic>two</italic>
different representational systems in the tactile modality but only
<italic>one</italic>
in the visual modality. Specifically, vertical tactile motion direction can be organized by referring to an endogenous, somatotopic reference frame. In this way an imaginary object jumps back and forth between two neighboring points of the body—in the present case the forearm. These two points may then be represented by two adjacent neural assemblies in the somatotopically organized somatosensory cortex. Proximity of the two cortical representations may thus enter as input from the endogenous reference frame to the perceptual motion interpretation. In endogenous reference frame coordinates the vertical (within-forearm) distance between stimulators is always smaller than the horizontal distance. This is because the representations of the stimulators from different forearms are in the somatosensory cortices of the contralateral hemispheres. SAM ARs (= horizontal distance / vertical distance) expressed in endogenous reference frames are thus always larger than 1, supporting the vertical motion interpretation.</p>
<p>Concurrently, tactile motion perception can also be constructed by referring to an exogenous space-based or world-based reference frame. The position of the forearm, the position of the stimulators on the forearm, and the back and forth jumps of the imaginary object can thus also be described in exogenous world-based coordinates. Here, the horizontal stimulator distance depends on the arm positions and small arm distances lead to ARs between 0 and 1, thus supporting a horizontal motion interpretation. For large arm distances, in contrast, ARs are greater than 1, supporting a vertical motion interpretation.</p>
<p>The information from the two reference frames needs to be conciliated in order to construct a stable and reliable motion percept. For large forearm distances the analysis outputs from both reference frames support a vertical motion interpretation. This may explain the observed tendency for vertical motion interpretations with large ARs. For small forearm distances, in contrast, information from the two reference frames support opposite interpretations evoking perceptual instability for a broad range of smaller ARs and, in particular, preventing a bias towards horizontal motion perception.</p>
<p>An obvious question remains, however, with this explanation: Why is the vertical bias for large ARs so weak in the tactile compared to the visual modality?</p>
</sec>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="sec019">
<title>Experiment 2: Increasing the Tactile AR Range</title>
<sec id="sec020" sec-type="intro">
<title>Background</title>
<p>Tactile SAM stimuli stayed ambiguous for a large range of ARs, which is in strong contrast to what is known from the visual modality. Potential differences in tactile and visual scaling may require more extreme AR values for tactile SAM disambiguation. In Experiment 2 we thus explored whether a further increase of AR would increase the vertical motion bias towards more stable motion percepts, comparable to the visual modality. Since we are anatomically restricted with respect to a further increase of the horizontal stimulator/forearm distances, we increased ARs by simply decreasing the vertical stimulator distance.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec021">
<title>Participants</title>
<p>Fifteen participants (8 males, 7 females; age range = 20–27, mean age = 23.7 years) participated in Experiment 2. All but one were right-handed. Five participants had participated in Experiment 1.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec022">
<title>Paradigm</title>
<p>Experiment 2 was identical to Experiment 1 with two changes:</p>
<p>(1) We only used four between-forearm distances: 20%, 66%, 133% and 200% of the participant’s total forearm length. (2) We combined these four between-forearm distances with two within-forearm distances: 25% and 50% of the participant’s total forearm length. This resulted in 8 combinations of within- and between-forearm distances, corresponding to the following 8 ARs: for 25% within-forearm distance, 0.8, 2.67, 5.32, 8; for the 50% within-forearm distance, 0.4, 1.34, 2.67, 4. Each AR was presented two times (i.e. in two blocks; see
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g002">Fig 2</xref>
). For technical reasons (recording failure) we had to dismiss one repetition for one participant for one combination.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec023" sec-type="results">
<title>Results</title>
<p>The mean percentage of percepts other than vertical or horizontal motion across conditions was 5 ± 2%.</p>
<p>For the 50% vertical (within-forearm) stimulator distances only large horizontal (between-forearm) stimulator distances showed a weak perceptual bias towards vertical motion perception (see
<xref ref-type="table" rid="pone.0152736.t002">Table 2</xref>
and the dark red trace in
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g003">Fig 3</xref>
). For the 25% vertical (within-forearm) stimulator distance the vertical motion bias is more obvious and seems to be present for all horizontal stimulator distances, although only the two smallest remained significant after correction for multiple testing (
<xref ref-type="table" rid="pone.0152736.t002">Table 2</xref>
and the light red trace in
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g003">Fig 3</xref>
). Again, this vertical bias in the tactile modality is weak (maximal 62% vertical motion perception).</p>
<table-wrap id="pone.0152736.t002" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0152736.t002</object-id>
<label>Table 2</label>
<caption>
<title>Statistical Results from Experiment 2.</title>
</caption>
<alternatives>
<graphic id="pone.0152736.t002g" xlink:href="pone.0152736.t002"></graphic>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<colgroup span="1">
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
</colgroup>
<thead>
<tr>
<th align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">between-forearm stim. dist. [%]</th>
<th align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">within-forearm stim. dist. [%]</th>
<th align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Aspect Ratio</th>
<th align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">p-Value</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">20</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">50</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.4</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.31</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">67</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">50</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">1.34</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.27</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">133</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">50</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">2.67</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.19</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">200</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">50</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">4.0</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.022</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">10</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">25</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.8</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.0054
<xref ref-type="table-fn" rid="t002fn002">*</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">67</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">25</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">2.67</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.0072
<xref ref-type="table-fn" rid="t002fn002">*</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">133</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">25</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">5.32</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.067</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">200</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">25</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">8.0</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.056</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</alternatives>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="t002fn001">
<p>Significance level after correction for multiple testing:</p>
</fn>
<fn id="t002fn002">
<p>* p < 0.05;</p>
</fn>
<fn id="t002fn003">
<p>Stimulator distances expressed as % total forearm length</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
</sec>
<sec id="sec024" sec-type="conclusions">
<title>Discussion</title>
<p>In Experiment 2, we tested whether a further increase of AR might further stabilize the weak vertical motion bias from Experiment 1 to values comparable to the visual modality.</p>
<p>For the 50% within-forearm/vertical stimulator distance AR values between 0 and 4 stayed in the range of Experiment 2 and motion perception remained largely ambiguous, close to a value of 0.5 (equal probability for horizontal and vertical motion perception). Only at AR = 4 we found a weak tendency for vertical motion bias, which however did not remain significant after multiple testing correction according to Holm (see
<xref ref-type="table" rid="pone.0152736.t002">Table 2</xref>
and dark red trace in
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g003">Fig 3</xref>
).</p>
