Serveur d'exploration sur les dispositifs haptiques

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

Linear Path Integration Deficits in Patients with Abnormal Vestibular Afference

Identifieur interne : 003150 ( Istex/Corpus ); précédent : 003149; suivant : 003151

Linear Path Integration Deficits in Patients with Abnormal Vestibular Afference

Auteurs : Michael C. Schubert ; Joeanna C. Arthur ; Mark Shelhamer ; Kathleen B. Kortte

Source :

RBID : ISTEX:1CF21DAD37B8234A4A160C65709A413B551FE25B
Url:
DOI: 10.1163/187847612X629928

Links to Exploration step

ISTEX:1CF21DAD37B8234A4A160C65709A413B551FE25B

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI wicri:istexFullTextTei="biblStruct">
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title>Linear Path Integration Deficits in Patients with Abnormal Vestibular Afference</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, Michael C" sort="Schubert, Michael C" uniqKey="Schubert M" first="Michael C." last="Schubert">Michael C. Schubert</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Arthur, Joeanna C" sort="Arthur, Joeanna C" uniqKey="Arthur J" first="Joeanna C." last="Arthur">Joeanna C. Arthur</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shelhamer, Mark" sort="Shelhamer, Mark" uniqKey="Shelhamer M" first="Mark" last="Shelhamer">Mark Shelhamer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kortte, Kathleen B" sort="Kortte, Kathleen B" uniqKey="Kortte K" first="Kathleen B." last="Kortte">Kathleen B. Kortte</name>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">ISTEX</idno>
<idno type="RBID">ISTEX:1CF21DAD37B8234A4A160C65709A413B551FE25B</idno>
<date when="2012" year="2012">2012</date>
<idno type="doi">10.1163/187847612X629928</idno>
<idno type="url">https://api.istex.fr/document/1CF21DAD37B8234A4A160C65709A413B551FE25B/fulltext/pdf</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Istex/Corpus">003150</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title level="a">Linear Path Integration Deficits in Patients with Abnormal Vestibular Afference</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, Michael C" sort="Schubert, Michael C" uniqKey="Schubert M" first="Michael C." last="Schubert">Michael C. Schubert</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Arthur, Joeanna C" sort="Arthur, Joeanna C" uniqKey="Arthur J" first="Joeanna C." last="Arthur">Joeanna C. Arthur</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shelhamer, Mark" sort="Shelhamer, Mark" uniqKey="Shelhamer M" first="Mark" last="Shelhamer">Mark Shelhamer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kortte, Kathleen B" sort="Kortte, Kathleen B" uniqKey="Kortte K" first="Kathleen B." last="Kortte">Kathleen B. Kortte</name>
</author>
</analytic>
<monogr></monogr>
<series>
<title level="j">Seeing and Perceiving</title>
<title level="j" type="sub">A Journal of Multisensory Science</title>
<title level="j" type="abbrev">SP</title>
<idno type="ISSN">1878-4755</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1878-4763</idno>
<imprint>
<publisher>BRILL</publisher>
<pubPlace>The Netherlands</pubPlace>
<date type="published" when="2012">2012</date>
<biblScope unit="volume">25</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="issue">2</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" from="155">155</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" to="178">178</biblScope>
</imprint>
<idno type="ISSN">1878-4755</idno>
</series>
<idno type="istex">1CF21DAD37B8234A4A160C65709A413B551FE25B</idno>
<idno type="DOI">10.1163/187847612X629928</idno>
<idno type="href">18784763_025_02_s003_text.pdf</idno>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
<seriesStmt>
<idno type="ISSN">1878-4755</idno>
</seriesStmt>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
<langUsage>
<language ident="en">en</language>
</langUsage>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
</TEI>
<istex>
<corpusName>brill-journals</corpusName>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>Michael C. Schubert</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>Joeanna C. Arthur</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>Mark Shelhamer</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>Kathleen B. Kortte</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<subject>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>path integration</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>Vestibular navigation</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>spatial orientation</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>vestibular hypofunction</value>
</json:item>
</subject>
<language>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</language>
<qualityIndicators>
<score>5.512</score>
<pdfVersion>1.4</pdfVersion>
<pdfPageSize>453.543 x 680.315 pts</pdfPageSize>
<refBibsNative>false</refBibsNative>
<keywordCount>4</keywordCount>
<abstractCharCount>0</abstractCharCount>
<pdfWordCount>9683</pdfWordCount>
<pdfCharCount>59272</pdfCharCount>
<pdfPageCount>24</pdfPageCount>
<abstractWordCount>1</abstractWordCount>
</qualityIndicators>
<title>Linear Path Integration Deficits in Patients with Abnormal Vestibular Afference</title>
<genre.original>
<json:string>research-article</json:string>
</genre.original>
<genre>
<json:string>research-article</json:string>
</genre>
<host>
<volume>25</volume>
<pages>
<last>178</last>
<first>155</first>
</pages>
<issn>
<json:string>1878-4755</json:string>
</issn>
<issue>2</issue>
<genre>
<json:string>Journal</json:string>
</genre>
<language>
<json:string>unknown</json:string>
</language>
<eissn>
<json:string>1878-4763</json:string>
</eissn>
<title>Seeing and Perceiving</title>
</host>
<publicationDate>2012</publicationDate>
<doi>
<json:string>10.1163/187847612X629928</json:string>
</doi>
<id>1CF21DAD37B8234A4A160C65709A413B551FE25B</id>
<score>1</score>
<fulltext>
<json:item>
<original>true</original>
<mimetype>application/pdf</mimetype>
