Serveur d'exploration sur les dispositifs haptiques

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

To have and to hold: looking vs. touching in the study of categorization

Identifieur interne : 003735 ( Ncbi/Merge ); précédent : 003734; suivant : 003736

To have and to hold: looking vs. touching in the study of categorization

Auteurs : Lynn K. Perry

Source :

RBID : PMC:4332276
Url:
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00178
PubMed: 25741314
PubMed Central: 4332276

Links toward previous steps (curation, corpus...)


Links to Exploration step

PMC:4332276

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI>
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">To have and to hold: looking vs. touching in the study of categorization</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Perry, Lynn K" sort="Perry, Lynn K" uniqKey="Perry L" first="Lynn K." last="Perry">Lynn K. Perry</name>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">PMC</idno>
<idno type="pmid">25741314</idno>
<idno type="pmc">4332276</idno>
<idno type="url">http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4332276</idno>
<idno type="RBID">PMC:4332276</idno>
<idno type="doi">10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00178</idno>
<date when="2015">2015</date>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Corpus">000D33</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Curation">000D33</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Checkpoint">000239</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Ncbi/Merge">003735</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title xml:lang="en" level="a" type="main">To have and to hold: looking vs. touching in the study of categorization</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Perry, Lynn K" sort="Perry, Lynn K" uniqKey="Perry L" first="Lynn K." last="Perry">Lynn K. Perry</name>
</author>
</analytic>
<series>
<title level="j">Frontiers in Psychology</title>
<idno type="eISSN">1664-1078</idno>
<imprint>
<date when="2015">2015</date>
</imprint>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<back>
<div1 type="bibliography">
<listBibl>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Adelson, E H" uniqKey="Adelson E">E. H. Adelson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bahrick, L E" uniqKey="Bahrick L">L. E. Bahrick</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lickliter, R" uniqKey="Lickliter R">R. Lickliter</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Flom, R" uniqKey="Flom R">R. Flom</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Behl Chadha, G" uniqKey="Behl Chadha G">G. Behl-Chadha</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Berthier, N E" uniqKey="Berthier N">N. E. Berthier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Deblois, S" uniqKey="Deblois S">S. DeBlois</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Poirier, C R" uniqKey="Poirier C">C. R. Poirier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Novak, M A" uniqKey="Novak M">M. A. Novak</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Clifton, R K" uniqKey="Clifton R">R. K. Clifton</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Best, C A" uniqKey="Best C">C. A. Best</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yim, H" uniqKey="Yim H">H. Yim</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sloutsky, V M" uniqKey="Sloutsky V">V. M. Sloutsky</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Haith, M M" uniqKey="Haith M">M. M. Haith</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hammer, R" uniqKey="Hammer R">R. Hammer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Diesendruck, G" uniqKey="Diesendruck G">G. Diesendruck</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnson, S P" uniqKey="Johnson S">S. P. Johnson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Slemmer, J A" uniqKey="Slemmer J">J. A. Slemmer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Amso, D" uniqKey="Amso D">D. Amso</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Keen, R" uniqKey="Keen R">R. Keen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kemler Nelson, D G" uniqKey="Kemler Nelson D">D. G. Kemler Nelson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lederman, S J" uniqKey="Lederman S">S. J. Lederman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Klatzky, R L" uniqKey="Klatzky R">R. L. Klatzky</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mandler, J M" uniqKey="Mandler J">J. M. Mandler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mandler, J M" uniqKey="Mandler J">J. M. Mandler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bauer, P J" uniqKey="Bauer P">P. J. Bauer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mandler, J M" uniqKey="Mandler J">J. M. Mandler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bauer, P J" uniqKey="Bauer P">P. J. Bauer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcdonough, L" uniqKey="Mcdonough L">L. McDonough</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mandler, J M" uniqKey="Mandler J">J. M. Mandler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcdonough, L" uniqKey="Mcdonough L">L. McDonough</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Oakes, L M" uniqKey="Oakes L">L. M. Oakes</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Madole, K L" uniqKey="Madole K">K. L. Madole</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Perone, S" uniqKey="Perone S">S. Perone</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Spencer, J P" uniqKey="Spencer J">J. P. Spencer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Perry, L K" uniqKey="Perry L">L. K. Perry</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Samuelson, L K" uniqKey="Samuelson L">L. K. Samuelson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Perry, L K" uniqKey="Perry L">L. K. Perry</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Samuelson, L K" uniqKey="Samuelson L">L. K. Samuelson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Burdinie, J B" uniqKey="Burdinie J">J. B. Burdinie</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Perry, L K" uniqKey="Perry L">L. K. Perry</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Samuelson, L K" uniqKey="Samuelson L">L. K. Samuelson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Malloy, L