<p>The 25% within-forearm stimulator distance extended the range of ARs to 8. With smaller within-forearm stimulator distances the vertical motion bias seemed to occur already for smaller ARs. However it stayed at a weak level of around 65%, which seemed to be the maximal available degree of perceptual disambiguation of apparent motion information for the tactile modality—at least in the so far tested range of ARs (see
<xref ref-type="table" rid="pone.0152736.t002">Table 2</xref>
and light red trace in
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g003">Fig 3</xref>
).</p>
<p>In summary, increasing AR by decreasing vertical (within-forearm) stimulator distance seemed to promote the occurrence of a vertical motion bias but doubling AR did not further increase its intensity.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="sec025">
<title>Experiment 3: Remapping Endogenous and Exogenous Reference Frames</title>
<sec id="sec026" sec-type="intro">
<title>Background</title>
<p>Perception of ambiguous tactile SAM stimuli remained highly ambiguous, even for very small ARs. SAM motion perception was only weakly disambiguated towards vertical motion and only for very large ARs up to 8. In Experiment 3 we tested whether the weak vertical motion bias for large ARs and the missing horizontal bias for small ARs result from confirmative and contradictory evidences from the two reference frames. In Experiment 3, participants’ forearms were rotated by ±90° in order to change the mapping of endogenous to exogenous reference axes. After this forearm rotation, vertical motion (exogenous reference frame) was related to between-forearm motion (endogenous reference frame) and vice versa (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g001">Fig 1</xref>
). If our results can be explained by the interaction of reference frame information, this realignment of reference frames should markedly change the pattern of results. At small ARs, we should expect to find higher likelihood for horizontal motion because small horizontal stimulator distances in exogenous coordinates correspond to small distances in endogenous coordinates, and large vertical stimulator distances in exogenous coordinates correspond to large distances in endogenous coordinates. At large ARs, in contrast, we would expect contradictory evidence, because small vertical stimulator distances in exogenous coordinates correspond to large distances in endogenous coordinates. According to these considerations we would expect a (weak) horizontal motion bias, but no more vertical bias.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec027">
<title>Participants</title>
<p>Twelve participants (7 males, 5 females; age range 20–28 years, mean age = 24.4) participated in Experiment 3. All but one of the participants were right-handed. One participant of Experiment 3 had participated also in Experiment 1, whereas three had participated in Experiment 2.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec028">
<title>Paradigm</title>
<p>Experiment 3 was identical to Experiment 2 with the following changes:</p>
<p>(
<italic>i</italic>
) The forearm positions were rotated by ±90° compared to Experiments 1 and 2. This resulted in forearm orientations orthogonal to the straight-ahead direction (see
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g001">Fig 1</xref>
). (
<italic>ii</italic>
) We kept the two within-forearm stimulator distances from Experiment 2 but used only the first three between-forearm distances (20%, 66% and 133%), leaving out the fourth one (200%), which is anatomically impossible in this configuration. This resulted in 6 combinations of within- and between-forearm distances (corresponding to the following 6 ARs: for 25% within-forearm distance, 0.19, 0.37, 1.25; for the 50% within-forearm distance, 0.38, 0.75, 2.5). Each AR was presented twice, i.e. in two blocks.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec029" sec-type="results">
<title>Results</title>
<p>The mean percentage of percepts other than vertical or horizontal motion across conditions was 3.0 ± 1.4%.</p>
<p>For the 50% vertical (within-forearm) stimulator distances only large horizontal (between-forearm) stimulator distances—in this case corresponding to small ARs—showed a weak tendency for a perceptual bias towards vertical motion perception (see
<xref ref-type="table" rid="pone.0152736.t003">Table 3</xref>
and dark blue trace in
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g003">Fig 3</xref>
), which was insignificant after multiple testing corrections. For the 25% vertical (within-forearm) stimulator distance, the vertical bias in motion perception was again observable only for large horizontal (between-forearm) stimulator distances (i.e. small ARs). It remained significant for an AR of 0.37 after correction for multiple testing (
<xref ref-type="table" rid="pone.0152736.t003">Table 3</xref>
and light blue trace in
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g003">Fig 3</xref>
).</p>
<table-wrap id="pone.0152736.t003" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0152736.t003</object-id>
<label>Table 3</label>
<caption>
<title>Statistical Results (p values) from Experiment 3.</title>
</caption>
<alternatives>
<graphic id="pone.0152736.t003g" xlink:href="pone.0152736.t003"></graphic>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<colgroup span="1">
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
</colgroup>
<thead>
<tr>
<th align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">between-forearm stim. dist. [%]</th>
<th align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">within-forearm stim. dist. [%]</th>
<th align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Aspect Ratio</th>
<th align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">p-value</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">20</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">50</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">2.5</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.7</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">67</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">50</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.75</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.63</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">133</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">50</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.38</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.042</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">20</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">25</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">1.25</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.16</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">67</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">25</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.37</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.0076
<xref ref-type="table-fn" rid="t003fn002">*</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">133</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">25</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.19</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.051</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</alternatives>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="t003fn001">
<p>Significance level after correction for multiple testing:</p>
</fn>
<fn id="t003fn002">
<p>*p < 0.05;</p>
</fn>
<fn id="t003fn003">
<p>Stimulator distances expressed as % total forearm length</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
</sec>
<sec id="sec030" sec-type="conclusions">
<title>Discussion</title>
<p>In this experiment we rotated the forearms by ±90°, with an additional realignment of exogenous and endogenous reference frames and in particular a reassignment of within- and between-forearm motion to horizontal and vertical motion. We had expected opposite results compared to Experiments 1 and 2 with a horizontal motion bias for small ARs and ambiguous percepts for large ARs but no vertical bias. However, we found again a weak perceptual bias towards vertical motion perception, which corresponds in this experiment to between-forearm motion. Nonetheless, the effect is smaller in Experiment 3 compared to Experiments 1 and 2. Perceptual data from the visual modality predict a clear horizontal motion bias at such a small AR value.</p>
<p>These results are in contrast to our above hypothesis, which makes explanations based on the interaction of reference frames less plausible. One possible alternative explanation of these unexpected results may be the existence of general preference for vertical motion (described in exogenous coordinates). In this case, the information from the endogenous reference frame may have been underweighted during the perceptual disambiguation process in Experiment 3 and thus forearm rotation may have been largely ignored.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="sec031">
<title>Experiment 4: Crossing Forearms, Variant 1</title>
<sec id="sec032" sec-type="intro">
<title>Background</title>
<p>The tactile motion perception was ambiguous for a broad range of SAM ARs with only a weak vertical but no horizontal bias. Remapping of endogenous and exogenous reference frames by 90° forearm rotation did not change the pattern of results. To further test the role of the reference frame interaction on tactile motion perception we tested a condition with 45° forearm rotation.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec033">
<title>Participants</title>
<p>11 participants (6 males, 5 females; age range 23–27, mean age = 26.5) participated in Experiment 4. All but one of the participants was right-handed. One participant had participated in Experiment 2 and seven had participated in Experiment 3.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec034">