<extension>pdf</extension>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/1CF21DAD37B8234A4A160C65709A413B551FE25B/fulltext/pdf</uri>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>application/zip</mimetype>
<extension>zip</extension>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/1CF21DAD37B8234A4A160C65709A413B551FE25B/fulltext/zip</uri>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>text/plain</mimetype>
<extension>txt</extension>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/1CF21DAD37B8234A4A160C65709A413B551FE25B/fulltext/txt</uri>
</json:item>
<istex:fulltextTEI uri="https://api.istex.fr/document/1CF21DAD37B8234A4A160C65709A413B551FE25B/fulltext/tei">
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title level="a">Linear Path Integration Deficits in Patients with Abnormal Vestibular Afference</title>
<respStmt xml:id="ISTEX-API" resp="Références bibliographiques récupérées via GROBID" name="ISTEX-API (INIST-CNRS)"></respStmt>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<authority>ISTEX</authority>
<publisher>BRILL</publisher>
<pubPlace>The Netherlands</pubPlace>
<availability>
<p>BRILL Journals</p>
</availability>
<date>2012</date>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct type="inbook">
<analytic>
<title level="a">Linear Path Integration Deficits in Patients with Abnormal Vestibular Afference</title>
<author>
<persName>
<forename type="first">Michael C.</forename>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
</persName>
</author>
<author>
<persName>
<forename type="first">Joeanna C.</forename>
<surname>Arthur</surname>
</persName>
</author>
<author>
<persName>
<forename type="first">Mark</forename>
<surname>Shelhamer</surname>
</persName>
</author>
<author>
<persName>
<forename type="first">Kathleen B.</forename>
<surname>Kortte</surname>
</persName>
</author>
</analytic>
<monogr>
<title level="j">Seeing and Perceiving</title>
<title level="j" type="sub">A Journal of Multisensory Science</title>
<title level="j" type="abbrev">SP</title>
<idno type="pISSN">1878-4755</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1878-4763</idno>
<imprint>
<publisher>BRILL</publisher>
<pubPlace>The Netherlands</pubPlace>
<date type="published" when="2012"></date>
<biblScope unit="volume">25</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="issue">2</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" from="155">155</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" to="178">178</biblScope>
</imprint>
</monogr>
<idno type="istex">1CF21DAD37B8234A4A160C65709A413B551FE25B</idno>
<idno type="DOI">10.1163/187847612X629928</idno>
<idno type="href">18784763_025_02_s003_text.pdf</idno>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<creation>
<date>2012</date>
</creation>
<langUsage>
<language ident="en">en</language>
</langUsage>
<textClass>
<keywords scheme="keyword">
<list>
<head>Keywords</head>
<item>
<term>path integration</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>Vestibular navigation</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>spatial orientation</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>vestibular hypofunction</term>
</item>
</list>
</keywords>
</textClass>
</profileDesc>
<revisionDesc>
<change when="2012">Created</change>
<change when="2012">Published</change>
<change xml:id="refBibs-istex" who="#ISTEX-API" when="2016-3-13">References added</change>
</revisionDesc>
</teiHeader>
</istex:fulltextTEI>
</fulltext>
<metadata>
<istex:metadataXml wicri:clean="corpus brill-journals not found" wicri:toSee="no header">
<istex:xmlDeclaration>version="1.0" encoding="UTF-8"</istex:xmlDeclaration>
<istex:docType PUBLIC="-//NLM//DTD Journal Publishing DTD v2.3 20070202//EN" URI="http://dtd.nlm.nih.gov/publishing/2.3/journalpublishing.dtd" name="istex:docType"></istex:docType>
<istex:document>
<article article-type="research-article" dtd-version="2.3">
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="e-issn">18784763</journal-id>
<journal-title>Seeing and Perceiving</journal-title>
<journal-subtitle>A Journal of Multisensory Science</journal-subtitle>
<abbrev-journal-title>SP</abbrev-journal-title>
<issn pub-type="ppub">1878-4755</issn>
<issn pub-type="epub">1878-4763</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>BRILL</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>The Netherlands</publisher-loc>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1163/187847612X629928</article-id>
<title-group>
<article-title>Linear Path Integration Deficits in Patients with Abnormal Vestibular Afference</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>Michael C.</given-names>
</name>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Arthur</surname>
<given-names>Joeanna C.</given-names>
</name>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Shelhamer</surname>
<given-names>Mark</given-names>
</name>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kortte</surname>
<given-names>Kathleen B.</given-names>
</name>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<pub-date pub-type="epub">
<year>2012</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>25</volume>
<issue>2</issue>
<fpage>155</fpage>
<lpage>178</lpage>
<permissions>
<copyright-statement>© Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, The Netherlands</copyright-statement>
<copyright-holder>Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, The Netherlands</copyright-holder>
</permissions>
<self-uri content-type="pdf" xlink:href="18784763_025_02_s003_text.pdf"></self-uri>
<kwd-group>
<kwd>path integration</kwd>
<kwd>Vestibular navigation</kwd>
<kwd>spatial orientation</kwd>
<kwd>vestibular hypofunction</kwd>
</kwd-group>
<custom-meta-wrap>
<custom-meta>
<meta-name>version</meta-name>
<meta-value>header</meta-value>
</custom-meta>
</custom-meta-wrap>
</article-meta>
</front>
<body>
