Serveur d'exploration sur les dispositifs haptiques

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

The rubber foot illusion

Identifieur interne : 003C08 ( Ncbi/Merge ); précédent : 003C07; suivant : 003C09

The rubber foot illusion

Auteurs : Simona Crea [Italie] ; Marco D Lonzo [Italie] ; Nicola Vitiello [Italie] ; Christian Cipriani [Italie]

Source :

RBID : PMC:4559902

Abstract

Background

Lower-limb amputation causes the individual a huge functional impairment due to the lack of adequate sensory perception from the missing limb. The development of an augmenting sensory feedback device able to restore some of the missing information from the amputated limb may improve embodiment, control and acceptability of the prosthesis.

Findings

In this work we transferred the Rubber Hand Illusion paradigm to the lower limb. We investigated the possibility of promoting body ownership of a fake foot, in a series of experiments fashioned after the RHI using matched or mismatched (vibrotactile) stimulation. The results, collected from 19 healthy subjects, demonstrated that it is possible to elicit the perception of possessing a rubber foot when modality-matched stimulations are provided synchronously on the biological foot and to the corresponding rubber foot areas. Results also proved that it is possible to enhance the illusion even with modality-mismatched stimulation, even though illusion was lower than in case of modality-matched stimulation.

Conclusions

We demonstrated the possibility of promoting a Rubber Foot Illusion with both matched and mismatched stimulation.


Url:
DOI: 10.1186/s12984-015-0069-6
PubMed: 26341285
PubMed Central: 4559902

Links toward previous steps (curation, corpus...)