<title>Tactile SAM stimuli and Paradigm</title>
<p>Experiment 4 was similar to Experiment 1 with the following exceptions:</p>
<p>(
<italic>i</italic>
) The position of the arms was rotated by 45° and thus crossed compared to the forearm posture in Experiment 1, with the left forearm placed above the right forearm and the two forearms now touching each other (see
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g001">Fig 1</xref>
). (
<italic>ii</italic>
) We used only one stimulus configuration with equal horizontal and vertical stimulator distances of 50% total forearm length, resulting in an AR = 1. (
<italic>iii</italic>
) The stimulation pattern was altered, compared to the previous experiments, in order to keep the direction of motion perception ambiguous. The two stimulators from one forearm were now active in synchrony and in alternation with the two stimulators from the other forearm (see also
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g001">Fig 1</xref>
), in order to allow horizontal and vertical motion perceptions (expressed in exogenous coordinates).</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec035" sec-type="results">
<title>Results</title>
<p>The mean percentage of percepts other than vertical or horizontal motion was 0.5 ± 0.5%.</p>
<p>With this crossed-forearm configuration we found a highly significant preference for horizontal motion (87% ± 6%, p = 1.9•10
<sup>−6</sup>
), which is in contrast to the results from all three previous experiments (see
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g004">Fig 4</xref>
).</p>
<fig id="pone.0152736.g004" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0152736.g004</object-id>
<label>Fig 4</label>
<caption>
<title>Comparison of %vertical motion perceptions from the five experiments.</title>
<p>Percentage of vertical motion percepts, selectively for the conditions with AR = 1 (i.e. equal within- and between-forearm stimulator distances) across all 5 experiments. The horizontal dotted line indicates chance level, i.e. equal probability for vertical and horizontal motion percepts. A weak vertical bias is indicated (but non-significant) in Exp. 1. Overall motion perception stayed ambiguous for Experiments 2 and 3 but was strongly biased towards horizontal motion in Experiments 4 and 5 with 45°-rotation of the forearms. Error bars indicate SEMs. *** indicates p<0.001 (corrected).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0152736.g004"></graphic>
</fig>
</sec>
<sec id="sec036" sec-type="conclusions">
<title>Discussion</title>
<p>Rotating the forearms by only 45° (instead of 90° as in
<xref ref-type="sec" rid="sec025">Experiment 3</xref>
) completely changed the pattern of tactile motion perception results. We found a strong perceptual bias towards horizontal motion perception (≈ 87%). It was necessary to change the pattern of stimulation in this experiment in order to keep the stimulus ambiguous and thus to allow for both horizontal and vertical motion perception (see
<xref ref-type="sec" rid="sec006">Methods</xref>
and
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g001">Fig 1</xref>
), which clearly complicates comparisons with previous experiments. But this hardly explains these unexpectedly clear results and a conclusive explanation is difficult.</p>
<p>There are two possible interpretations of these surprising results:</p>
<p>(
<italic>i</italic>
) The perceptual system takes the crossed forearms into account (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g005">Fig 5A.a</xref>
). If so, the tactile stimulation should evoke the perception of two dots moving back and forth in opposite (i.e. antiparallel) directions and both horizontal and vertical motion perceptions are in principle possible. The reason for the strong horizontal bias in our data, is unclear, however.</p>
<p>(
<italic>ii</italic>
) The perceptual system ignores the forearm-crossing (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g005">Fig 5A.b</xref>
). Then the tactile stimulation should evoke the perception of two dots moving left and right in parallel. In this case only horizontal motion perception would be possible, which would indeed explain our results.</p>
<fig id="pone.0152736.g005" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0152736.g005</object-id>
<label>Fig 5</label>
<caption>
<title>Tactile stimulation modes and possible motion percepts.</title>
<p>Top: the representation of the two different tactile SAM stimulation modes from Experiments 4 and 5. Middle: the possible motion percepts, with horizontal or vertical direction and the relation of the perceived motion directions of the two dots’ streams (parallel or antiparallel). Bottom: Indication of whether spontaneous perceptual alternations are possible, given the perceived arm configuration and perceived motion direction.</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0152736.g005"></graphic>
</fig>
<p>In order to control for the two possibilities, we asked six of our participants after the experiment about their percept with respect to the two options. Three of them perceived antiparallel horizontal motion, which indicates that the crossing of the forearms was integrated in the perceptual construct (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g005">Fig 5A.a</xref>
). The other three participants reported parallel horizontal motion, which indicates that the arm crossing was ignored during perceptual construction (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g005">Fig 5A.b</xref>
).</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="sec037">
<title>Experiment 5: Crossing Forearms, Variant 2</title>
<sec id="sec038" sec-type="intro">
<title>Background</title>
<p>A forearm rotation of only 45° provided the strongest perceptual disambiguation effect on tactile SAM motion perception. However, in contrast to the previous results and to our surprise, perception was biased towards horizontal motion. In order to further understand this puzzling result from Experiment 4, we added Experiment 5 with two conditions. In Experiment 4 we had changed the order of tactile stimulation from Experiments 1–3, in order to allow for two possible perceptual interpretations of motion direction and thus keep the stimulus ambiguous. In Condition 1 of Experiment 5, we simply replicated Experiment 4, whereas in Condition 2, we kept the crossed arms but stimulated in the same order as in Experiments 1 to 3. We would like to acknowledge one anonymous reviewer for this suggestion.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec039">
<title>Participants</title>
<p>6 participants (2 males, 4 females; age range 24–32, mean age = 27.5) participated in Experiment 5. Five participants were right-handed and one was left-handed. No participant had participated in Experiments 2; two had participated in Experiment 3 and three had participated in Experiment 4.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec040">
<title>Tactile SAM stimuli and Paradigm</title>
<p>Experiment 5 consisted of two conditions: Condition 1 was a replication of Experiment 4: The two stimulators from one forearm were active in synchrony and in alternation with the two stimulators from the other forearm.</p>
<p>Condition 2 served as control to Condition 1: Here the stimulator closest to the hand of one arm was active in synchrony with the stimulator closest to the crook of the other arm and vice versa. This stimulation mode corresponds to that of Experiments 1–3.</p>
<p>Like in Experiment 4, we tested only one configuration with equal between- and within-forearm stimulator distances at 50% of the total forearm length.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec041" sec-type="results">
<title>Results</title>
<p>In Condition 1 we again found a highly significant preference for horizontal motion (94% ± 7%, p = 5.8•10
<sup>−6</sup>
), which confirms the results from Experiment 4 (see
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g004">Fig 4</xref>
and
<xref ref-type="table" rid="pone.0152736.t004">Table 4</xref>
). Five out of six participants reported parallel motion perception, indicating that the information about the crossed arms was not integrated into motion processing. Additionally, only one participant indicated antiparallel motion perception.</p>
<table-wrap id="pone.0152736.t004" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0152736.t004</object-id>
<label>Table 4</label>
<caption>
<title>Results from Experiment 5.</title>
</caption>
<alternatives>
<graphic id="pone.0152736.t004g" xlink:href="pone.0152736.t004"></graphic>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<colgroup span="1">
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
</colgroup>
<thead>
<tr>
<th align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1"></th>
<th align="center" colspan="2" rowspan="1">Condition 1 (Replication of
<xref ref-type="sec" rid="sec031">Exp. 4</xref>
)</th>
<th align="center" colspan="2" rowspan="1">Condition 2</th>
</tr>
<tr>
<th align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Participant</th>
<th align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Motion Percepts</th>
<th align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Reports</th>
<th align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Motion Percepts</th>