<p>Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 brill.nl/sp Linear Path Integration Deficits in Patients with Abnormal Vestibular Afference Joeanna C. Arthur 1 , Kathleen B. Kortte 2 , Mark Shelhamer 1 , 3 and Michael C. Schubert 1 , ∗ 1 Department of Otolaryngology – Head and Neck Surgery, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA 2 Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA 3 Department of Biomedical Engineering, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA Received 19 March 2011; accepted 21 December 2011 Abstract Effective navigation requires the ability to keep track of one’s location and maintain orientation during linear and angular displacements. Path integration is the process of updating the representation of body position by integrating internally-generated self-motion signals over time (e.g., walking in the dark). One major source of input to path integration is vestibular afference. We tested patients with reduced vestibu- lar function (unilateral vestibular hypofunction, UVH), patients with aberrant vestibular function (benign paroxysmal positional vertigo, BPPV), and healthy participants (controls) on two linear path integration tasks: experimenter-guided walking and target-directed walking. The experimenter-guided walking task re- vealed a systematic underestimation of self-motion signals in UVH patients compared to the other groups. However, we did not find any difference in the distance walked between the UVH group and the control group for the target-directed walking task. Results from neuropsychological testing and clinical balance measures suggest that the errors in experimenter-guided walking were not attributable to cognitive and/or balance impairments. We conclude that impairment in linear path integration in UVH patients stem from deficits in self-motion perception. Importantly, our results also suggest that patients with a UVH deficit do not lose their ability to walk accurately without vision to a memorized target location. © Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2012 Keywords Vestibular navigation, vestibular hypofunction, path integration, spatial orientation * To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: mschube1@jhmi.edu © Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2012 DOI:10.1163/187847612X629928</p>
<p>156 J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 1. Introduction Human spatial orientation and navigation rely on the crucial ability to sense self- motion during linear and angular displacements. The ability to keep track of our location and maintain orientation is so essential to our daily lives that those without this skill are significantly disabled. Vision provides multiple sources of information for determining self-position and orientation in external space. However, in the ab- sence of vision, we retain the ability to remain oriented by relying upon internally- generated (idiothetic) self-motion signals such as proprioceptive cues from the musculature and acceleration signals from the vestibular end organs (Highstein, 1996; Israël and Berthoz, 1989). These signals are then referenced to a remem- bered spatial origin or target location in working memory, along with a method of updating one’s current position and orientation in space, known as path integra- tion or dead reckoning (Etienne et al. , 1996; Loomis et al. , 1999; Mittelstaedt and Mittelstaedt, 1980). There is extensive evidence from animal studies suggesting that the vestibular system is vital in path integration and spatial navigation (Barlow, 1964; Beritoff, 1965; Cullen and Roy, 2004; Etienne et al. , 1996; Mittelstaedt and Mittelstaedt, 1980, Wallace et al. , 2002). Rats with unilateral and bilateral vestibular lesions dis- play deficits in spatial memory and navigation tasks, such as the water maze, radial arm maze, T-maze and foraging tasks (Russell et al. , 2003; Stackman and Herbert, 2002; Wallace et al. , 2002; Zheng et al. , 2006, 2009). In these studies, the rats were tested months after labyrinthectomy, suggesting that any impairment in path inte- gration was not related to acute vestibular or locomotor deficits. Interestingly, the spatial memory and navigational deficits were manifest differently depending on the type of lesion and type of task. Rats with unilateral vestibular deafferentation (UVD) initially showed impairments on a spatial navigation task (food foraging in the dark), which resolved six months after the onset of the lesion (Zheng et al. , 2006). In contrast, although rats with bilateral vestibular deafferentation (BVD) were able to complete the same task in the light, they had chronic deficits when tested in darkness (Zheng et al. , 2009). These data suggest that vestibular affer- ence is essential for spatial orientation and navigation, likely by informing the brain about changes in head and body orientation in space during self-motion. In humans, there is limited but emerging evidence for the role of vestibular affer- ence in path integration. Behavioral testing using a ‘blind walking’ paradigm has been the main method for assessing path integration in humans. In a typical blind walking task, blindfolded participants attempt to walk without vision to a previously viewed target location. Using this approach it has been shown that healthy humans can estimate changes in body location and orientation at distances up to 20 m or more and precisely walk to the target location without vision (Loomis et al. , 1992; Philbeck and Loomis, 1997; Thomson, 1983; for a review see Mittelstaedt and Mit- telstaedt, 2001). In contrast, patients with vestibular deficits show veering and other impairments, during similar non-visually guided walking tasks (Borel et al. , 2004; Cohen, 2000; Cohen and Kimball, 2002; Guidetti et al. , 2008; Péruch et al. , 1999,</p>