Links to Exploration step

PMC:4559902

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI>
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">The rubber foot illusion</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Crea, Simona" sort="Crea, Simona" uniqKey="Crea S" first="Simona" last="Crea">Simona Crea</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff1">The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI), Italy</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Italie</country>
<wicri:regionArea>The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI)</wicri:regionArea>
<wicri:noRegion>Pontedera (PI)</wicri:noRegion>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="D Lonzo, Marco" sort="D Lonzo, Marco" uniqKey="D Lonzo M" first="Marco" last="D Lonzo">Marco D Lonzo</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff1">The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI), Italy</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Italie</country>
<wicri:regionArea>The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI)</wicri:regionArea>
<wicri:noRegion>Pontedera (PI)</wicri:noRegion>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vitiello, Nicola" sort="Vitiello, Nicola" uniqKey="Vitiello N" first="Nicola" last="Vitiello">Nicola Vitiello</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff1">The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI), Italy</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Italie</country>
<wicri:regionArea>The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI)</wicri:regionArea>
<wicri:noRegion>Pontedera (PI)</wicri:noRegion>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff2">Don Carlo Gnocchi Foundation, via di Scandicci, Firenze (FI), 269 Italy</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Italie</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Don Carlo Gnocchi Foundation, via di Scandicci, Firenze (FI)</wicri:regionArea>
<wicri:noRegion>Firenze (FI)</wicri:noRegion>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cipriani, Christian" sort="Cipriani, Christian" uniqKey="Cipriani C" first="Christian" last="Cipriani">Christian Cipriani</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff1">The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI), Italy</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Italie</country>
<wicri:regionArea>The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI)</wicri:regionArea>
<wicri:noRegion>Pontedera (PI)</wicri:noRegion>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">PMC</idno>
<idno type="pmid">26341285</idno>
<idno type="pmc">4559902</idno>
<idno type="url">http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4559902</idno>
<idno type="RBID">PMC:4559902</idno>
<idno type="doi">10.1186/s12984-015-0069-6</idno>
<date when="2015">2015</date>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Corpus">000848</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Curation">000848</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Checkpoint">000250</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Ncbi/Merge">003C08</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title xml:lang="en" level="a" type="main">The rubber foot illusion</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Crea, Simona" sort="Crea, Simona" uniqKey="Crea S" first="Simona" last="Crea">Simona Crea</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff1">The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI), Italy</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Italie</country>
<wicri:regionArea>The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI)</wicri:regionArea>
<wicri:noRegion>Pontedera (PI)</wicri:noRegion>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="D Lonzo, Marco" sort="D Lonzo, Marco" uniqKey="D Lonzo M" first="Marco" last="D Lonzo">Marco D Lonzo</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff1">The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI), Italy</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Italie</country>
<wicri:regionArea>The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI)</wicri:regionArea>
<wicri:noRegion>Pontedera (PI)</wicri:noRegion>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vitiello, Nicola" sort="Vitiello, Nicola" uniqKey="Vitiello N" first="Nicola" last="Vitiello">Nicola Vitiello</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff1">The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI), Italy</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Italie</country>
<wicri:regionArea>The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI)</wicri:regionArea>
<wicri:noRegion>Pontedera (PI)</wicri:noRegion>
</affiliation>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff2">Don Carlo Gnocchi Foundation, via di Scandicci, Firenze (FI), 269 Italy</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Italie</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Don Carlo Gnocchi Foundation, via di Scandicci, Firenze (FI)</wicri:regionArea>
<wicri:noRegion>Firenze (FI)</wicri:noRegion>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cipriani, Christian" sort="Cipriani, Christian" uniqKey="Cipriani C" first="Christian" last="Cipriani">Christian Cipriani</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:aff id="Aff1">The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI), Italy</nlm:aff>
<country xml:lang="fr">Italie</country>
<wicri:regionArea>The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI)</wicri:regionArea>
<wicri:noRegion>Pontedera (PI)</wicri:noRegion>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<series>
<title level="j">Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation</title>
<idno type="eISSN">1743-0003</idno>
<imprint>
<date when="2015">2015</date>
</imprint>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">
<sec>
<title>Background</title>
<p>Lower-limb amputation causes the individual a huge functional impairment due to the lack of adequate sensory perception from the missing limb. The development of an augmenting sensory feedback device able to restore some of the missing information from the amputated limb may improve embodiment, control and acceptability of the prosthesis.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Findings</title>
<p>In this work we transferred the Rubber Hand Illusion paradigm to the lower limb. We investigated the possibility of promoting body ownership of a fake foot, in a series of experiments fashioned after the RHI using matched or mismatched (vibrotactile) stimulation. The results, collected from 19 healthy subjects, demonstrated that it is possible to elicit the perception of possessing a rubber foot when modality-matched stimulations are provided synchronously on the biological foot and to the corresponding rubber foot areas. Results also proved that it is possible to enhance the illusion even with modality-mismatched stimulation, even though illusion was lower than in case of modality-matched stimulation.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Conclusions</title>
<p>We demonstrated the possibility of promoting a Rubber Foot Illusion with both matched and mismatched stimulation.</p>
</sec>
</div>
</front>
<back>
<div1 type="bibliography">
<listBibl>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Botvinick, M" uniqKey="Botvinick M">M Botvinick</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cohen, J" uniqKey="Cohen J">J Cohen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ehrsson, Hh" uniqKey="Ehrsson H">HH Ehrsson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Holmes, Np" uniqKey="Holmes N">NP Holmes</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Passingham, Re" uniqKey="Passingham R">RE Passingham</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shimada, S" uniqKey="Shimada S">S Shimada</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fukuda, K" uniqKey="Fukuda K">K Fukuda</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hiraki, K" uniqKey="Hiraki K">K Hiraki</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Michel, C" uniqKey="Michel C">C Michel</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Velasco, C" uniqKey="Velasco C">C Velasco</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Salgado Montejo, A" uniqKey="Salgado Montejo A">A Salgado-Montejo</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Spence, C" uniqKey="Spence C">C Spence</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct></biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Der Hoort, B" uniqKey="Van Der Hoort B">B Van Der Hoort</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Guterstam, A" uniqKey="Guterstam A">A Guterstam</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ehrsson, Hh" uniqKey="Ehrsson H">HH Ehrsson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ehrsson, Hh" uniqKey="Ehrsson H">HH