<th align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Reports</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">A</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">100% horiz.</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">parallel</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">100% horiz.</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">antiparallel</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">B</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">100% horiz.</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">parallel</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">95% horiz.</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">antiparallel</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">C</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">100% horiz.</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">parallel</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">100% horiz.</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">parallel</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">D</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">95% horiz.</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">parallel</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">52% horiz.</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">both</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">E</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">100% horiz.</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">antiparallel</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">100% horiz.</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">both</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">F</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">65% horiz.</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">parallel</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">61% horiz.</td>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">antiparallel</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</alternatives>
</table-wrap>
<p>In Condition 2, where we used the stimulation order from Experiments 1–3, we found a similarly strong preference for horizontal motion (85% ± 9%, p = 9.2•10
<sup>−4</sup>
). Three out of six participants reported having always had antiparallel motion percepts. Two participants reported having had sometimes antiparallel and sometimes parallel motion percepts. Only one participant reported having had always parallel motion percepts. The participants’ perceptual reports from Experiments 5 are summarized in
<xref ref-type="table" rid="pone.0152736.t004">Table 4</xref>
.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec042" sec-type="conclusions">
<title>Discussion</title>
<p>Condition 1 of Experiment 5 replicated the surprising results of close to 90% horizontal motion percepts from Experiment 4. We had to change the stimulation order in Experiment 4 compared to Experiments 1–3, in order to keep the tactile stimulation ambiguous. In the present Experiment 5, we repeated Experiment 4 and added a second condition with crossed arms but the stimulation order from Experiments 1–3. To our surprise, the strong horizontal bias remained. The two conditions from Experiment 5 help to understand the puzzling results from Experiment 4 at least partially, if we postulate the following: the perceptual system of some participants always or sometimes ignored the crossed forearm position during motion perception, even though the crossed forearms touched each other.</p>
<p>The ignorance of the forearm-crossing and the alternative assumption of parallel arms implies horizontal motion as the only interpretation of the tactile stimulation in Condition 1. The two dots will then be perceived as moving in parallel as illustrated in (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g005">Fig 5A.b</xref>
). This is what was reported by participants A, B, C and D in Condition 1 (see
<xref ref-type="table" rid="pone.0152736.t004">Table 4</xref>
). The ignorance of the forearm-crossing together with the stimulation mode from Condition 2 would lead to motion ambiguity, i.e. both horizontal and vertical motion percepts are possible, but the two dots should be perceived as moving antiparallel as illustrated in (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g005">Fig 5B.b1 and 5B.b2</xref>
). Participants A and B both report almost uniquely horizontal-antiparallel motion perception in Condition 2. Participant B also indicates some alternations of perceived motion direction in Condition 2, as above predicted.</p>
<p>An interesting observation of Experiment 5 is the existence of two types of perceptual alternations. First, participants’ percepts can alternate between horizontal and vertical motion perception. Second, participants motion processing can alternate between a mode
<italic>with ignorance of forearm-crossing</italic>
and a mode
<italic>with integration of forearm-crossing</italic>
. Participants C, D may be candidates for the latter observer type: Both seemed to ignore forearm-crossing in Condition 1 and thus reported 95% and 100% parallel-horizontal motion percepts. Participant C further reported also 100% parallel horizontal-motion percepts in Condition 2, which is only possible if he integrated the crossed-forearms in his motion processing, as illustrated in (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g005">Fig 5B.a</xref>
).</p>
<p>Interestingly, Participant D reported 52% horizontal motion percepts with both parallel and antiparallel motion percepts in Condition 2. This is only possible if the participant alternated between integration of the forearm-crossing and ignorance of the forearm-crossing within one experimental condition, as illustrated in (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g005">Fig 5B.a and 5B.b</xref>
). Similarly, the results from Participant E can only be explained if we assume alternations between integration (results from Condition1) and ignorance of forearm-crossing (results from Condition 2).</p>
<p>Participant F's reports from Condition 2 also nicely fit to our explanation. He perceived horizontal and vertical antiparallel motion and thus must have ignored the forearm-crossing as illustrated in (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g005">Fig 5B.b1 and 5B.b2</xref>
).</p>
<p>The only exceptional data in Experiment 5 are from Participant F in Condition 1, where he reported vertical and horizontal parallel motion percepts. Vertical parallel motion perception is not possible from our line of reasoning (see
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0152736.g005">Fig 5A.a.2</xref>
). Whether Participant F erroneously pressed the wrong button will stay unresolved.</p>
<p>In summary, our postulate of two types of perceptual alternations—concerning motion direction and concerning integration or ignorance of forearm crossing—and different observer types, explained about 92% of the data.</p>
<p>In the present experiment the parallel or antiparallel motion perception was only reported at the end of the measurement, because Experiment 5 was aimed as a control to Experiment 4 and we tried to keep everything beyond the presentation mode identical. These types of tactile stimulation experiments with crossed forearms need to be replicated and extended. In particular, participants should report with each presentation both the motion direction and whether they perceived parallel or antiparallel motion.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="sec043">
<title>General Discussion</title>
<p>In the present study we analyzed motion perception instability of tactile versions of von Schiller’s [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref004" ref-type="bibr">4</xref>
] SAM motion stimulus as a function of stimulus ARs (i.e. the ratio between horizontal vertical dot distances) with a specific focus on the relation between the two reference frames involved in tactile motion perception.</p>
<p>Tactile SAM stimulation, with vibrators attached to the participants forearms, evoked tactile apparent motion percepts, with similarities to the visual modality: In both modalities motion perception gets unstable at certain stimulus ARs. Further, there is a general preference/bias towards vertical motion perception (i.e. motion from and to the body in the tactile modality). We also found differences between visual and tactile apparent motion perception. It is well known that decreasing ARs strongly biases visual SAM perception to horizontal motion, whereas tactile SAM motion perception stays ambiguous even for very small ARs. Increasing visual SAM ARs strongly biases visual perception towards vertical motion. We also found a vertical bias for the tactile SAM, however, the effect was much weaker (< 70%) than in the visual modality (> 90%), even with very large tactile ARs up to 8.</p>
<sec id="sec044">
<title>About the differences between modalities</title>
<p>Our tactile SAM experiments differ necessarily in a number of aspects (stimulus size, timing, frequency of point alternations, etc.) from those in the visual modality, which may explain the differences in results. However, several modifications of visual SAM experiments did not alter the strong perceptual biases at extreme AR values, e.g. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref019" ref-type="bibr">19</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref034" ref-type="bibr">34</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref035" ref-type="bibr">35</xref>
], indicating the robustness of this effect.</p>