M" uniqKey="Malloy L">L. M. Malloy</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schiffer, R N" uniqKey="Schiffer R">R. N. Schiffer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Perry, L K" uniqKey="Perry L">L. K. Perry</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Samuelson, L K" uniqKey="Samuelson L">L. K. Samuelson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Spencer, J P" uniqKey="Spencer J">J. P. Spencer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Perry, L K" uniqKey="Perry L">L. K. Perry</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, L B" uniqKey="Smith L">L. B. Smith</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hockema, S A" uniqKey="Hockema S">S. A. Hockema</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Quinn, P C" uniqKey="Quinn P">P. C. Quinn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Eimas, P D" uniqKey="Eimas P">P. D. Eimas</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rosenkrantz, S L" uniqKey="Rosenkrantz S">S. L. Rosenkrantz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Roberts, K" uniqKey="Roberts K">K. Roberts</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cuff, M D" uniqKey="Cuff M">M. D. Cuff</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Samuelson, L K" uniqKey="Samuelson L">L. K. Samuelson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bloom, P" uniqKey="Bloom P">P. Bloom</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Samuelson, L K" uniqKey="Samuelson L">L. K. Samuelson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Horst, J S" uniqKey="Horst J">J. S. Horst</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Samuelson, L K" uniqKey="Samuelson L">L. K. Samuelson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Perry, L K" uniqKey="Perry L">L. K. Perry</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Warrington, A K" uniqKey="Warrington A">A. K. Warrington</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Samuelson, L K" uniqKey="Samuelson L">L. K. Samuelson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, L B" uniqKey="Smith L">L. B. Smith</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Samuelson, L K" uniqKey="Samuelson L">L. K. Samuelson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, L B" uniqKey="Smith L">L. B. Smith</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Samuelson, L K" uniqKey="Samuelson L">L. K. Samuelson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, L B" uniqKey="Smith L">L. B. Smith</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Perry, L K" uniqKey="Perry L">L. K. Perry</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Spencer, J P" uniqKey="Spencer J">J. P. Spencer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schrodinger, E" uniqKey="Schrodinger E">E. Schrödinger</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, L B" uniqKey="Smith L">L. B. Smith</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, L B" uniqKey="Smith L">L. B. Smith</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gasser, M" uniqKey="Gasser M">M. Gasser</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Soska, K C" uniqKey="Soska K">K. C. Soska</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Adolph, K E" uniqKey="Adolph K">K. E. Adolph</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnson, S P" uniqKey="Johnson S">S. P. Johnson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Spelke, E S" uniqKey="Spelke E">E. S. Spelke</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Breinlinger, K" uniqKey="Breinlinger K">K. Breinlinger</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Macomber, J" uniqKey="Macomber J">J. Macomber</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jacobson, K" uniqKey="Jacobson K">K. Jacobson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thelen, E" uniqKey="Thelen E">E. Thelen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, L B" uniqKey="Smith L">L. B. Smith</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yee, E" uniqKey="Yee E">E. Yee</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chrysikou, E G" uniqKey="Chrysikou E">E. G. Chrysikou</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hoffman, E" uniqKey="Hoffman E">E. Hoffman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thompson Schill, S L" uniqKey="Thompson Schill S">S. L. Thompson-Schill</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yee, M" uniqKey="Yee M">M. Yee</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jones, S S" uniqKey="Jones S">S. S. Jones</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, L B" uniqKey="Smith L">L. B. Smith</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yoshida, H" uniqKey="Yoshida H">H. Yoshida</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, L B" uniqKey="Smith L">L. B. Smith</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
</listBibl>
</div1>
</back>
</TEI>
<pmc article-type="editorial">
<pmc-dir>properties open_access</pmc-dir>
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="nlm-ta">Front Psychol</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="iso-abbrev">Front Psychol</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="publisher-id">Front. Psychol.</journal-id>
<journal-title-group>
<journal-title>Frontiers in Psychology</journal-title>
</journal-title-group>
<issn pub-type="epub">1664-1078</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Frontiers Media S.A.</publisher-name>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmid">25741314</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmc">4332276</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00178</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Opinion Article</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>To have and to hold: looking vs. touching in the study of categorization</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Perry</surname>
<given-names>Lynn K.</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="author-notes" rid="fn001">
<sup>*</sup>
</xref>
<uri xlink:type="simple" xlink:href="http://community.frontiersin.org/people/u/34980"></uri>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<aff>
<institution>Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison</institution>
<country>Madison, WI, USA</country>