Ehrsson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rosen, B" uniqKey="Rosen B">B Rosén</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stockselius, A" uniqKey="Stockselius A">A Stockselius</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ragno, C" uniqKey="Ragno C">C Ragnö</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kohler, P" uniqKey="Kohler P">P Köhler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lundborg, G" uniqKey="Lundborg G">G Lundborg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Marasco, Pd" uniqKey="Marasco P">PD Marasco</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kim, K" uniqKey="Kim K">K Kim</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Colgate, Je" uniqKey="Colgate J">JE Colgate</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Peshkin, Ma" uniqKey="Peshkin M">MA Peshkin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kuiken, Ta" uniqKey="Kuiken T">TA Kuiken</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="D Lonzo, M" uniqKey="D Lonzo M">M D’Alonzo</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cipriani, C" uniqKey="Cipriani C">C Cipriani</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="D Lonzo, M" uniqKey="D Lonzo M">M D’Alonzo</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Clemente, F" uniqKey="Clemente F">F Clemente</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cipriani, C" uniqKey="Cipriani C">C Cipriani</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Abernethy, B" uniqKey="Abernethy B">B Abernethy</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hanna, A" uniqKey="Hanna A">A Hanna</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Plooy, A" uniqKey="Plooy A">A Plooy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Murray, Cd" uniqKey="Murray C">CD Murray</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lenggenhager, B" uniqKey="Lenggenhager B">B Lenggenhager</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hilti, L" uniqKey="Hilti L">L Hilti</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brugger, P" uniqKey="Brugger P">P Brugger</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cipriani, C" uniqKey="Cipriani C">C Cipriani</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="D Lonzo, M" uniqKey="D Lonzo M">M D’Alonzo</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Carrozza, Mc" uniqKey="Carrozza M">MC Carrozza</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tsakiris, M" uniqKey="Tsakiris M">M Tsakiris</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Carpenter, L" uniqKey="Carpenter L">L Carpenter</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="James, D" uniqKey="James D">D James</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fotopoulou, A" uniqKey="Fotopoulou A">A Fotopoulou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct></biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tsakiris, M" uniqKey="Tsakiris M">M Tsakiris</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Haggard, P" uniqKey="Haggard P">P Haggard</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Vignemont, F" uniqKey="De Vignemont F">F De Vignemont</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ionta, S" uniqKey="Ionta S">S Ionta</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fourkas, Ad" uniqKey="Fourkas A">AD Fourkas</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fiorio, M" uniqKey="Fiorio M">M Fiorio</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aglioti, Sm" uniqKey="Aglioti S">SM Aglioti</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Harrar, V" uniqKey="Harrar V">V Harrar</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Harris, Lr" uniqKey="Harris L">LR Harris</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ehrsson, Hh" uniqKey="Ehrsson H">HH Ehrsson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Spence, C" uniqKey="Spence C">C Spence</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Passingham, Re" uniqKey="Passingham R">RE Passingham</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Crea, S" uniqKey="Crea S">S Crea</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vitiello, N" uniqKey="Vitiello N">N Vitiello</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Rossi, Smm" uniqKey="De Rossi S">SMM De Rossi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lenzi, T" uniqKey="Lenzi T">T Lenzi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Donati, M" uniqKey="Donati M">M Donati</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cipriani, C" uniqKey="Cipriani C">C Cipriani</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct></biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kuiken, Ta" uniqKey="Kuiken T">TA Kuiken</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Marasco, Pd" uniqKey="Marasco P">PD Marasco</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lock, Ba" uniqKey="Lock B">BA Lock</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Harden, Rn" uniqKey="Harden R">RN Harden</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dewald, Jp" uniqKey="Dewald J">JP Dewald</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
</listBibl>
</div1>
</back>
</TEI>
<pmc article-type="research-article">
<pmc-dir>properties open_access</pmc-dir>
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="nlm-ta">J Neuroeng Rehabil</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="iso-abbrev">J Neuroeng Rehabil</journal-id>
<journal-title-group>
<journal-title>Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation</journal-title>
</journal-title-group>
<issn pub-type="epub">1743-0003</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>BioMed Central</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmid">26341285</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmc">4559902</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="publisher-id">69</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1186/s12984-015-0069-6</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Short Report</subject>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>The rubber foot illusion</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author" corresp="yes" equal-contrib="yes">
<name>
<surname>Crea</surname>
<given-names>Simona</given-names>
</name>
<address>
<email>s.crea@sssup.it</email>
</address>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="Aff1"></xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author" equal-contrib="yes">
<name>
<surname>D’Alonzo</surname>
<given-names>Marco</given-names>
</name>
<address>
<email>m.dalonzo@sssup.it</email>
</address>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="Aff1"></xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Vitiello</surname>
<given-names>Nicola</given-names>
</name>
<address>
<email>n.vitiello@sssup.it</email>
</address>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="Aff1"></xref>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="Aff2"></xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cipriani</surname>
<given-names>Christian</given-names>
</name>
<address>
<email>ch.cipriani@sssup.it</email>
</address>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="Aff1"></xref>
</contrib>
<aff id="Aff1">
<label></label>
The BioRobotics Institute, Scuola Superiore Sant’Anna, viale Rinaldo Piaggio, Pontedera (PI), Italy</aff>
<aff id="Aff2">
<label></label>
Don Carlo Gnocchi Foundation, via di Scandicci, Firenze (FI), 269 Italy</aff>
</contrib-group>
<pub-date pub-type="epub">
<day>4</day>
<month>9</month>
<year>2015</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="pmc-release">
<day>4</day>
<month>9</month>
<year>2015</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="collection">
<year>2015</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>12</volume>
<elocation-id>77</elocation-id>
<history>
<date date-type="received">
<day>20</day>
<month>2</month>
<year>2015</year>
</date>
<date date-type="accepted">
<day>28</day>
<month>8</month>
<year>2015</year>
</date>
</history>
<permissions>
<copyright-statement>© Crea et al. 2015</copyright-statement>
<license license-type="OpenAccess">
<license-p>
<bold>Open Access</bold>
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License(
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/">http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/</ext-link>
), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver(
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/">http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/</ext-link>
) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.</license-p>
</license>
</permissions>
<abstract id="Abs1">
<sec>
<title>Background</title>
<p>Lower-limb amputation causes the individual a huge functional impairment due to the lack of adequate sensory perception from the missing limb. The development of an augmenting sensory feedback device able to restore some of the missing information from the amputated limb may improve embodiment, control and acceptability of the prosthesis.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Findings</title>