<p>An alternative explanation for the differences between visual and tactile apparent motion results may be, that tactile motion perception requires the alignment of the information from the two reference frames, whereas in vision there is only one. At certain stimulus configurations the two reference frames may provide contradictory information, probably destabilizing perceptual constructs. The results from Experiments 1 and 2 point in this direction. We further studied this potential explanation in Experiments 3–5 by rotating the forearms and thus changing the alignment between exogenous and endogenous reference axes. To our surprise, a 90° forearm rotation still supported the primary results: we again found a broad AR range with perceptual instability, a weak bias for horizontal motion, but no bias at all for horizontal motion. Finally, and even more surprisingly, a ±45° forearm rotation evoked a strong horizontal bias (> 87%) even in a configuration with an AR = 1. These findings question the conflict of reference frames as general explanation for the difference between visual and tactile apparent motion perception.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec045">
<title>About the similarities between modalities</title>
<p>There are numerous stimulus examples for perceptual ambiguity both within and between modalities. One important question is whether all these different examples share some common principles and potentially also common neural sources, a possibility discussed at length in previous accounts [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref011" ref-type="bibr">11</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref036" ref-type="bibr">36</xref>
]. This refers to the old and still vivid bottom-up vs. top-down debate of whether ambiguity is resolved at lower sensory (bottom-up), or at higher cognitive levels (top-down) beyond sensory modalities. For reviewed, see [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref037" ref-type="bibr">37</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref038" ref-type="bibr">38</xref>
]. The present SAM stimuli are well suited to test this generality assumption. The most obvious commonality between visual and tactile motion ambiguity we found, was a general preference for vertical motion (expressed in exogenous coordinates), even though the effect in the tactile modality was much weaker than in the visual modality. Is this preference for vertical motion controlled by a system beyond sensory modalities, which would be in accordance with the top-down approach? And why should such a system prefer vertical and not horizontal motion?</p>
<p>One prominent explanation for the vertical motion bias in the visual modality refers to
<italic>inter-hemispheric integration hypothesis</italic>
[
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref021" ref-type="bibr">21</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref022" ref-type="bibr">22</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref039" ref-type="bibr">39</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref040" ref-type="bibr">40</xref>
]. In short, this hypothesis maintains that, because the right and left visual hemifields are processed in the contralateral brain hemispheres, vertical motion is processed within hemispheres, while horizontal motion needs the interplay of hemispheres. Processing horizontal motion is thus more difficult and less preferred as a solution during perceptual disambiguation.</p>
<p>The same reasoning may apply to tactile motion perception. The endogenous reference frame is most probably based on the somatotopic body representation in the somatosensory cortex. It is known that the left forearm is represented in the right hemisphere and vice versa, e.g. [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref041" ref-type="bibr">41</xref>
]. Perceived within-forearm motion should thus be processed within hemispheres whereas between-forearm motion would require the correspondence between hemispheres. The exogenous reference frame is most probably shared with the visual modality [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref042" ref-type="bibr">42</xref>
] and thus again, what we called perceived vertical motion should have been processed within hemispheres and perceived horizontal motion should have needed the interplay between hemispheres. This reasoning might help to explain the finding of a (weak) tactile vertical bias in Experiments 2 and 3. However, it is difficult to reconcile this explanation with the results from Experiment 3. After ±90° rotation of the forearms, both horizontal and vertical motion directions need both hemispheres and no preference for one motion direction should be observed. However, the patterns of results stay unchanged concerning the weak vertical bias. It is even more difficult to reconcile the results from Experiments 4 and 5 (forearm rotation of ±45°) with the inter-hemispheric integration theory: Motion integration with respect to the endogenous reference frame needs inter-hemispheric integration for both motion directions. For the exogenous reference frame inter-hemispheric integration in the crossed-arm condition is only necessary for horizontal motion. Again, assuming inter-hemispheric communication as more effortful, at least a slight vertical bias would be expected. But we found the opposite, namely a horizontal motion bias, which was much stronger (87%) than all previous tactile vertical motion biases, a finding that we replicated in Experiment 5 with a new set of participants.</p>
<p>Another important factor may be our everyday experience with gravity, which probably more often produces a vertical trajectory on our retinae and thus may, due to long-term memory,
<italic>a priori</italic>
bias motion perception in the case of ambiguous sensory evidence. Impressive examples of how gravity influences perception can be found in Clément & Demel [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref043" ref-type="bibr">43</xref>
] and Clément & Wood [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref044" ref-type="bibr">44</xref>
].</p>
<p>For the visual SAM, where only one reference system is available, gravity or the inter-hemispheric integration approach or even both may explain the vertical motion bias. For the tactile SAM the perceptual system has to adjust the information from two reference frames and evidences from those can become contradictory. Such a situation may increase the range of stimulus parameters for instability of motion perception, as found for the tactile stimuli. Top-down factors, like experience with gravity, may then tip the balance between interpretations in one or the other direction. This, however, cannot explain the strong horizontal bias, found in Experiments 4 and 5.</p>
<p>In summary, it is neither clear why tactile apparent motion perception stays ambiguous for a broad range of ARs, nor what constitutes the tactile vertical motion bias. It is also unclear whether this weak vertical bias shares some causal mechanisms with the strong visual vertical SAM motion bias and whether inter-hemispheric integration is the mainly relevant factor or only one of several influencing factors. The sudden and strong disambiguation of the otherwise highly ambiguous tactile motion perception in a crossed-forearm condition in Experiments 4 and 5 is clearly the most puzzling finding of the present study and further studies are necessary to understand this phenomenon.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec sec-type="conclusions" id="sec046">
<title>Conclusions</title>
<p>Overall, our present data are good examples of the general perceptual inference problem [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref045" ref-type="bibr">45</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref046" ref-type="bibr">46</xref>
]. The availability of information from the world around us is restricted by the capacities of our senses. The available information is thus inherently incomplete and to a varying degree ambiguous, even though we normally do not notice this. Every available bit of information has potential relevance and enters into a (Bayesian) probability calculation, necessary to construct stable and reliable percepts and thus to solve the perceptual inference problem [
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref045" ref-type="bibr">45</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0152736.ref046" ref-type="bibr">46</xref>
]. In the case of tactile apparent SAM motion percepts, our results suggest that there is no fixed rule or relevance hierarchy for the weighting of bottom-up and top-down evidence. Rather, as evident from the differential outcomes of Experiments 1 and 2 on one hand and Experiment 3 and in particular Experiments 4 and 5 on the other hand, the perceptual outcome depends on the current context and in particular on the quality of available information at each perceptual moment. It also depends on the perceptual memory content and probably long-term perceptual statistics (e.g. gravity), as well as on the psychological state and individual perceptual strategy of the observer. The brain architecture with its two hemispheres may also play a role. In the case of sensory ambiguity, slight changes of the sensory input can evoke sudden and fundamental perceptual changes, like a change from a weak vertical to a strong horizontal motion bias, when we change the forearm rotation angle by only ± 45° (Experiments
<xref ref-type="sec" rid="sec031">4</xref>
and
<xref ref-type="sec" rid="sec037">5</xref>
compared to Experiments
<xref ref-type="sec" rid="sec011">1</xref>
<xref ref-type="sec" rid="sec025">3</xref>
). Our perceptual system is flexible enough to quickly and fundamentally change perceptual interpretations, which may have been one major advantage during evolution.</p>
</sec>
</body>
<back>
<ref-list>
<title>References</title>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref001">
<label>1</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Kornmeier</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Mayer</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>The alien in the forest OR when temporal context dominates perception</article-title>
.