<p>J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 157 2005). Of the studies that have been conducted, the majority have relied upon eval- uating individuals with vestibular hypofunction, chronic vestibulopathy, and benign paroxysmal positional vertigo (BPPV) (e.g., Borel et al. , 2004; Cohen, 2000; Co- hen and Kimball, 2002; Guidetti et al. , 2008). These individuals often complain of spatial disorientation in conjunction with the common symptoms of vertigo, im- balance, and gaze instability during head rotation (Herdman and Whitney, 2000; Hillier and Hollohan, 2007). In summary, the overarching findings support the notion that the loss of vestibu- lar afference impacts path integration during blind walking. However, the majority of these studies do not explicitly examine self-motion perception , per se . Instead, gait measures, such as lateral walking deviations, response time, and directional turn errors were reported (Borel et al. , 2004; Cohen, 2000; Guidetti et al. , 2008). For instance, Cohen (2000) asked patients with chronic peripheral vestibular hy- pofunction to walk a linear distance of 7.62 m, once with their eyes open and then three times with their eyes closed. The investigator measured the forward dis- tanced walked prior to and after veering. Compared to healthy controls and patients with vestibular schwawanoma (pre-operative), the chronic vestibulopathy patients had significant errors in the mean distance walked prior to veering and angle of veering when walking a specified linear path with their eyes closed. While these impairments may be directly related to deficits in path integration as a result of the vestibular pathology, this particular study offers minimal insight into the underlying cause of linear path integration deficits due to methodological limitations: patients walked a straight course and were instructed when to stop when they crossed the finish line, which specified the target distance. This meant that participants received feedback as to when they arrived at the true target distance (7.62 m). Theoretically, participants may have responded without using path integration and simply walked the course until receiving the verbal command to stop. In addition, these partici- pants completed an eyes-open condition prior to the eyes-closed condition, which may have confounded the results further by allowing participants to remember the path distance and to reproduce it during the eyes-closed condition. It may also be possible that the participants counted paces and/or estimated the temporal profile (i.e., duration). While path integration likely played a role in this study, the extent to which the monitoring and updating of non-visual self-motion signals contributed to the errors is unclear. In addition to reduced vestibular afference, cognitive and sensorimotor dys- function may also impact performance on non-visual walking tasks. Allen et al. (2004) examined the extent to which cognitive functions, specifically information- processing speed and working memory capacity, contribute to performance on a triangle completion task in healthy young and old participants. Results showed that these cognitive functions significantly accounted for age-declined performance. To date however, most studies on path integration in patients with vestibular disor- ders inadequately control for cognitive impairments or existing balance dysfunction (Borel et al. , 2004; Cohen, 2000; Cohen and Kimball, 2002; Péruch et al. , 1999,</p>
<p>158 J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 2006). There is conflicting evidence of cognitive impairments in humans and ro- dents with vestibular dysfunction (Avni et al. , 2009; Grimm et al. , 1989; Guidetti et al. , 2008; Zheng et al. , 2006). Peripheral lesions in rats are known to cause deficits in attention and spatial memory (Zheng et al. , 2006, 2009). Similarly, using a Corsi block test it has been shown that patients with unilateral vestibular hypofunction (UVH) exhibit spatial memory deficits (Guidetti et al. , 2008). However, others have reported normal performance on tests of general memory, attention, and intelligence (for a review see Hanes and McCollum, 2006; see also Smith et al. , 2005, 2010). One possible explanation for these conflicting results is a majority of patient studies employ self-report questionnaires that assess perceived handicap due to dizziness and imbalance (e.g., Dizziness Handicap Inventory) that are highly correlated with performance (such as posturography tests; see Jacobson et al. , 1991), but do not assess general cognitive functions (e.g., visuospatial memory). As a result, it is difficult to ascertain the degree to which patients’ cognitive impairments (if any) contribute to self-ratings. The primary goal of this study was to explore the parameters in which path in- tegration deficits occur. In addition, we wished to glean insight into the underlying cause of linear path integration deficits in patients with abnormal vestibular affer- ence. Our approach in the current study was to take an initial step in characterizing the effects of unilateral vestibular hypofunction (UVH). For comparison we also tested healthy controls and patients that were treated for BPPV. BPPV is a transient peripheral vestibular disorder caused when calcium carbonate material (otoconia) displaces into the semicircular canals. This addition renders the semicircular canals sensitive to gravity, when they should not normally be. Treatment involves physi- cally repositioning the head and body to return the irritating otoconia back into the utricle (Hilton and Pinder, 2004). We chose to include the BPPV patients to further probe whether a loss of vestibular afference, instead of transient aberrant function, is truly detrimental to path integration. To address these concerns, we tested participants’ performance on two blind- walking tasks: target-directed and experimenter-guided walking. Target-directed walking is a goal-directed task that requires the participant to walk to a previously viewed target location in the absence of direct visual input (Philbeck and Loomis, 1997; Philbeck et al. , 2004). Experimenter-guided walking is also a blind-walking task; however, a target location is not specified before walking. Instead, participants estimated distances walked without vision while guided along a linear trajectory by a sighted experimenter. This task provides an opportunity to assess path integration without drawing upon visual perception and spatial memory of a target. In contrast with prior studies (Borel et al. , 2004; Cohen, 2000; Cohen and Kimball, 2002), our walking tasks are direct measures of self-motion sensing during non-visual loco- motion — with no prior trials that allowed walking with vision. We tested the hypothesis that vestibular afference plays a role in path integration, particularly self-motion updating, and we predict that only patients with missing vestibular afference would exhibit abnormal path integration. We restricted our</p>