<p>In this work we transferred the Rubber Hand Illusion paradigm to the lower limb. We investigated the possibility of promoting body ownership of a fake foot, in a series of experiments fashioned after the RHI using matched or mismatched (vibrotactile) stimulation. The results, collected from 19 healthy subjects, demonstrated that it is possible to elicit the perception of possessing a rubber foot when modality-matched stimulations are provided synchronously on the biological foot and to the corresponding rubber foot areas. Results also proved that it is possible to enhance the illusion even with modality-mismatched stimulation, even though illusion was lower than in case of modality-matched stimulation.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Conclusions</title>
<p>We demonstrated the possibility of promoting a Rubber Foot Illusion with both matched and mismatched stimulation.</p>
</sec>
</abstract>
<kwd-group xml:lang="en">
<title>Keywords</title>
<kwd>Rubber foot illusion</kwd>
<kwd>Augmenting sensory feedback</kwd>
<kwd>Lower-limb prostheses</kwd>
<kwd>Amputees</kwd>
</kwd-group>
<custom-meta-group>
<custom-meta>
<meta-name>issue-copyright-statement</meta-name>
<meta-value>© The Author(s) 2015</meta-value>
</custom-meta>
</custom-meta-group>
</article-meta>
</front>
<body>
<sec id="Sec1" sec-type="introduction">
<title>Introduction</title>
<p>The Rubber Hand Illusion (RHI) is a perceptual illusion causing the feeling of ownership of a realistic rubber hand when placed in full view and synchronously stimulated with the person’s own hand, which is hidden from view [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR1">1</xref>
]. The illusion happens as a result of the interaction and coherence of vision, touch and proprioception [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR2">2</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR3">3</xref>
] and it typically induces a shift of the perceived location of the participant’s hand towards the rubber hand (proprioceptive drift) and a strong skin conductance response (SCR) to a threat stimulus on the rubber hand. This illusion does not occur when the stimulations are not synchronous [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR1">1</xref>
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR3">3</xref>
].</p>
<p>In recent years the paradigm of the RHI was investigated on different body parts, e.g. the tongue [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR4">4</xref>
], the arm [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR5">5</xref>
] or the entire body [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR6">6</xref>
] and translated from basic neuroscience studies to different application scenarios, like virtual reality [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR6">6</xref>
] or upper-limb prostheses [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR7">7</xref>
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR10">10</xref>
]. Indeed Ehrsson and colleagues demonstrated that the RHI can be elicited even when the natural hand is missing, namely in upper-limb amputees when stimulated on
<italic>referred phantom fingers</italic>
on the stump [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR7">7</xref>
]. This finding suggested engineers to develop hand prostheses with tactile sensory feedback capable of promoting the embodiment of the prosthesis itself [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR7">7</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR8">8</xref>
]. In our previous studies we suggested and proofed the possibility of inducing the RHI using mismatched stimulation (i.e. sensory substitution) on healthy subjects [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR9">9</xref>
] and transradial amputees [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR10">10</xref>
]. In particular we used miniaturized vibrotactile stimulators, that are small enough to be embedded in a prosthetic socket, conversely to current state-of-the-art haptic stimulators, too bulky [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR9">9</xref>
].</p>
<p>Lower-limb amputation, similarly to upper-limb loss, causes the individual a huge functional impairment due to the lack of adequate sensory perception from the missing limb. While ambulation is generally a well-learned activity, which requires little or no cognitive effort by healthy adults [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR11">11</xref>
], ambulating with an above-knee prosthesis is a cognitively demanding task, because the normal proprioceptive afferent flow is lost, and amputees find it difficult to control their prostheses, especially in unstructured environment, i.e. uneven terrain. The use of vision combined to a sensory feedback stimulation provided in a natural and intuitive fashion, (e.g. by exploiting the phantom limb sensation, as suggested by Murray [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR12">12</xref>
] and Ehrsson and collegues [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR7">7</xref>
]) could improve the controllability of the prosthesis, as tactile information from the prosthesis would match and enrich the information from the vision. At the same time, an intuitive proprioceptive feedback could also be an important asset in itself; in fact if the prosthesis could induce a feeling of ownership it is predictable that the ratio of unsatisfied patients will reduce [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR12">12</xref>
].</p>
<p>In this work we transferred the RHI paradigm to the lower limb. The first goal of the study, similarly to the work recently published by Lenggenhager and collegues [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR13">13</xref>
], was to investigate the possibility of promoting body ownership of a fake foot, in series of experiment fashioned after the RHI, using matched stimulation. The second goal of the study was to investigate the possibility to enhance the feeling of possessing a rubber foot when applying mismatched (vibrotactile) stimulation, which can be potentially integrated in a sensory feedback system for lower-limb prosthesis. The results, collected from 19 healthy subjects, demonstrated indeed the possibility of promoting a
<italic>Rubber Foot Illusion</italic>
(RFI) with both matched and mismatched stimulation.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec2" sec-type="materials|methods">
<title>Materials and methods</title>
<p>Nineteen able-bodied volunteers (10 females) participated in this study after signing a written informed consent. All experimental procedures were performed according to the standards set by the declaration of Helsinki for medical research involving human subjects. Each participant sat comfortably on a bed with both legs lying in a supine position and with his/her right leg out of sight, behind a screen. The participant was instructed to fix his/her sight on a rubber foot (single-axis foot, Ottobock Gmbh) during the experiment. Modality-matched and modality-mismatched stimulation conditions were tested. During the modality-matched conditions, congruent stimulation was delivered on the real and the rubber feet, using two paintbrushes, as in the original RHI experiment. In the modality-mismatched conditions the stimulation was incongruent; the rubber foot, in full view, was manually stimulated with the paintbrush while the hidden real foot was stimulated by small vibrators taped on the first and second toes, triggered off by a keypad, as in our previous studies [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR9">9</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR10">10</xref>
] (as depicted in (Fig.
<xref rid="Fig1" ref-type="fig">1</xref>
)). Each vibrator (8 mm diameter, 3.4 mm height, 0.7 g weight) could be selectively activated to vibrate at a pre-defined vibration frequency (165 Hz) and force amplitude (0.36 N; i.e. largely supra-threshold) or deactivated. As soon as a specific key on the keypad was pressed, the corresponding unit would start vibrating, until the key was released. The time delay between the pressure of a key and starting of perceivable vibration can be considered negligible (i.e. <10 ms). Paintbrush stimulations on the rubber foot were applied on the areas corresponding to where the vibrators were placed on the real foot, i.e. on the first and second toes. Details on this set-up can be found in [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR14">14</xref>