<source>Perception</source>
.
<year>2014</year>
;
<volume>43</volume>
:
<fpage>1270</fpage>
<lpage>1274</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p7844">10.1068/p7844</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">25638942</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref002">
<label>2</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Necker</surname>
<given-names>LA</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Observations on some remarkable optical phaenomena seen in Switzerland; and on an optical phaenomenon which occurs on viewing a figure of a crystal or geometrical solid</article-title>
.
<source>Lond Edinb Philos Mag J Sci</source>
.
<year>1832</year>
;
<volume>1</volume>
:
<fpage>329</fpage>
<lpage>337</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref003">
<label>3</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Ammons</surname>
<given-names>RB</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Ulrich</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Voluntary control over perceived dimensionality (perspective) of three-dimensional objects</article-title>
.
<source>Proc Mont Acad Sci</source>
.
<year>1959</year>
;
<volume>19</volume>
:
<fpage>169</fpage>
<lpage>173</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref004">
<label>4</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Schiller</surname>
<given-names>PV</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Stroboskopische Alternativversuche</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol Forsch</source>
.
<year>1933</year>
;
<volume>17</volume>
:
<fpage>179</fpage>
<lpage>214</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref005">
<label>5</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Boring</surname>
<given-names>EG</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>A new ambiguous figure</article-title>
.
<source>Am J Psychol</source>
.
<year>1930</year>
;
<volume>42</volume>
:
<fpage>444</fpage>
<lpage>445</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1415447">10.2307/1415447</ext-link>
</comment>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref006">
<label>6</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Blake</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Logothetis</surname>
<given-names>NK</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Visual competition</article-title>
.
<source>Nat Rev Neurosci</source>
.
<year>2002</year>
;
<volume>3</volume>
:
<fpage>13</fpage>
<lpage>21</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn701">10.1038/nrn701</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11823801</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref007">
<label>7</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>O’Shea</surname>
<given-names>RP</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Kornmeier</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Roeber</surname>
<given-names>U</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Predicting visual consciousness electrophysiologically from intermittent binocular rivalry</article-title>
.
<name>
<surname>Ward</surname>
<given-names>LM</given-names>
</name>
, editor.
<source>PLoS ONE</source>
.
<year>2013</year>
;
<volume>8</volume>
:
<fpage>e76134</fpage>
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076134">10.1371/journal.pone.0076134</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">24124536</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref008">
<label>8</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Brascamp</surname>
<given-names>JW</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>van Ee</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Pestman</surname>
<given-names>WR</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>van den Berg</surname>
<given-names>AV</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Distributions of alternation rates in various forms of bistable perception</article-title>
.
<source>J Vis</source>
.
<year>2005</year>
;
<volume>5</volume>
:
<fpage>287</fpage>
<lpage>298</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/5.4.1">10.1167/5.4.1</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15929652</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref009">
<label>9</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Wernery</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Atmanspacher</surname>
<given-names>H</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Kornmeier</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Candia</surname>
<given-names>V</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Folkers</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Wittmann</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Temporal processing in bistable perception of the Necker cube</article-title>
.
<source>Perception</source>
.
<year>2015</year>
;
<volume>44</volume>
:
<fpage>157</fpage>
<lpage>168</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p7780">10.1068/p7780</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">26561969</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref010">
<label>10</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Kornmeier</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Hein</surname>
<given-names>CM</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Bach</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Multistable perception: when bottom-up and top-down coincide</article-title>
.
<source>Brain Cogn</source>
.
<year>2009</year>
;
<volume>69</volume>
:
<fpage>138</fpage>
<lpage>147</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2008.06.005">10.1016/j.bandc.2008.06.005</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18682314</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref011">
<label>11</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Leopold</surname>
<given-names>DA</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Logothetis</surname>
<given-names>NK</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Multistable phenomena: changing views in perception</article-title>
.
<source>Trends Cogn Sci</source>
.
<year>1999</year>
;
<volume>3</volume>
:
<fpage>254</fpage>
<lpage>264</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01332-7">10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01332-7</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10377540</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref012">
<label>12</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Miller</surname>
<given-names>GA</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>The trill threshold</article-title>
.
<source>J Acoust Soc Am</source>
.
<year>1950</year>
;
<volume>22</volume>
:
<fpage>637</fpage>
<lpage>8</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.1906663">10.1121/1.1906663</ext-link>
</comment>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref013">
<label>13</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Radilová</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Pöppel</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Ilmberger</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Auditory reversal timing</article-title>
.
<source>Act Nerv Super</source>
.
<year>1990</year>
;
<volume>32</volume>
:
<fpage>137</fpage>
<lpage>138</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref014">
<label>14</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Zhou</surname>
<given-names>W</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Chen</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Binaral rivalry between the nostrils and in the cortex</article-title>
.
<source>Curr Biol</source>
.
<year>2009</year>
;
<volume>19</volume>
:
<fpage>1561</fpage>
<lpage>1565</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.052">10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.052</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19699095</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref015">
<label>15</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Harrar</surname>
<given-names>V</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Harris</surname>
<given-names>LR</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Multimodal Ternus: Visual, tactile, and visuo—tactile grouping in apparent motion</article-title>
.
<source>Perception</source>
.
<year>2007</year>
;
<volume>36</volume>
:
<fpage>1455</fpage>
<lpage>1464</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p5844">10.1068/p5844</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18265828</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref016">
<label>16</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<name>
<surname>Pikler</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
.
<source>Sinnesphysiologische Untersuchungen</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Leipzig</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Barth</publisher-name>
;
<year>1917</year>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref017">
<label>17</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Hein</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Moore</surname>
<given-names>CM</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Spatio-temporal priority revisited: the role of feature identity and similarity for object correspondence in apparent motion</article-title>
.
<source>J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform</source>
.
<year>2012</year>
;
<volume>38</volume>
:
<fpage>975</fpage>
<lpage>988</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22564159</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref018">
<label>18</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Carter</surname>
<given-names>O</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Konkle</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Wang</surname>
<given-names>Q</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Hayward</surname>
<given-names>V</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Moore</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Tactile rivalry demonstrated with an ambiguous apparent-motion quartet</article-title>
.