</aff>
<author-notes>
<corresp id="fn001">*Correspondence:
<email xlink:type="simple">lkperry@wisc.edu</email>
</corresp>
<fn fn-type="other" id="fn002">
<p>This article was submitted to Cognition, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology.</p>
</fn>
<fn fn-type="edited-by">
<p>Edited by: Vladimir Sloutsky, The Ohio State University, USA</p>
</fn>
<fn fn-type="edited-by">
<p>Reviewed by: Hanako Yoshida, University of Houston, USA; Chris Robinson, The Ohio State University, USA</p>
</fn>
</author-notes>
<pub-date pub-type="epub">
<day>18</day>
<month>2</month>
<year>2015</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="collection">
<year>2015</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>6</volume>
<elocation-id>178</elocation-id>
<history>
<date date-type="received">
<day>01</day>
<month>11</month>
<year>2014</year>
</date>
<date date-type="accepted">
<day>04</day>
<month>2</month>
<year>2015</year>
</date>
</history>
<permissions>
<copyright-statement>Copyright © 2015 Perry.</copyright-statement>
<copyright-year>2015</copyright-year>
<license license-type="open-access" xlink:href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/">
<license-p>This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.</license-p>
</license>
</permissions>
<kwd-group>
<kwd>categorization</kwd>
<kwd>novel noun generalization</kwd>
<kwd>manual exploration</kwd>
<kwd>context</kwd>
<kwd>looking</kwd>
</kwd-group>
<counts>
<fig-count count="0"></fig-count>
<table-count count="0"></table-count>
<equation-count count="0"></equation-count>
<ref-count count="39"></ref-count>
<page-count count="4"></page-count>
<word-count count="3085"></word-count>
</counts>
</article-meta>
</front>
<body>
<p>In order to make sense of the “blooming, buzzing, confusion” of a world where no two objects or events are ever exactly identical, infants form categories of perceptually distinct items that can be treated equivalently. The study of categorization often arises from one of two motivations: (1) to examine the mechanisms by which infants learn to treat distinct objects/events as equivalent (e.g., how an infant comes to name two different animals dog) and (2) to examine the current state of an infant's knowledge (e.g., does an infant
<italic>have</italic>
a category of dog?). As an illustration, consider Quinn et al.'s (
<xref rid="B23" ref-type="bibr">1993</xref>
) study. After habituating to different dog pictures, 3- and 4-month-olds do not dishabituate to cats. However, after habituating to cats, they
<italic>do</italic>
dishabituate to dogs. If we were examining the current state of knowledge, what should we conclude? Unless we want to suggest these infants are participants in Schrödinger's thought experiment (
<xref rid="B31" ref-type="bibr">1935</xref>
)—they both have and do not have a cat category—we cannot say anything conclusive. Instead, the results reveal something important about the mechanisms of categorization. Quinn et al. (
<xref rid="B23" ref-type="bibr">1993</xref>
) showed variability between category members affects the exclusivity of the categories infants form. This example demonstrates that studying whether infants
<italic>have</italic>
a category misses the crucial point that categories are neither things that people have in their heads nor that exist in the world; but rather categorization is a process (see also Oakes and Madole,
<xref rid="B16" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
; Samuelson and Smith,
<xref rid="B29" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
; Samuelson et al.,
<xref rid="B27" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
).</p>
<p>In addition to these two main motivations, the study of categorization typically utilizes one of two methodologies: (1)
<italic>looking measures</italic>
, e.g., habituation and preferential looking, and (2)
<italic>touching/reaching measures</italic>
, e.g., sequential touching or manual forced choice measures. What I will argue in this paper is that looking measures (1) too readily lend themselves to interpretations of infants having a category or not, and (2) miss the importance the body and physical context have on development.</p>
<sec>
<title>A temptation of looking</title>
<p>Too often the focus of looking measures is the outcome—do infants look longer at one stimulus—without regard for how past experiences and the present context influence infants' looking (but see, Perone and Spencer,
<xref rid="B17" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
). Interpretations that infants do or do not
<italic>have</italic>
a category have proven to be particularly tempting in studies examining infants' categorization of physical events (see also Haith,
<xref rid="B6" ref-type="bibr">1998</xref>
). For example, evidence that 3 to 4-month-old infants habituated to a ball rolling behind an occluder dishabituate to an impossible event—a ball rolling behind an occluder and not stopping at a barrier—has been interpreted as evidence for infants having knowledge of physical laws of solidity (Spelke et al.,
<xref rid="B35" ref-type="bibr">1992</xref>
). However, in experiments employing reaching measures, when a ball rolls behind a panel of doors and stops at a barrier to the right of one of the doors, older children fail to select the correct door, instead perseveratively reaching for the same door regardless of the barrier location (Berthier et al.,
<xref rid="B4" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
). This failure makes it difficult to say whether infants
<italic>have</italic>
knowledge of physical laws.</p>
<p>However, failure can tell us something more interesting about development (Keen,
<xref rid="B9" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
; Perry et al.,
<xref rid="B22" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
,
<xref rid="B21" ref-type="bibr">2009</xref>
). For example, Perry et al. (
<xref rid="B21" ref-type="bibr">2009</xref>