]. Both the congruent and incongruent conditions were tested with either synchronous or asynchronous stimulation (a temporal delay of about 0.5 s was included between stimulations) for a total of four combinations. The asynchronous conditions were included as control conditions for the synchronous ones. The four combinations of modality and timing (congruent synchronous - CS, congruent asynchronous - CA, incongruent synchronous - IS and incongruent asynchronous - IA) were tested twice for each subject; 50 % of the trials (balanced across conditions) ended with a threat stimulus, i.e. a needle was used to stab the rubber foot, in order to measure the SCR (as in the conventional RHI experiment [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR7">7</xref>
]). Each trial lasted 150 s; in previous studies the stimulation duration was ranged from a minimum of 45 s [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR9">9</xref>
] to a maximum of 240 s [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR15">15</xref>
]. The order of execution of the trials was randomized across the participants.
<fig id="Fig1">
<label>Fig. 1</label>
<caption>
<p>Experimental setup. The participant is looking at the rubber foot, placed in front of him while his real foot is hidden from view</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="12984_2015_69_Fig1_HTML" id="MO1"></graphic>
</fig>
</p>
<p>We quantified the embodiment of the rubber foot by means of the three state-of-art control measures, i.e. self-assessment questionnaires, proprioceptive drift and SCR [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR15">15</xref>
]. The questionnaire (Table
<xref rid="Tab1" ref-type="table">1</xref>
) comprised of nine statements adapted from [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR1">1</xref>
] for application on the foot instead of on the hand and translated into Italian. Three of the statements, namely illusion statements, referred to the extent of sensory transfer into the rubber foot and self-attribution of it during the trial (S1–S3). The remaining six statements, namely control statements, served as controls, for assessing suggestibility and “placebo effect”. Moreover, in addition to the nine statements, subjects were asked to rate vividness and prevalence of self-attribution of the rubber foot. The vividness was defined as how life-like and realistic the illusion was when it was experienced; it was rated from 0 to 10. The prevalence rating (from 0–100 %) reflected the percentage of time that the illusion was experienced (equivalent to the continuance of the illusion). To assess the proprioceptive drift, the participant had to indicate the position of his/her big toe on a ruler placed over his/her foot, before and immediately after the stimulation. The subtraction of the two measurements resulted in the proprioceptive drift. In order to avoid any memory effect the ruler was positioned with different offsets all the times [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR15">15</xref>
]. When the trial ended with a threat stimulus (i.e. a needle attached to a syringe, which was used to stab the rubber foot), we measured the SCR first, then the proprioceptive drift; in the other trials first we measured the proprioceptive drift then we asked to fill-in the questionnaire.
<table-wrap id="Tab1">
<label>Table 1</label>
<caption>
<p>Questionnaire. Statements from 1 to 3 are illusion statements. Statements from 4 to 9 are suggestion statements. Statements 10 and 11 are respectively used to quantify the vividness and prevalence of the illusion</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th align="left"></th>
<th align="left">Question</th>
<th align="left">Score</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="left">S1</td>
<td align="justify">It seemed like I was feeling the stimulation in the point where the rubber foot was being touched</td>
<td align="left">[ −3+3]</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">S2</td>
<td align="justify">It seemed like the stimulation I was feeling was caused by the touch of the paintbrush on the rubber foot</td>
<td align="left">[ −3+3]</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">S3</td>
<td align="justify">I felt like the rubber foot was mine</td>
<td align="left">[ −3+3]</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">S4</td>
<td align="justify">I felt like my (real) foot was moving towards left (towards the rubber foot)</td>
<td align="left">[ −3+3]</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">S5</td>
<td align="justify">It seemed like I had more than one right foot and leg</td>
<td align="left">[ −3+3]</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">S6</td>
<td align="justify">It seemed like the stimulation I was feeling became from some places between my foot and the rubber foot</td>
<td align="left">[ −3+3]</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">S7</td>
<td align="justify">I felt like my (real) foot like was becoming rubbery</td>
<td align="left">[ −3+3]</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">S8</td>
<td align="justify">It seemed like the rubber foot was moving towards right (towards my real foot)</td>
<td align="left">[ −3+3]</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">S9</td>
<td align="justify">The rubber foot started to look like my (real) foot, in terms of shape, skin tone, o other characteristics</td>
<td align="left">[ −3+3]</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">S10</td>
<td align="justify">Using a score from 0 to 10, quantify the vividness (how much your illusion of a rubber foot was realistic)</td>
<td align="left">[0 10]</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">S11</td>
<td align="justify">Using a score from 0 to 100 %, quantify how long you had the illusion that the rubber foot was yours</td>
<td align="left">[0 100]</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</table-wrap>
</p>
<p>The Kolgomorov-Smirnov test was used to verify whether the measurements were normally distributed. Then, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA (factors: timing and modality) was performed singularly for each measurement (i.e. questionnaires, drifts or SCR) in order to assess significant differences in the stimulation conditions. Similarly the results were pairwise compared using two-tailed paired t-test [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR15">15</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR16">16</xref>
], with Tukey-Kramer correction. Statistical significance was set to 0.05.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec3" sec-type="results">
<title>Results</title>
<p>Results of the self-assessment questionnaire substantially agree with previous studies on the hand, with illusion statements rated significantly higher than control statements (aimed to assess suggestibility of the subjects) (Fig.
<xref rid="Fig2" ref-type="fig">2</xref>
<xref rid="Fig2" ref-type="fig">(a)</xref>
). Statistical differences between the average scores on illusion and suggestion statements (
<italic>p</italic>
<0.05) were found in all the four experimental conditions (t test, CS,
<italic>p</italic>
<.0001; CA,
<italic>p</italic>
=.0003; IS,
<italic>p</italic>
<.0001; IA,
<italic>p</italic>
=.0194). Moreover, a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA on average values of illusion statements revealed a significant main effect of timing (
<italic>F</italic>
(1,18)=76.74,
<italic>p</italic>
<.0001) and modality (
<italic>F</italic>
(1,18)=19.81,
<italic>p</italic>
=.0003). No statistically significant interaction between the effects of timing and modality was found (
<italic>F</italic>
(1,18)=2.39,
<italic>p</italic>
=.1395). From the post-hoc multiple comparison with Tukey-Kramer adjustment, both matched and mismatched modalities revealed higher illusion in the synchronous conditions than in asynchronous ones (CS vs CA
<italic>p</italic>
<.0001, IS vs IA
<italic>p</italic>
=.0001). Also, in the illusion statements, the modality (congruent vs incongruent) revealed a statistical difference between synchronous conditions, with higher illusion in the congruent than in the incongruent conditions (CS vs IS
<italic>p</italic>
=.0005; CA vs IA
<italic>p</italic>
=.0548).
<fig id="Fig2">
<label>Fig. 2</label>
<caption>
<p>Average ratings of the self-assessment questionnaire.
<bold>a</bold>
9 statements. Illusion statements are from 1 to 3. Suggestion statements are from 4 to 9.
<bold>b</bold>
Vividness of the illusion.
<bold>c</bold>
Prevalence of the illusion. CS = Congruent Synchronous, CA = Congruent Asynchronous, IS = Incongruent Synchronous, IA = Incongruent Asynchronous. * indicates
<italic>p</italic>
<.05, *** indicates
<italic>p</italic>
<.001</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="12984_2015_69_Fig2_HTML" id="MO2"></graphic>
</fig>
</p>
<p>Results from the two-way repeated measures ANOVA showed a statistically significant interaction effect between the timing and modality on the vividness of the illusion (
<italic>F</italic>
(1,17)=7.37,
<italic>p</italic>
=.0147) (Fig.
<xref rid="Fig2" ref-type="fig">2</xref>