<source>Curr Biol</source>
.
<year>2008</year>
;
<volume>18</volume>
:
<fpage>1050</fpage>
<lpage>1054</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2008.06.027">10.1016/j.cub.2008.06.027</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18635355</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref019">
<label>19</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Kruse</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Carmesin</surname>
<given-names>HO</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Pahlke</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Struber</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Stadler</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Continuous phase transitions in the perception of multistable visual patterns</article-title>
.
<source>Biol Cybern</source>
.
<year>1996</year>
;
<volume>75</volume>
:
<fpage>321</fpage>
<lpage>330</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s004220050298">10.1007/s004220050298</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8953741</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref020">
<label>20</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Bukowski</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Hock</surname>
<given-names>H</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Huisman</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Nichols</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Rivera</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Dynamical vs. judgmental comparison: hysteresis effects in motion perception</article-title>
.
<source>Spat Vis</source>
.
<year>2005</year>
;
<volume>18</volume>
:
<fpage>317</fpage>
<lpage>335</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/1568568054089393">10.1163/1568568054089393</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16060230</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref021">
<label>21</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Chaudhuri</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Glaser</surname>
<given-names>DA</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Metastable motion anisotropy</article-title>
.
<source>Vis Neurosci</source>
.
<year>1991</year>
;
<volume>7</volume>
:
<fpage>397</fpage>
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0952523800009706">10.1017/S0952523800009706</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">1764411</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref022">
<label>22</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Gengerelli</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Apparent movement in relation to homonymous and heteronymous stimulation of the cerebral hemispheres</article-title>
.
<source>J Exp Psychol</source>
.
<year>1948</year>
;
<volume>38</volume>
:
<fpage>592</fpage>
<lpage>599</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18885707</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref023">
<label>23</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Conrad</surname>
<given-names>V</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Vitello</surname>
<given-names>MP</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Noppeney</surname>
<given-names>U</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Interactions between apparent motion rivalry in vision and touch</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol Sci</source>
.
<year>2012</year>
;
<volume>23</volume>
:
<fpage>940</fpage>
<lpage>948</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797612438735">10.1177/0956797612438735</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22810167</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref024">
<label>24</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<name>
<surname>Vitello</surname>
<given-names>MP</given-names>
</name>
.
<source>Perception of moving tactile stimuli</source>
.
<publisher-name>Logos-Verlag</publisher-name>
<year>2010</year>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref025">
<label>25</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Bach</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>The Freiburg Visual Acuity test—automatic measurement of visual acuity</article-title>
.
<source>Optom Vis Sci</source>
.
<year>1996</year>
;
<volume>73</volume>
:
<fpage>49</fpage>
<lpage>53</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8867682</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref026">
<label>26</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Bach</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>The Freiburg Visual Acuity test-variability unchanged by post-hoc re-analysis</article-title>
.
<source>Graef Arch Clin Exp</source>
.
<year>2007</year>
;
<volume>245</volume>
:
<fpage>965</fpage>
<lpage>971</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref027">
<label>27</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<collab>World Medical Association</collab>
.
<article-title>Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects</article-title>
.
<source>JAMA</source>
.
<year>2000</year>
;
<volume>284</volume>
:
<fpage>3043</fpage>
<lpage>3045</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11122593</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref028">
<label>28</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<name>
<surname>Edgington</surname>
<given-names>ES</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Onghena</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
.
<source>Randomization Tests</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Boca Raton, FL, USA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Chapman & Hall/CRC</publisher-name>
;
<year>2007</year>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref029">
<label>29</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Holm</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure</article-title>
.
<source>Scand J Stat</source>
.
<year>1979</year>
;
<volume>6</volume>
:
<fpage>65</fpage>
<lpage>70</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref030">
<label>30</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Alais</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Burr</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration</article-title>
.
<source>Curr Biol</source>
.
<year>2004</year>
;
<volume>14</volume>
:
<fpage>257</fpage>
<lpage>262</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2004.01.029">10.1016/j.cub.2004.01.029</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14761661</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref031">
<label>31</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Hock</surname>
<given-names>HS</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Kelso</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Schoner</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Bistability and hysteresis in the organization of apparent motion patterns</article-title>
.
<source>J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform</source>
.
<year>1993</year>
;
<volume>19</volume>
:
<fpage>63</fpage>
<lpage>80</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8440989</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref032">
<label>32</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Gepshtein</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Kubovy</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>The lawful perception of apparent motion</article-title>
.
<source>J Vis</source>
.
<year>2007</year>
;
<volume>7</volume>
:
<fpage>9</fpage>
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/7.8.9">10.1167/7.8.9</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17685816</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref033">
<label>33</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Overvliet</surname>
<given-names>KE</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Krampe</surname>
<given-names>RT</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Wagemans</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Perceptual grouping in haptic search: The influence of proximity, similarity, and good continuation</article-title>
.
<source>J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform</source>
.
<year>2012</year>
;
<volume>38</volume>
:
<fpage>817</fpage>
<lpage>821</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029222">10.1037/a0029222</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22774798</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref034">
<label>34</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="other">van Rooij M, Atmanspacher H, Kornmeier J. Hysteresis in Processing of Perceptual Ambiguity on Three Different Time Scales. submitted;</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref035">
<label>35</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Maloney</surname>
<given-names>LT</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Martello</surname>
<given-names>MFD</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Sahm</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Spillmann</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Past trials influence perception of ambiguous motion quartets through pattern completion</article-title>
.
<source>Proc Natl Acad Sci</source>
.
<year>2005</year>
;
<volume>102</volume>
:
<fpage>3164</fpage>
<lpage>3169</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0407157102">10.1073/pnas.0407157102</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15710897</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref036">
<label>36</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Schwartz</surname>
<given-names>JL</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Grimault</surname>
<given-names>N</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Hupe</surname>
<given-names>JM</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Moore</surname>
<given-names>BC</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Pressnitzer</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Multistability in perception: binding sensory modalities, an overview</article-title>
.
<source>Philos T Roy Soc B</source>
.
<year>2012</year>
;
<volume>367</volume>
:
<fpage>896</fpage>
<lpage>905</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0254">10.1098/rstb.2011.0254</ext-link>
</comment>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref037">
<label>37</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Long</surname>
<given-names>GM</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Toppino</surname>
<given-names>TC</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Enduring interest in perceptual ambiguity: alternating views of reversible figures</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol Bull</source>
.
<year>2004</year>
;
<volume>130</volume>
:
<fpage>748</fpage>
<lpage>68</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15367079</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref038">
<label>38</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Kornmeier</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Bach</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Ambiguous figures—what happens in the brain when perception changes but not the stimulus</article-title>
.
<source>Front Hum Neurosci</source>
.