<p>J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 159 focus to self-motion updating on linear trajectories using two tasks designed to evaluate path integration separately (described in more detail below). Our focus on two particular vestibular populations (UVH and BPPV) further allowed us to con- duct more comprehensive neuropsychological testing in conjunction with the main behavioral walking tasks. Given that cognitive factors such as visual perception, verbal and visual memory, and visuospatial processing are likely to be implicated in our blind-walking tasks, we chose neuropsychological tests to rule out the pos- sibility that specific cognitive impairments could account for potential errors in self-motion updating. Finally, the severity of ataxia was rated using a qualitative scale in order to assess patients’ imbalance during gait, stance, and sitting. 2. Method 2.1. Participants The study was approved by the Johns Hopkins Medical Institution’s Institutional Review Board and was performed in accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. Three groups of participants provided written in- formed consent prior to their participation. Data from n = 6 patients with UVH and n = 4 patients with BPPV were separately compared to those of five healthy controls (Table 1). Patients were identified from the vestibular rehabilitation clinic at Johns Hopkins Hospital. Patients were included based on clinical and laboratory testing. Testing was performed by one of the authors. Diagnosis of UVH was based on: (1) a history of imbalance, (2) non-positional vertigo, (3) physical examination showing a positive head impulse test result toward the affected ear, (4) absence of a Table 1. Patient and control participants demographics Participant Group Sex Age 1 BPPV Male 63 2 BPPV Male 66 3 BPPV Female 47 4 BPPV Male 60 5 UVH Female 63 6 UVH Female 54 7 UVH Male 38 8 UVH Female 80 9 UVH Female 59 10 UVH Male 46 11 Healthy Female 47 12 Healthy Male 42 13 Healthy Female 35 14 Healthy Female 45 15 Healthy Male 53</p>
<p>160 J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 mass-enhancing lesion within the internal auditory canals or cerebellopontine angle and (5) abnormal electronystagmography (ENG) examination establishing > 20% asymmetry; the ENG battery included oculomotor testing, the caloric test, and po- sitional testing. Posterior semicircular canal canalithiasis (BPPV) was diagnosed based on: (1) normal caloric examination within the ENG testing suite and (2) up- beating nystagmus with a torsional component beating geotropically elicited during the Dix Hallpike test. Healthy control group participants were identified from hos- pital staff and students and by use of IRB-approved fliers. Inclusion criteria for the healthy controls included: (1) negative Romberg test, (2) absent history of neuro- logical disorder, and (3) absent orthopedic impairment. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Exclusion criteria included: (1) non-consentable age (i.e., under 18), (2) over 85 years of age, due to age-related (peripheral and cen- tral) decline of vestibular function, (3) specific motor or visuomotor disorder (e.g., ataxia) that affected posture or dynamic gait control that required use of an assistive device, (4) legal blindness, (5) use of neuroleptics or vestibular-suppressant med- ications and/or (6) inability to tolerate or cooperate with study protocols. We did not test our healthy controls for abnormal vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) function or BPPV; we had no reason to expect any pathology of their inner ear. 2.2. Design Participants were individually tested. The experimental trials were completed in a well-lit, carpeted hallway approximately 15 m long and consisted of three tasks which were blocked and presented in the following order: verbal distance estima- tion , target-directed walking and experimenter-guided walking . The order of trials within each block was randomized. Distances of 3 and 6 m were presented three times within each task. To increase the range of targets, three ‘dummy’ trials were randomly interspersed within the experimental trials. During the dummy trials the target was place at 1.5, 3.5 and 5.5 m or 2.5, 3.5 and 4.5 m, measured one apiece. The dummy trials were also randomly determined. These trials were intended to discourage the participant from pre-determining the two stimulus target distances (i.e., ‘near’ and ‘far’) and attempting to produce the same response on each trial; data from these trials were not included in analyses. Participants were asked to per- form each trial as accurately as possible. There were no time constraints imposed. Walking duration was measured with an electronic stopwatch. A tape measure was used to record distance walked. No feedback concerning results was provided. 2.3. Procedure 2.3.1. Objective and Subjective Measures 2.3.1.1. Path Integration. Using a blind-walking paradigm, two tasks, target- directed walking and experimenter-guided walking , were used to assess path inte- gration (Philbeck and Loomis, 1997; Philbeck et al. , 2004). Target-directed walking requires the participant to walk to a previously viewed target location in the absence of direct visual input (Fig. 1(a)). Experimenter-guided walking requires participants</p>