<xref rid="Fig2" ref-type="fig">(b)</xref>
). Significant main effects of timing (
<italic>F</italic>
(1,17)=70.62,
<italic>p</italic>
<.0001) and modality were found (
<italic>F</italic>
(1,17)=25.42,
<italic>p</italic>
=.0001). Post-hoc pairwise analysis revealed that, in both congruent and incongruent conditions, vividness scored significantly higher when stimulations were synchronous than in case they were asynchronous (t-test with Tukey-Kramer adjustment, CS vs CA
<italic>p</italic>
<.0001, IS vs IA
<italic>p</italic>
=.0009). Significantly higher vividness was also found in the CS compared to the IS (t-test with Tukey-Kramer adjustment,
<italic>p</italic>
<.0001) while no significant difference was found between CA and IA cases (t-test,
<italic>p</italic>
=.1182).</p>
<p>Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA applied on the prevalence scores (Fig.
<xref rid="Fig2" ref-type="fig">2</xref>
<xref rid="Fig2" ref-type="fig">(c)</xref>
) revealed again a significant interaction effect between timing and modality factors (
<italic>F</italic>
(1,17)=11.02,
<italic>p</italic>
=.0041), and two main effects for the timing (
<italic>F</italic>
(1,17)=91.68,
<italic>p</italic>
<.0001) and the modality (
<italic>F</italic>
(1,17)=22.31,
<italic>p</italic>
=.0002). Post-hoc analysis found that prevalence scored higher in synchronous conditions than in asynchronous ones (CS vs CA
<italic>p</italic>
<.0001; IS vs IA
<italic>p</italic>
=.0004). The effect of modality was also significant for prevalence scores (
<italic>F</italic>
(1,17)=22.31,
<italic>p</italic>
=.0002), and pairwise comparison resulted in higher prevalence in synchronous congruent condition than in synchronous incongruent one (CS vs IS
<italic>p</italic>
<.0001; CA vs IA
<italic>p</italic>
=.3346).
<sup>1</sup>
</p>
<p>Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA showed no significant interaction effect between timing and modality factors (
<italic>F</italic>
(1,18)=.33,
<italic>p</italic>
=.5735) and a significant main effect of timing in the proprioceptive drift results (
<italic>F</italic>
(1,18)=12.45,
<italic>p</italic>
=.0024) (Fig.
<xref rid="Fig3" ref-type="fig">3</xref>
<xref rid="Fig3" ref-type="fig">(a)</xref>
). Proprioceptive drift in congruent synchronous trial was higher than in the corresponding asynchronous trial (t-test, CS vs CA
<italic>p</italic>
=.0095), while no difference was found for incongruent conditions (IS vs IA
<italic>p</italic>
=.0511). In this case, the effect of stimulation modality was not significant and pairwise comparison was not performed.
<fig id="Fig3">
<label>Fig. 3</label>
<caption>
<p>Results of the objective measures of the embodiment. Mean values ± Standard Error of Mean across subjects.
<bold>a</bold>
Proprioceptive drift.
<bold>b</bold>
Skin conductance response (SCR). Synch. = Average of synchronous congruent and incongruent results. Asynch. = Average of asynchronous congruent and incongruent results. * indicates
<italic>p</italic>
<.05, ** indicates
<italic>p</italic>
<.01</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="12984_2015_69_Fig3_HTML" id="MO3"></graphic>
</fig>
</p>
<p>Regarding the SCR measurements, the two-way ANOVA showed a significant main effect for the timing of the stimulations (
<italic>F</italic>
(1,18)=6.75,
<italic>p</italic>
=.0181), no significant main effect of the stimulation modality (
<italic>F</italic>
(1,18)=4.02,
<italic>p</italic>
=.0603) and no significant interaction effect (
<italic>F</italic>
(1,18)=3.5,
<italic>p</italic>
=.0777) (Fig.
<xref rid="Fig3" ref-type="fig">3</xref>
<xref rid="Fig3" ref-type="fig">(b)</xref>
). Pairwise comparison of different timing conditions revealed higher SCR in CS condition than in CA (
<italic>p</italic>
=.0054), while no difference was found between IS and IA (
<italic>p</italic>
=.6132). Since modality did not result in a main effect, pairwise comparison was not assessed.</p>
<p>It is worth to recall that combined measurements of embodiment, i.e. questionnaire, proprioceptive drift and the SCR, scored in line with previous studies on the RHI [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR2">2</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR9">9</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR17">17</xref>
].</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec4">
<title>Discussion and conclusions</title>
<p>The body schema is usually defined as a continuously updated sensorimotor map of the body that informs the brain about what parts belongs to the body and where those parts are located [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR18">18</xref>
]. The trick of the RHI is a direct consequence of multisensory integration, which basically misleads the brain as to the status of ownership of a fake hand, provoking its inclusion in the body schema. The largest part of neurophysiological studies has explored the body schema using stimulation to and near the hands. Hands may be considered special in that they are used for almost any kind of action we perform, but it seems that processing principles revealed for the hands may not generalize to other body parts [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR19">19</xref>
]. Indeed, combining multisensory information has different implications in case the stimuli come from different parts of the body’s surface. For example tactile information takes different amounts of time to be processed by the brain depending on the distance between the stimulated surface and the brain and some studies showed how the integration of visual and tactile stimulation of the foot can lead to bad perception of simultaneity of the stimuli [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR20">20</xref>
]. Our results provide evidence that it is possible to elicit the perception of possessing a rubber foot when modality-matched stimulations are provided synchronously on the biological foot and to the equivalent rubber foot areas. Indeed, all the three independent measurements of embodiment, classically employed in similar experiments on the RHI [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR9">9</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR15">15</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR21">21</xref>
], gave closely matched results supporting this main outcome, thus confirming that even though visual and tactile stimulations applied on the foot undergo different processing mechanisms, the multisensory integration can still effectively compound the illusion and the brain integrates the fake foot in the body schema. Second to that, we investigated the RFI approach for application in the development of sensory feedback systems for lower-limb prostheses in order to provide the individual with information from the foot sole [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR22">22</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR23">23</xref>
]. Results proved that it is possible to enhance the illusion even with modality-mismatched stimulations, even though illusion was lower than in case of modality-matched stimulation. Notably asynchronous stimulation, which is used as a standard control test, in both modality congruent and incongruent cases did not induce the illusion, as reported also in previous studies [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR1">1</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR3">3</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR7">7</xref>
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR9">9</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR13">13</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR15">15</xref>
]. These results open important perspectives in the field of lower-limb prosthetics; indeed, vibrotactile elements can be easily integrated into the socket of lower-limb prostheses in order to stimulate the referred phantom foot areas and induce the RFI on a daily basis. This feature could improve the controllability of the prosthesis [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR24">24</xref>
] and enhance the satisfaction of the amputee in using the prosthesis even more [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR12">12</xref>
].</p>
<p>Future studies will be devoted at investigating whether the proposed mismatched-stimulation paradigm can promote the embodiment of the prosthesis in the body schema of lower-limb amputees.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec5">
<title>Endnote</title>
<p>
<sup>1</sup>
Eighteen of nineteen participants replied to vividness and prevalence questions.</p>
</sec>
</body>
<back>
<fn-group>
<fn>
<p>
<bold>Competing interests</bold>
</p>
<p>The authors declare that they have no competing interests.</p>
</fn>
<fn>
<p>
<bold>Authors’ contributions</bold>
</p>