) demonstrated changes in children's ability to align their body and movements with relevant spatial reference frames support search accuracy. When 2-year-olds underwent training with the barrier in a fixed position while the ramp moved, such that the correct door was aligned with midline, they correctly located the object during the standard testing procedure. The context of children's environment—where their bodies are relative to objects and events in the world—influences where they attend and subsequently remember. Such findings demonstrate that the context of learning is critical to understanding developmental change—findings that would not have been made through looking measures. This realization can be applied to the study of categorization.</p>
<p>The widespread use of habituation beginning in the 1980s means that much of our knowledge about infants' categorization comes from their visual exploration of (often) static images (or occasionally visual stimuli in the context of auditory stimuli). Looking measures offer advantages over touching/reaching measures. They can be used with young infants with poor motor control; and computer displays allow increased precision in stimuli construction and presentation. The ability to conduct experiments with a variety of age groups and with high precision has benefited the study of many developmental phenomena, including categorization.</p>
<p>However, looking methods can only tell us about categorization of visual or auditory stimuli. A growing body of evidence suggests that word learning and categorization are often closely tied to other perceptual and sensori-motor aspects of the learning context (see e.g., Samuelson et al.,
<xref rid="B30" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
; Perry et al.,
<xref rid="B19" ref-type="bibr">2014</xref>
). Additionally, the particular task used to measure categorization matters. For example, researchers investigating basic and superordinate-level categorization discovered infants generally form broader categories (corresponding to adult-defined superordinate-level categories such as animal and vehicle) during touching tasks (see e.g., Mandler and Bauer,
<xref rid="B13" ref-type="bibr">1988</xref>
; Mandler et al.,
<xref rid="B14" ref-type="bibr">1991</xref>
; Mandler and McDonough,
<xref rid="B15" ref-type="bibr">1993</xref>
; and see Mandler,
<xref rid="B12" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
for review) and narrower categories (corresponding to adult-defined basic-level categories such as horse and zebra) during looking tasks (see e.g., Roberts and Cuff,
<xref rid="B24" ref-type="bibr">1989</xref>
; Behl-Chadha,
<xref rid="B3" ref-type="bibr">1996</xref>
; and see Mandler,
<xref rid="B12" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
for review). Thus, relying
<italic>solely</italic>
on looking measures therefore provides an incomplete picture of categorization. Next, I discuss evidence from one particular type of categorization—novel noun generalization—that illustrates an essential role for manual actions in learning.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Novel noun generalization: a test case for touching</title>
<p>Children develop word-learning biases that help them generalize novel names to novel objects. By 2-years-old, children typically generalize names of novel solid objects by similarity in shape (shape bias). By 4-years-old, they typically generalize names of novel non-solid substances by material (material bias). Researchers debate what generalization by shape or material similarity says about the role of perceptual and conceptual information in children's category learning (see Samuelson and Bloom,
<xref rid="B25" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
). The attentional learning account proposes biases emerge out of associations between perceptual and linguistic regularities (e.g., Samuelson and Smith,
<xref rid="B28" ref-type="bibr">1999</xref>
; Yoshida and Smith,
<xref rid="B39" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
; Perry and Samuelson,
<xref rid="B18" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
). On the other hand, knowledge-based accounts argue biases are driven not by lower-level regularities but higher-level knowledge about, e.g., functions and core properties (Hammer and Diesendruck,
<xref rid="B7" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
; Kemler Nelson and 11 Swarthmore College Students,
<xref rid="B10" ref-type="bibr">1995</xref>
). Importantly, both accounts recognize the effects of context in categorization. For example, children's tendency to demonstrate the shape bias (Hammer and Diesendruck,
<xref rid="B7" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
) and the material bias (Samuelson and Horst,
<xref rid="B26" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
) varies with the specific stimuli and task used. Context-dependency is not a problem of a task that needs to be removed, however, but a fact of categorization. Categorization is not static, but emerges as prior knowledge is brought to bear in a given context (see e.g., Samuelson et al.,
<xref rid="B27" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
). Converging evidence supporting this view also suggests an important role for manual action in categorization.</p>
<p>Smith (
<xref rid="B32" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
) found that the actions children performed on an object influenced how they generalized its name. When 2-year-olds were told a novel object was a “wug” and taught to move it up and down, they generalized the name to an object elongated on the vertical axis rather than one elongated on the horizontal axis. Critically, the child had perform the actions, as children in a condition who watched the experimenter perform the action did not systematically select objects elongated consistent with the movement (Smith,
<xref rid="B32" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
). By examining children's manual interactions with real, physical objects, these results demonstrate that the actions children perform with objects determine what information they use for categorization.</p>
<p>Similarly, Perry et al. (
<xref rid="B19" ref-type="bibr">2014</xref>