<p>J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 161 (a) (b) Figure 1. Overhead depiction of the two blind-walking tasks: (a) target-direct and (b) experi- menter-guided walking. to keep track of their displacement from a known starting point while being led to an unknown location and to then generate a verbal estimate of the distance traveled (Fig. 1(b)). Participants were allowed to veer slightly since intrinsic veering ten- dencies exist in blind and blindfolded navigators (Kallie et al. , 2007). No feedback was given. 2.3.1.2. Target-Directed Walking. Participants viewed the target (generally, for several seconds), then lowered a blindfold and donned hearing protectors (overall noise reduction rating: − 30 dB) to minimize auditory cues. They then attempted to walk directly to the estimated target location. The target was removed prior to blind walking for safety reasons and so that participants did not receive haptic feedback. Participants were instructed to walk until they felt they had reached the remembered target location. We did not control for walking speed; instead, participants were in- structed to walk at their normal speed (preferred walking velocity) as they would in everyday life. For safety, on all walking trials a sighted experimenter shadowed the participant to avoid collisions with any environmental objects (e.g., walls, etc.). In addition, to prevent the use of pace counting and other verbalization strategies, participants were given a random four-digit number to memorize. At the end of the trial, participants were asked to recall the four-digit memory number given at the beginning of the trial. Participants were led back to the origin by the sighted exper- imenter, still without vision, to prevent error feedback. The experimenter measured the walked distance from the origin to the terminal location. 2.3.1.3. Experimenter-Guided Walking. Participants viewed their environment for several seconds (no target present), then donned a blindfold and hearing pro- tectors. They were then led by a sighted experimenter along a straight path to a predetermined stimulus distance; participants grasped the experimenter’s upper arm and walked along a straight path between 1.5 to 6 m long. Upon stopping, partic- ipants were required to provide a verbal estimate of the walked distance from the</p>
<p>162 J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 origin (in feet or meters). The four-digit number manipulation was also used in this task to discourage use of pace-counting strategies. 2.3.1.4. Visual Distance Perception. Verbal estimates of target distance were gathered to assess participants’ distance perception. Participants binocularly viewed a single target (an orange cone approximately 31 cm tall) under full-cue viewing conditions and verbally estimated its distance from their location (in feet or meters) after a short delay (less than 5 s) with their eyes closed. This evaluated partici- pants’ ability to visually perceive the target and verbally report its location, without drawing on participant’s path integration abilities (Philbeck et al. , 2004). 2.3.1.5. Neuropsychological Tests. To test for specific cognitive impairments and to characterize the sample populations, each participant performed a battery of neu- ropsychological tests. This included subjective reports and objective measures to allow for comparison between the samples tested in our study with those described in previous studies. Neuropsychological and visuomotor tests were administered prior to the walking task. To minimize the risk of boredom and/or fatigue, breaks were given between each test, and between the neuropsychological testing and ex- periment proper. 2.3.1.6. Verbal Memory. Verbal memory deficits can impact the aforementioned walking tasks, as well as performance on other neuropsychological tests adminis- tered; specifically, patients may have deficits in the acquisition, encoding and reten- tion of experimental instructions. To control for this, we assessed verbal memory using the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R) (Brandt and Benedict, 2001). The HVLT-R examines the ability of patients to learn and remember a ver- bally presented list of 12 target words. The test consists of three immediate recall trials, one delayed recall trial, and one recognition test. Scores from the three recall trials were combined to calculate the total recall score. The percentage of words retained after the delay was also calculated (number of words recalled after delay divided by the greatest number of words recalled on any of the three learning trials). Finally, the recognition discrimination index was calculated by taking the number of true positives minus the number of false positives. Higher scores on all three measures indicate better verbal learning and memory performance. The HVLT-R has been shown to have high test–retest reliability (see Woods et al. , 2005) and construct validity (see Shapiro et al. , 1999). 2.3.1.7. Visual Memory. Deficits in encoding and retrieving the spatial image of the target may impact the target-directed walking tasks used to assess path inte- gration, as well as the visual distance perception task. To control for this, we used the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R; Benedict, 1997) to exam- ine visuospatial learning and memory. Patients were presented with an 8 × 11 inch page containing six geometric visual designs in a 2 × 3 array. The stimulus page was viewed for 10 s, after which the patient was instructed to reproduce as many designs in the correct locations as precisely possible. The total recall score, per-</p>