<p>Study concept and design: SC, MD, NV, CC. Acquisition of data: SC, MD. Statistical analysis and interpretation of data: SC, MD, CC. Drafting of manuscript: SC, MD, CC. Study supervision: NV, CC. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.</p>
</fn>
</fn-group>
<ack>
<title>Acknowledgements</title>
<p>The authors would like to thank Eng. M. Moisé for helping with artwork in (Fig.
<xref rid="Fig1" ref-type="fig">1</xref>
). This work was partly supported by EU within the CYBERLEGs Project (Grant num. 287894) and the WAY Project (Grant num. 288551), and by Fondazione Pisa within the IUVO Project (prog. 154/11).</p>
</ack>
<ref-list id="Bib1">
<title>References</title>
<ref id="CR1">
<label>1</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Botvinick</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cohen</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Rubber hands’ feel’touch that eyes see</article-title>
<source>Nature</source>
<year>1998</year>
<volume>391</volume>
<issue>6669</issue>
<fpage>756</fpage>
<lpage>6</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1038/35784</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">9486643</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR2">
<label>2</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ehrsson</surname>
<given-names>HH</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Holmes</surname>
<given-names>NP</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Passingham</surname>
<given-names>RE</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Touching a rubber hand: feeling of body ownership is associated with activity in multisensory brain areas</article-title>
<source>J Neurosci</source>
<year>2005</year>
<volume>25</volume>
<issue>45</issue>
<fpage>10564</fpage>
<lpage>10573</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0800-05.2005</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16280594</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR3">
<label>3</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Shimada</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fukuda</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hiraki</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Rubber hand illusion under delayed visual feedback</article-title>
<source>PLoS ONE</source>
<year>2009</year>
<volume>4</volume>
<issue>7</issue>
<fpage>6185</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0006185</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR4">
<label>4</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Michel</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Velasco</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Salgado-Montejo</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Spence</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The butcher’s tongue illusion</article-title>
<source>Perception</source>
<year>2014</year>
<volume>43</volume>
<fpage>818</fpage>
<lpage>24</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1068/p7733</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">25549512</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR5">
<label>5</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="other">Slater M, Perez-Marcos D, Ehrsson HH, Sanchez-Vives MV. Towards a digital body: the virtual arm illusion. Front Hum Neurosci. 2008;2:6. doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/neuro.09.006.2008">10.3389/neuro.09.006.2008</ext-link>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR6">
<label>6</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Van Der Hoort</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Guterstam</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ehrsson</surname>
<given-names>HH</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Being barbie: the size of one’s own body determines the perceived size of the world</article-title>
<source>PLoS ONE</source>
<year>2011</year>
<volume>6</volume>
<issue>5</issue>
<fpage>20195</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0020195</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR7">
<label>7</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ehrsson</surname>
<given-names>HH</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rosén</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Stockselius</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ragnö</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Köhler</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lundborg</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Upper-limb amputees can be induced to experience a rubber hand as their own</article-title>
<source>Brain</source>
<year>2008</year>
<volume>131</volume>
<issue>12</issue>
<fpage>3443</fpage>
<lpage>452</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1093/brain/awn297</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19074189</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR8">
<label>8</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Marasco</surname>
<given-names>PD</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kim</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Colgate</surname>
<given-names>JE</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Peshkin</surname>
<given-names>MA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kuiken</surname>
<given-names>TA</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Robotic touch shifts perception of embodiment to prosthesis on targeted reinnervation amputees</article-title>
<source>Brain</source>
<year>2011</year>
<volume>134</volume>
<fpage>747</fpage>
<lpage>58</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1093/brain/awq361</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21252109</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR9">
<label>9</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>D’Alonzo</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cipriani</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Vibrotactile sensory substitution elicits feeling of ownership of an alien hand</article-title>
<source>PLoS ONE</source>
<year>2012</year>
<volume>7</volume>
<issue>11</issue>
<fpage>50756</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0050756</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR10">
<label>10</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>D’Alonzo</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Clemente</surname>
<given-names>F</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cipriani</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Vibrotactile stimulation promotes embodiment of an alien hand in amputees with phantom sensations</article-title>
<source>Neural Syst Rehabil Eng IEEE Trans</source>
<year>2014</year>
<volume>23</volume>
<issue>3</issue>
<fpage>450</fpage>
<lpage>7</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1109/TNSRE.2014.2337952</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR11">
<label>11</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Abernethy</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hanna</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Plooy</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The attentional demands of preferred and non-preferred gait patterns</article-title>
<source>Gait and Posture</source>
<year>2002</year>
<volume>15</volume>
<fpage>256</fpage>
<lpage>65</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0966-6362(01)00195-3</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11983500</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR12">
<label>12</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Murray</surname>
<given-names>CD</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>Embodiment and prosthetics. Psychoprosthetics</source>
<year>2008</year>
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Springer</publisher-name>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR13">
<label>13</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lenggenhager</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hilti</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brugger</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Disturbed body integrity and the “rubber foot illusion”</article-title>
<source>Neuropsychology</source>
<year>2014</year>
<volume>29</volume>
<issue>2</issue>
<fpage>205</fpage>
<lpage>11</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/neu0000143</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">25265068</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR14">
<label>14</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cipriani</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>D’Alonzo</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Carrozza</surname>
<given-names>MC</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>A miniature vibrotactile sensory substitution device for multifingered hand prosthetics</article-title>
<source>Biomed Eng IEEE Trans</source>
<year>2012</year>