) found that children's understanding of non-solid substances (e.g., pudding) is influenced by context-dependent actions. At home, toddlers primarily learn about non-solids during mealtimes, when they sit in a highchair and can touch and eat non-solid foods. When 16-months-olds sat in a highchair in the laboratory, they were more likely to messily touch novel substances than those seated at a table. The messier children were, the more likely they were to generalize names of novel non-solid substances by material. The highchair is a cue to a context-dependent action pattern supporting attention to material. Critically, children who did not touch the substances failed to generalize novel names by material, suggesting looking is not enough. We would miss important discoveries if we only used looking measures to study children's novel noun generalization. What the touching method afforded was the ability to see
<italic>how</italic>
children learn about material.</p>
<p>Together these studies reveal context is not a factor to eliminate from experiments, but instead an inseparable part of categorization. Responses in these studies were measured via touching (which object/substance did the child hand to the experimenter). Nevertheless, contrasting touching (or amount of touching in Perry et al.'s study) with looking
<italic>during learning</italic>
revealed children's manual actions determine the information they use to generalize. Moreover, by showing how critical actions are to learning, these studies provide evidence for the usefulness of employing touching measures in studying categorization.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Why touching matters</title>
<p>Mandler (
<xref rid="B12" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
) proposed an important difference between looking- and touching-based tasks: infants are more interested in and more actively explore objects than static images. Infants develop and explore within a world full of rich multimodal information. Indeed, even adults' perception of familiar objects is influenced by manual exploration (Lederman and Klatzky,
<xref rid="B11" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
; Yee et al.,
<xref rid="B37" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
). Embodied cognition theorists posit exploration and the multimodal nature of experiences are critical to development (Thelen and Smith,
<xref rid="B36" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
; Smith and Gasser,
<xref rid="B33" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
). Redundant information across modalities (e.g., visual, haptic) facilitates learning of more abstract information (see Bahrick et al.,
<xref rid="B2" ref-type="bibr">2004</xref>
).</p>
<p>Cross-modal redundancy is especially useful for understanding shape and material. As an infant explores a toy, haptic information is integrated with visual information. Over development, exploration changes representation of that toy's shape, and objects' shapes in general (e.g., Soska et al.,
<xref rid="B34" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
). The ability to recognize an object by its shape, is a critical precursor to developing a shape bias (Yee et al.,
<xref rid="B38" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
). As infants explore non-solids, there is similarly redundant sensory information. Nonsolid substances are particularly difficult to recognize by vision alone (cf. Adelson,
<xref rid="B1" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
)—e.g., visual similarity between milk and glue is high, while haptic similarity is low. Over development, exploration increases infants' ability to learn subtle visual cues associated with different materials—critical for categorization of non-solids (see also, Perry et al.,
<xref rid="B19" ref-type="bibr">2014</xref>
).</p>
<p>Given how important multimodal interactions are to development, we as a field cannot rely on looking measures at the expense of what can be learned studying categorization in the context of real objects. Looking measures focusing only on learning
<italic>outcomes</italic>
cannot reveal the processes driving categorization. Nevertheless, looking measures have a place in the study of categorization. As critical as touching is to infants' development, not every question is a touching question. Studies examining where infants look during learning have provided important insights into what infants glean from a given context and subsequent effects on object perception (Johnson et al.,
<xref rid="B8" ref-type="bibr">2004</xref>
) and categorization (Best et al.,
<xref rid="B5" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
). Studying looking as an exploratory process and not just an index of cognition—whether using eye-tracking or traditional habituation—will be a fruitful complement to studies of manual exploration.</p>
</sec>
<sec sec-type="conclusions" id="s1">
<title>Conclusions</title>
<p>Categorization is a critical process by which we make sense of the world. Importantly, when children learn about individual categories, they are also learning higher-order regularities between categories, changing the way they learn categories in the future (e.g., Perry et al.,
<xref rid="B20" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
). Thus, studying children's categorization is valuable in understanding how knowledge and learning
<italic>change</italic>
over time. Given that knowledge is dynamically constructed with information from multiple senses, inseparable from context, and children are developing within a world where they are engaging these senses, we miss something important if we
<italic>only</italic>
study categorization using looking measures. As a field, we cannot ignore the richness of children's everyday actions and experiences; therefore, we must engage a variety of methodologies to understand development and the categorization process.</p>
<sec>
<title>Conflict of interest statement</title>
<p>The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
</body>
<back>
<ack>
<p>I would like to thank Larissa Samuelson and Michelle Quinn for helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper.</p>
</ack>
<ref-list>
<title>References</title>
<ref id="B1">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Adelson</surname>
<given-names>E. H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>On seeing stuff: the perception of materials by humans and machines,</article-title>
in
<source>Proceedings of the SPIE, Vol. 4299</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Rogowitz</surname>
<given-names>B. E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pappas</surname>