<p>J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 163 centage retained, and discrimination index were calculated in the same manner as the HVLT-R. Higher scores on all three measures indicate better visual learning and memory performance. Prior research has shown that the BVMT-R has good interform reliability and supports the construct and criterion-related validity (see Benedict et al. , 1996). 2.3.1.8. Visual Perception. Deficits in perceiving the initial target location may affect performance on the target-directed walking task. The Judgment of Line Ori- entation (JLOT; Benton et al. , 1978) was used to control for deficits in visual spatial processing. The JLOT requires little motor skill to complete, and is regarded as a “pure” measure of visual perception. This task required patients to match a pair of visually-presented angled lines to two of eleven numbered lines appearing as a semicircle. The number of correct responses out of the thirty items was totaled. Good performance on this test reflects good visual acuity, visual attention, and vi- suospatial processing. The JLOT has high test–retest reliability and demonstrable construct validity (see Benton et al. , 1978; see also Riccio and Hybd, 1992). 2.3.1.9. Working Memory. Our walking tasks required participants to attend to the stimuli (i.e., distance walked) while at the same time performing other men- tal tasks (i.e., maintain a four-digit number and the target location). Therefore, deficits in working memory and attentional capabilities may significantly decrease performance on the walking tasks. We used two subtests of the Wechsler Mem- ory Scale-III (WMS-III; Wechsler, 1997), digit span and spatial span, to examine working memory capacity. We specifically addressed the ability to concentrate on, hold, and manipulate both simple and complex information. The digit span subtest tests auditory working memory by requiring patients to repeat sequences of num- bers forward and backward. The spatial span subtest tests spatial sequence learning by requiring patients to reproduce a tapped sequence of block locations on a board both forward and backwards. The total number of items completed correctly on digit span and spatial span were determined. High test scores reflect good audi- tory and visual working memory capacity. Both subtests are reliable and valid (see WMS III Technical Manual, 1997). 2.3.1.10. Visual–Spatial Integration. The Hooper-Visual Organization Test (VOT; Hooper, 1958) was used to assess mental rotation, visual analysis and integration. Patients were presented with pictures of thirty common objects that had been cut into several pieces and placed in a random order on the card. Patients were required to identify and write the name of the original objects as if the pieces were assembled into one image. The number of correct responses out of the thirty items was totaled. Higher scores on this test reflect good visual spatial and perceptual abilities. The VOT possesses high interrater reliability and validity (see Lopez et al. , 2003). 2.3.1.11. Ataxia. A subset of the Scale for the Assessment and Rating of Ataxia (SARA; Schmitz-Hübsch et al. , 2006) and the Romberg Test Suite were used to quantify balance and visuomotor function. Four functional items from the SARA</p>
<p>164 J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 were assessed: gait, stance, sitting, and speech. To assess gait, participants were asked to: (1) walk at a safe distance parallel to a wall, (2) turn around to face the opposite direction of gait and (3) walk in tandem without support. To assess stance, participants were asked to stand: (1) in a natural position, (2) with feet together in parallel and (3) in tandem. For the stance test, three trials are allowed for each condition and the best trial was rated. On average, participants completed each con- dition in one trial. SARA scores are known to increase with the disease stage of ataxia (Schmitz-Hübsch et al. , 2006). The SARA has high interrater and test–retest reliability and high validity (Schmitz-Hübsch et al. , 2006). For the Romberg Test Suite, the three stance positions (as above) were repeated with eyes closed. Partici- pants were screened for truncal ataxia by sitting on an examination bed without the support of their feet, with their eyes open and arms stretched out in front of them. The experimenter stood nearby and observed if any difficulties were present in each task (i.e., staggering, intermittent or continuous sway and/or support). Speech was also assessed during normal conversation for signs of dysarthria. Behavior on each task was rated with a qualitative scale, where a low score (i.e., ‘0’) indicates no ob- servable difficulties or irregularities during performance (‘normal’) and a high score indicating a greater disturbances (e.g., a ‘6’ on the stance scale indicates ‘unable to stand for > 10 s even with the constant support of one arm’). 2.3.1.12. Self-Reported Handicap from Dizziness. The Dizziness Handicap In- ventory (DHI; Jacobson and Newman, 1990) is frequently used to evaluate the general ability of patients to cope with activities of daily life and quantify the self- perceived handicap (e.g., avoiding social situations, quitting their jobs) caused by vestibular symptoms. The DHI consists of 25 items with 3 subscales pertaining to the physical, functional, and emotional aspects associated with vertigo. The total score quantifies patients’ perception of disablement in everyday function. Scores range between 0 and 100: 0–30 suggests a mild handicap, 31–60 suggests a moder- ate handicap and 61–100 suggests severe handicap (physical, functional and emo- tional). The DHI has high concurrent validity and discriminate validity, and is able to detect statistically significant changes following interventions (Cattaneo et al. , 2006; Enloe and Shields, 1997; Jacobson and Newman, 1990). The DHI also has high test–retest reliability and moderate to high internal consistency (Cattaneo et al. , 2006; Enloe and Shields, 1997). 2.3.1.13. Level of Independence. The Vestibular Disorders Activities of Daily Living (VADL; Cohen and Kimball, 2000), assesses patients’ perception of inde- pendence on a much broader range of activities of daily life than the DHI (e.g., putting on shoes, reaching overhead, standing up, driving a car, etc.). The VADL consists of 28 items and a multilevel scale to rate self-efficacy on functional, am- bulatory, and instrumental tasks. Scale ratings range from 1 (independent) to 10 (disabled). The VADL has good face validity, high internal consistency, and high test–retest reliability.</p>