<volume>59</volume>
<issue>2</issue>
<fpage>400</fpage>
<lpage>8</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1109/TBME.2011.2173342</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR15">
<label>15</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tsakiris</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Carpenter</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>James</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fotopoulou</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Hands only illusion: multisensory integration elicits sense of ownership for body parts but not for non-corporeal objects</article-title>
<source>Exp Brain Res</source>
<year>2010</year>
<volume>204</volume>
<issue>3</issue>
<fpage>343</fpage>
<lpage>52</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s00221-009-2039-3</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19820918</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR16">
<label>16</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="other">Howell D. Statistical methods for psychology. Cengage Learning. 2012.
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="https://books.google.it/books?hl=it&lr=&id=CRYKAAAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PR5&dq=Statistical+methods+for+psychology&ots=au7JL5A_rU&sig=yTyZArv-ipiGqq4tJ46X9NRaacg#v=onepage&q=Statistical%20methods%20for%20psychology&f=false">https://books.google.it/books?hl=it&lr=&id=CRYKAAAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PR5&dq=Statistical+methods+for+psychology&ots=au7JL5A_rU&sig=yTyZArv-ipiGqq4tJ46X9NRaacg#v=onepage&q=Statistical%20methods%20for%20psychology&f=false</ext-link>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR17">
<label>17</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tsakiris</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Haggard</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The rubber hand illusion revisited: visuotactile integration and self-attribution</article-title>
<source>J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform</source>
<year>2005</year>
<volume>31</volume>
<issue>1</issue>
<fpage>80</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.80</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15709864</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR18">
<label>18</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>De Vignemont</surname>
<given-names>F</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Embodiment, ownership and disownership</article-title>
<source>Conscious Cogn</source>
<year>2011</year>
<volume>20</volume>
<issue>1</issue>
<fpage>82</fpage>
<lpage>93</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.004</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20943417</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR19">
<label>19</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ionta</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fourkas</surname>
<given-names>AD</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fiorio</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Aglioti</surname>
<given-names>SM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The influence of hands posture on mental rotation of hands and feet</article-title>
<source>Exp Brain Res</source>
<year>2007</year>
<volume>183</volume>
<issue>1</issue>
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>7</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s00221-007-1020-2</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17643238</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR20">
<label>20</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Harrar</surname>
<given-names>V</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Harris</surname>
<given-names>LR</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Simultaneity constancy: detecting events with touch and vision</article-title>
<source>Exp Brain Res</source>
<year>2005</year>
<volume>166</volume>
<issue>3-4</issue>
<fpage>465</fpage>
<lpage>73</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s00221-005-2386-7</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16028031</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR21">
<label>21</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ehrsson</surname>
<given-names>HH</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Spence</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Passingham</surname>
<given-names>RE</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>That’s my hand! activity in premotor cortex reflects feeling of ownership of a limb</article-title>
<source>Science</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>305</volume>
<issue>5685</issue>
<fpage>875</fpage>
<lpage>7</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1126/science.1097011</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15232072</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR22">
<label>22</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Crea</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Vitiello</surname>
<given-names>N</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>De Rossi</surname>
<given-names>SMM</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lenzi</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Donati</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cipriani</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
<article-title>Development of an experimental set-up for providing lower-limb amputees with an augmenting feedback</article-title>
<source>Converging Clin Eng Res Neurorehabil</source>
<year>2013</year>
<volume>1</volume>
<fpage>321</fpage>
<lpage>325</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/978-3-642-34546-3_51</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR23">
<label>23</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="other">Crea S, Cipriani C, Donati M, Carrozza MC, Vitiello N. Providing time-discrete gait information by wearable feedback apparatus for lower-limb amputees: usability and functional validation. Neural Syst Rehabil Eng, IEEE Trans. 2014:1–8. doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/TNSRE.2014.2365548">10.1109/TNSRE.2014.2365548</ext-link>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR24">
<label>24</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kuiken</surname>
<given-names>TA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Marasco</surname>
<given-names>PD</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lock</surname>
<given-names>BA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Harden</surname>
<given-names>RN</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dewald</surname>
<given-names>JP</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Redirection of cutaneous sensation from the hand to the chest skin of human amputees with targeted reinnervation</article-title>
<source>Proc. Nat Acad Sci USA</source>
<year>2007</year>
<volume>104</volume>
<issue>50</issue>
<fpage>20061</fpage>
<lpage>0066</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1073/pnas.0706525104</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18048339</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</pmc>
<affiliations>
<list>
<country>
<li>Italie</li>
</country>
</list>
<tree>
<country name="Italie">
<noRegion>
<name sortKey="Crea, Simona" sort="Crea, Simona" uniqKey="Crea S" first="Simona" last="Crea">Simona Crea</name>
</noRegion>
<name sortKey="Cipriani, Christian" sort="Cipriani, Christian" uniqKey="Cipriani C" first="Christian" last="Cipriani">Christian Cipriani</name>
<name sortKey="D Lonzo, Marco" sort="D Lonzo, Marco" uniqKey="D Lonzo M" first="Marco" last="D Lonzo">Marco D Lonzo</name>
<name sortKey="Vitiello, Nicola" sort="Vitiello, Nicola" uniqKey="Vitiello N" first="Nicola" last="Vitiello">Nicola Vitiello</name>
<name sortKey="Vitiello, Nicola" sort="Vitiello, Nicola" uniqKey="Vitiello N" first="Nicola" last="Vitiello">Nicola Vitiello</name>
</country>
</tree>
</affiliations>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Ticri/CIDE/explor/HapticV1/Data/Ncbi/Merge
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 003C08 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Ncbi/Merge/biblio.hfd -nk 003C08 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Ticri/CIDE
   |area=    HapticV1
   |flux=    Ncbi
   |étape=   Merge
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     PMC:4559902
   |texte=   The rubber foot illusion
}}

Pour générer des pages wiki

HfdIndexSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Ncbi/Merge/RBID.i   -Sk "pubmed:26341285" \
       | HfdSelect -Kh $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Ncbi/Merge/biblio.hfd   \
       | NlmPubMed2Wicri -a HapticV1 

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.23.
Data generation: Mon Jun 13 01:09:46 2016. Site generation: Wed Mar 6 09:54:07 2024