<year>2012</year>
;
<volume>6</volume>
:
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>23</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00051">10.3389/fnhum.2012.00051</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22279433</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref039">
<label>39</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Genç</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Bergmann</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Singer</surname>
<given-names>W</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Kohler</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Interhemispheric connections shape subjective experience of bistable motion</article-title>
.
<source>Curr Biol</source>
.
<year>2011</year>
;
<volume>21</volume>
:
<fpage>1494</fpage>
<lpage>1499</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.003">10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.003</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21885284</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref040">
<label>40</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Strüber</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Rach</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Trautmann-Lengsfeld</surname>
<given-names>SA</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Engel</surname>
<given-names>AK</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Herrmann</surname>
<given-names>CS</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Antiphasic 40 Hz oscillatory current stimulation affects bistable motion perception</article-title>
.
<source>Brain Topogr</source>
.
<year>2014</year>
;
<volume>27</volume>
:
<fpage>158</fpage>
<lpage>171</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10548-013-0294-x">10.1007/s10548-013-0294-x</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23709044</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref041">
<label>41</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Eickhoff</surname>
<given-names>SB</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Grefkes</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Fink</surname>
<given-names>GR</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Zilles</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Functional lateralization of face, hand, and trunk representation in anatomically defined human somatosensory areas</article-title>
.
<source>Cereb Cortex</source>
.
<year>2008</year>
;
<volume>18</volume>
:
<fpage>2820</fpage>
<lpage>2830</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn039">10.1093/cercor/bhn039</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18372289</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref042">
<label>42</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<name>
<surname>Heed</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Röder</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
.
<chapter-title>The Body in a Multisensory World</chapter-title>
In:
<name>
<surname>Murray</surname>
<given-names>MM</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Wallace</surname>
<given-names>MT</given-names>
</name>
, editors.
<source>The Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Boca Raton (FL)</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>CRC Press</publisher-name>
;
<year>2012</year>
Available:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK92834/">http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK92834/</ext-link>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref043">
<label>43</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Clément</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Demel</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Perceptual reversal of bi-stable figures in microgravity and hypergravity during parabolic flight</article-title>
.
<source>Neurosci Lett</source>
.
<year>2012</year>
;
<volume>507</volume>
:
<fpage>143</fpage>
<lpage>146</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2011.12.006">10.1016/j.neulet.2011.12.006</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22188656</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref044">
<label>44</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Clément</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Wood</surname>
<given-names>SJ</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Rocking or rolling-perception of ambiguous motion after returning from space</article-title>
.
<source>PloS One</source>
.
<year>2014</year>
;
<volume>9</volume>
:
<fpage>e111107</fpage>
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0111107">10.1371/journal.pone.0111107</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">25354042</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref045">
<label>45</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<name>
<surname>von Helmholtz</surname>
<given-names>H</given-names>
</name>
.
<source>Handbuch der physiologischen Optik</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Hamburg/Leipzig</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Leopold Voss</publisher-name>
;
<year>1911</year>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0152736.ref046">
<label>46</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Kersten</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Mamassian</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Yuille</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Object perception as Bayesian inference</article-title>
.
<source>Annu Rev Psychol</source>
.
<year>2004</year>
;
<volume>55</volume>
:
<fpage>271</fpage>
<lpage>304</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142005">10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142005</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14744217</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</pmc>
<affiliations>
<list>
<country>
<li>Allemagne</li>
</country>
<region>
<li>Bade-Wurtemberg</li>
<li>District de Fribourg-en-Brisgau</li>
</region>
<settlement>
<li>Fribourg-en-Brisgau</li>
</settlement>
</list>
<tree>
<country name="Allemagne">
<region name="Bade-Wurtemberg">
<name sortKey="Liaci, Emanuela" sort="Liaci, Emanuela" uniqKey="Liaci E" first="Emanuela" last="Liaci">Emanuela Liaci</name>
</region>
<name sortKey="Bach, Michael" sort="Bach, Michael" uniqKey="Bach M" first="Michael" last="Bach">Michael Bach</name>
<name sortKey="Bach, Michael" sort="Bach, Michael" uniqKey="Bach M" first="Michael" last="Bach">Michael Bach</name>
<name sortKey="Heinrich, Sven P" sort="Heinrich, Sven P" uniqKey="Heinrich S" first="Sven P." last="Heinrich">Sven P. Heinrich</name>
<name sortKey="Heinrich, Sven P" sort="Heinrich, Sven P" uniqKey="Heinrich S" first="Sven P." last="Heinrich">Sven P. Heinrich</name>
<name sortKey="Kornmeier, Jurgen" sort="Kornmeier, Jurgen" uniqKey="Kornmeier J" first="Jürgen" last="Kornmeier">Jürgen Kornmeier</name>
<name sortKey="Kornmeier, Jurgen" sort="Kornmeier, Jurgen" uniqKey="Kornmeier J" first="Jürgen" last="Kornmeier">Jürgen Kornmeier</name>
<name sortKey="Kornmeier, Jurgen" sort="Kornmeier, Jurgen" uniqKey="Kornmeier J" first="Jürgen" last="Kornmeier">Jürgen Kornmeier</name>
<name sortKey="Liaci, Emanuela" sort="Liaci, Emanuela" uniqKey="Liaci E" first="Emanuela" last="Liaci">Emanuela Liaci</name>
<name sortKey="Liaci, Emanuela" sort="Liaci, Emanuela" uniqKey="Liaci E" first="Emanuela" last="Liaci">Emanuela Liaci</name>
<name sortKey="Liaci, Emanuela" sort="Liaci, Emanuela" uniqKey="Liaci E" first="Emanuela" last="Liaci">Emanuela Liaci</name>
<name sortKey="Tebartz Van Elst, Ludger" sort="Tebartz Van Elst, Ludger" uniqKey="Tebartz Van Elst L" first="Ludger" last="Tebartz Van Elst">Ludger Tebartz Van Elst</name>
<name sortKey="Tebartz Van Elst, Ludger" sort="Tebartz Van Elst, Ludger" uniqKey="Tebartz Van Elst L" first="Ludger" last="Tebartz Van Elst">Ludger Tebartz Van Elst</name>
</country>
</tree>
</affiliations>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Ticri/CIDE/explor/HapticV1/Data/Pmc/Checkpoint
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 000167 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Checkpoint/biblio.hfd -nk 000167 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Ticri/CIDE
   |area=    HapticV1
   |flux=    Pmc
   |étape=   Checkpoint
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     PMC:4865082
   |texte=   Ambiguity in Tactile Apparent Motion Perception
}}

Pour générer des pages wiki

HfdIndexSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Checkpoint/RBID.i   -Sk "pubmed:27171276" \
       | HfdSelect -Kh $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Checkpoint/biblio.hfd   \
       | NlmPubMed2Wicri -a HapticV1 

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.23.
Data generation: Mon Jun 13 01:09:46 2016. Site generation: Wed Mar 6 09:54:07 2024