<given-names>T. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-name>Human Vision and Electronic Imaging VI</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>12</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B2">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bahrick</surname>
<given-names>L. E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lickliter</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Flom</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Intersensory redundancy guides the development of selective attention, perception, and cognition in infancy</article-title>
.
<source>Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci</source>
.
<volume>13</volume>
,
<fpage>99</fpage>
<lpage>102</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00283.x</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B3">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Behl-Chadha</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1996</year>
).
<article-title>Basic-level and superordinate-like categorical representations in early infancy</article-title>
.
<source>Cognition</source>
<volume>60</volume>
,
<fpage>105</fpage>
<lpage>141</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0010-0277(96)00706-8</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8811742</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B4">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Berthier</surname>
<given-names>N. E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>DeBlois</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Poirier</surname>
<given-names>C. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Novak</surname>
<given-names>M. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Clifton</surname>
<given-names>R. K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Where's the ball? Two- and three-year-olds reason about unseen events</article-title>
.
<source>Dev. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>36</volume>
,
<fpage>394</fpage>
<lpage>401</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0012-1649.36.3.394</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10830982</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B5">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Best</surname>
<given-names>C. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Yim</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sloutsky</surname>
<given-names>V. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>The cost of selective attention in category learning: developmental differences between adults and infants</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Child Psychol</source>
.
<volume>116</volume>
,
<fpage>105</fpage>
<lpage>119</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jecp.2013.05.002</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23773914</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B6">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Haith</surname>
<given-names>M. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1998</year>
).
<article-title>Who put the cog in infant cognition? Is rich interpretation too costly?</article-title>
<source>Infant Behav. Dev</source>
.
<volume>21</volume>
,
<fpage>167</fpage>
<lpage>179</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90001-7</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B7">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hammer</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Diesendruck</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>The role of dimensional distinctiveness in children's and adults' artifact categorization</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci</source>
.
<volume>16</volume>
,
<fpage>137</fpage>
<lpage>144</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00794.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15686580</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B8">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Johnson</surname>
<given-names>S. P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Slemmer</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Amso</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Where infants look determines how they see: eye movements and object perception performance in 3-month-olds</article-title>
.
<source>Infancy</source>
<volume>6</volume>
,
<fpage>185</fpage>
<lpage>201</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1207/s15327078in0602_3</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B9">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Keen</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Representation of objects and events why do infants look so smart and toddlers look so dumb?</article-title>
<source>Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci</source>
.
<volume>12</volume>
,
<fpage>79</fpage>
<lpage>83</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/1467-8721.01234</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B10">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kemler Nelson</surname>
<given-names>D. G.</given-names>
</name>
<collab>11 Swarthmore College Students.</collab>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>Principle-based inferences in young children's categorization: revisiting the impact of function on the naming of artifacts</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Dev</source>
.
<volume>10</volume>
,
<fpage>347</fpage>
<lpage>380</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0885-2014(95)90002-0</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B11">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lederman</surname>
<given-names>S. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Klatzky</surname>
<given-names>R. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<article-title>Haptic classification of common objects: knowledge-driven exploration</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>22</volume>
,
<fpage>421</fpage>
<lpage>459</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0010-0285(90)90009-S</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">2253454</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B12">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Mandler</surname>
<given-names>J. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Perceptual and conceptual processes in infancy</article-title>
.
<source>J. Cogn. Dev</source>
.
<volume>1</volume>
,
<fpage>3</fpage>
<lpage>36</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1207/S15327647JCD0101N_2</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B13">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Mandler</surname>
<given-names>J. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bauer</surname>
<given-names>P. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1988</year>
).
<article-title>The cradle of categorization: is the basic level basic?</article-title>
<source>Cogn. Dev</source>
.
<volume>3</volume>
,
<fpage>247</fpage>
<lpage>264</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0885-2014(88)90011-1</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B14">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Mandler</surname>
<given-names>J. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bauer</surname>
<given-names>P. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McDonough</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1991</year>
).
<article-title>Separating the sheep from the goats: differentiating global categories</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>23</volume>
,
<fpage>263</fpage>
<lpage>298</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0010-0285(91)90011-C</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B15">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Mandler</surname>
<given-names>J. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McDonough</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1993</year>
).
<article-title>Concept formation in infancy</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Dev</source>
.
<volume>8</volume>
,
<fpage>291</fpage>
<lpage>318</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0885-2014(93)80003-C</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B16">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Oakes</surname>
<given-names>L. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Madole</surname>
<given-names>K. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>The future of infant categorization research: a process-oriented approach</article-title>
.
<source>Child Dev</source>
.
<volume>71</volume>
,
<fpage>119</fpage>
<lpage>126</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/1467-8624.00125</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10836565</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B17">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Perone</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Spencer</surname>
<given-names>J. P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Autonomy in action: linking the act of looking to memory formation in infancy via dynamic neural fields</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Sci</source>
.
<volume>37</volume>
,
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>60</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/cogs.12010</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23136815</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B18">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Perry</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Samuelson</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>The shape of the vocabulary predicts the shape of the bias</article-title>
.
<source>Front. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>2</volume>
:
<issue>345</issue>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00345</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22125547</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B19">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Perry</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Samuelson</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Burdinie</surname>
<given-names>J. B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2014</year>
).
<article-title>Highchair philosophers: the impact of seating context-dependent exploration on children's naming biases</article-title>
.
<source>Dev. Sci</source>
.
<volume>17</volume>
,
<fpage>757</fpage>
<lpage>765</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/desc.12147</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">24289734</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B20">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Perry</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Samuelson</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Malloy</surname>
<given-names>L. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schiffer</surname>
<given-names>R. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Learn locally, think globally: exemplar variability supports higher-order generalization and word learning</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci</source>
.
<volume>21</volume>
,
<fpage>1894</fpage>
<lpage>1902</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0956797610389189</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21106892</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B21">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Perry</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Samuelson</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Spencer</surname>
<given-names>J. P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Aligning body and world: stable reference frames improve young children's search for hidden objects</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Child Psychol</source>
.
<volume>102</volume>
,
<fpage>445</fpage>
<lpage>455</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jecp.2008.11.006</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19167014</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B22">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Perry</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>L. B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hockema</surname>
<given-names>S. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Representational momentum and children's sensori-motor representations of objects</article-title>
.
<source>Dev. Sci</source>
.
<volume>11</volume>
,
<fpage>F17</fpage>
<lpage>F23</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00672.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18466366</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B23">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Quinn</surname>
<given-names>P. C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Eimas</surname>
<given-names>P. D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rosenkrantz</surname>
<given-names>S. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1993</year>
).
<article-title>Evidence for representations of perceptually similar natural categories by 3-month-old and 4-month-old infants</article-title>
.
<source>Perception</source>
<volume>22</volume>
,
<fpage>463</fpage>
<lpage>475</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1068/p220463</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8378134</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B24">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Roberts</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cuff</surname>
<given-names>M. D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1989</year>
).
<article-title>Categorization studies of 9- to 15-month-old infants: evidence for superordinate categorization?</article-title>
<source>Infant Behav. Dev</source>
.
<volume>12</volume>
,
<fpage>265</fpage>
<lpage>288</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0163-6383(89)90039-8</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B25">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Samuelson</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bloom</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>The shape of controversy: what counts as an explanation of development? Introduction to the special section</article-title>
.
<source>Dev. Sci</source>
.
<volume>11</volume>
,
<fpage>183</fpage>
<lpage>184</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00663.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18333972</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B26">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Samuelson</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Horst</surname>
<given-names>J. S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Dynamic noun generalization: moment-to-moment interactions shape children's naming biases</article-title>
.
<source>Infancy</source>
<volume>11</volume>
,
<fpage>97</fpage>
<lpage>110</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1207/s15327078in1101_5</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B27">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Samuelson</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Perry</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Warrington</surname>
<given-names>A. K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Drawing conclusions about categorization: integrating perceptual and conceptual processes in naming</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Creier Comportament</source>
<volume>11</volume>
,
<fpage>695</fpage>
<lpage>712</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B28">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Samuelson</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>L. B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<article-title>Early noun vocabularies: do ontology, category structure and syntax correspond?</article-title>
<source>Cognition</source>
<volume>73</volume>
,
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>33</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00034-7</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10536222</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B29">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Samuelson</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>L. B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Grounding development in cognitive processes</article-title>
.
<source>Child Dev</source>
.
<volume>71</volume>
,
<fpage>98</fpage>
<lpage>106</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/1467-8624.00123</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10836563</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B30">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Samuelson</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>L. B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Perry</surname>
<given-names>L. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Spencer</surname>
<given-names>J. P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Grounding word learning in space</article-title>
.
<source>PLoS ONE</source>
<volume>6</volume>
:
<fpage>e28095</fpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0028095</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22194807</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B31">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schrödinger</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1935</year>
).
<article-title>Die gegenwärtige situation in der quantenmechanik</article-title>
.
<source>Naturwissenschaften</source>
<volume>23</volume>
,
<fpage>807</fpage>
<lpage>812</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/BF01491891</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B32">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>L. B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Action alters shape categories</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Sci</source>
.
<volume>29</volume>
,
<fpage>665</fpage>
<lpage>679</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1207/s15516709cog0000_13</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21702789</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B33">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>L. B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gasser</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>The development of embodied cognition: six lessons from babies</article-title>
.
<source>Artif. Life</source>
<volume>11</volume>
,
<fpage>13</fpage>
<lpage>29</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1162/1064546053278973</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15811218</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B34">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Soska</surname>
<given-names>K. C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Adolph</surname>
<given-names>K. E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Johnson</surname>
<given-names>S. P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Systems in development: motor skill acquisition facilitates three-dimensional object completion</article-title>
.
<source>Dev. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>46</volume>
,
<fpage>129</fpage>
<lpage>138</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0014618</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20053012</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B35">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Spelke</surname>
<given-names>E. S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Breinlinger</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Macomber</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jacobson</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1992</year>
).
<article-title>Origins of knowledge</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Rev</source>
.
<volume>99</volume>
,
<fpage>605</fpage>
<lpage>632</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-295X.99.4.605</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">1454901</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B36">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Thelen</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>L. B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<source>A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>MIT Press</publisher-name>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B37">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Yee</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Chrysikou</surname>
<given-names>E. G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hoffman</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thompson-Schill</surname>
<given-names>S. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Manual experience shapes object representations</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci</source>
.
<volume>24</volume>
,
<fpage>909</fpage>
<lpage>919</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0956797612464658</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23633520</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B38">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Yee</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jones</surname>
<given-names>S. S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>L. B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Changes in visual object recognition precede the shape bias in early noun learning</article-title>
.
<source>Front. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>3</volume>
:
<issue>533</issue>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00533</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23227015</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B39">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Yoshida</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>L. B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Linguistic cues enhance the learning of perceptual cues</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci</source>
.
<volume>16</volume>
,
<fpage>90</fpage>
<lpage>95</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00787.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15686573</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</pmc>
<affiliations>
<list></list>
<tree>
<noCountry>
<name sortKey="Perry, Lynn K" sort="Perry, Lynn K" uniqKey="Perry L" first="Lynn K." last="Perry">Lynn K. Perry</name>
</noCountry>
</tree>
</affiliations>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Ticri/CIDE/explor/HapticV1/Data/Ncbi/Merge
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 003735 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Ncbi/Merge/biblio.hfd -nk 003735 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Ticri/CIDE
   |area=    HapticV1
   |flux=    Ncbi
   |étape=   Merge
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     PMC:4332276
   |texte=   To have and to hold: looking vs. touching in the study of categorization
}}

Pour générer des pages wiki

HfdIndexSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Ncbi/Merge/RBID.i   -Sk "pubmed:25741314" \
       | HfdSelect -Kh $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Ncbi/Merge/biblio.hfd   \
       | NlmPubMed2Wicri -a HapticV1 

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.23.
Data generation: Mon Jun 13 01:09:46 2016. Site generation: Wed Mar 6 09:54:07 2024