<p>J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 165 2.4. Statistical Analysis The walking and verbal data were analyzed in terms of three types of error: (1) vari- able error, as measured by the within-subject standard deviations (this provides a measure of response consistency, with high error indicating low consistency); (2) constant error (or ‘bias’), calculated as response value minus the physical target location (this provides a measure of the overall tendency to overshoot or under- shoot the physically accurate value); (3) absolute (unsigned) error, calculated as the absolute value of the walked response minus the physical target location (this provides a measure of the overall magnitude of errors, ignoring their direction). Participants’ errors were averaged across repetitions before analysis. Separate re- peated measures ANOVAs were conducted on the variable, constant, and absolute errors, with ‘Group’ as the between-subjects variable and ‘Target Distance’ (3 and 6 m) included as the within-subject variable. We report the exact p -values and mean squared errors (MSE) to give an indication of the effect size. The performance of the participants on the neuropsychological tests was com- pared in separate analyses of variance (ANOVAs). 3. Results There was no differences in age between groups ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 2 . 47; MSE = 296.93, p > 0 . 05). Likewise, we found no differences among the groups on any of the nine neuropsychological tests (separate ANOVAs, p > 0 . 05). Please see Table 2 for the individual test scores by group. 3.1. Verbal Estimates of Target Distance The mean indicated verbal estimates are plotted in Fig. 2. 3.1.1. Variable Error An ANOVA on the variable errors showed there was no reliable group differ- ence in consistency in the verbal distance estimation task ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 17 . 98; p = 0 . 887). There was a statistically significant effect of target distance on con- sistency ( F [ 1 , 12 ] = 10 . 59; MSE = 0.655; p = 0 . 007). However, we found no difference in interaction between group and verbal distance estimation ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 0 . 65; p = 0 . 539). The overall mean variable errors (averaged across distances) for Healthy Control, BPPV, and UVH groups were 0.32, 0.24 and 0.29 m, respectively. 3.1.2. Constant Error An ANOVA on the constant errors showed no effect of Group ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 0 . 23; p = 0 . 800), and no effect of Target Distance ( F [ 1 , 12 ] = 1 . 95; p = 0 . 188). There was no statistically significant Target Distance × Group interaction ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 0 . 01; p = 0 . 989). The overall mean constant errors for the Healthy Control, BPPV, and UVH group were − 0.41, − 0.42 and − 0.67 m, respectively, with corresponding between-subject standard errors 0.32, 0.36 and 0.30 m.</p>
<p>166 J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 Table 2. Demographic information and neuropsychological test scores for all participant groups a Healthy control b BPPV UVH Sex (M/F) 2 / 3 3 / 1 2 / 4 Age 41.4 (35–53) 59 (47–66) 56.7 (38–80) HVLT-R Recall (max = 36) 24.3 (22–26) 27.3 (21–33) 26.3 (20–31) Retained (%) 107 (100–111.1) 85 (66.7–100) 93.1 (28.6–112.5) Discrimination (max = 1) 0.95 (0.92–1) 0.94 (0.92–1) 0.96 (0.83–1) BVMT-R Recall (max = 36) 23.7 (15–32) 28.8 (25–33) 29.3 (16–32) Learning (max = 12) 3.7 (3–5) 5.5 (2–8) 3.8 (2–5) Retained (%) 122.3 (100–167) 95.5 (90–100) 90.5 (43–100) Recognition (max = 6) 6 6 c 5.7 (5–6) DHI d (max = 100) N/A 46 (32–62) 46.8 e (28–56) Physical (max = 28) N/A 9 (4–14) 19.2 (14–24) Emotional (max = 36) N/A 16 (12–20) 10 (0–16) Functional (max = 36) N/A 21 (14–32) 17.6 (14–22) VADL e, f (max = 280) N/A 101 (19–249) 91.2 d (62–119) Functional (max = 10) N/A 4.5 (1–9) 2.5 (1–4) Ambulatory (max = 10) N/A 4.5 (2–9) 2.9 (1–4) Instrumental (max = 10) N/A 5 (2–10) 3.3 (2–4) Digit span (max = 30) 18.7 (15–23) 21 (13–30) 18.5 (16–23) Spatial span (max = 32) 18.7 (18–20) 16.8 (14–20) 17 (9–20) JLOT (max = 30) 26.3 (24–28) 26.8 (23–28) 26.8 (21–30) VOT (max = 30) 24.7 (23.5–26.5) 26.4 (22–29) 26.9 (20–29) SARA d (max = 24) 0 0.3 (0–1) 0.83 (0–3) a Except where indicated, mean raw scores are presented. Values in parentheses denote the range; b n = 3 for neuropsychological battery; c n = 1; d total score; e n = 5; f median total and/or subscale scores are presented to account for omissions, extreme and/or not applicable rating. Figure 2. Mean indicated distance for the three groups of participants in verbal distance estimation trials. Error bars denote ± one standard error of the mean.</p>
<p>J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 167 3.1.3. Absolute Error An ANOVA on the absolute errors showed no effect of Group ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 0 . 12; p = 0 . 887). There was a significant effect of Target Distance on absolute error ( F [ 1 , 12 ] = 12 . 67; MSE = 2.623; p = 0 . 004). Results showed that this main effect was attributable to a larger amount of absolute error in the 6 m target distance than in the 3 m target distance, averaging 1.09 and 0.49 m, respectively. There was no statistically significant Target Distance × Group interaction ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 0 . 14; p = 0 . 277). The overall absolute mean error for the Healthy Control, BPPV and UVH group were 0.83, 0.82 and 0.73 m, respectively, with corresponding between- subject standard errors 0.17, 0.19 and 0.15 m. 3.2. Target-Directed Walking Estimates The mean walked responses are plotted in Fig. 3. 3.2.1. Response Time An ANOVA showed no effect of Group ( F [ 2 , 11 ] = 3 . 38; p = 0 . 072) on response time (s) to walk to target distances of 3 or 6 m (Table 3). Figure 3. Mean walked distances in the target-directed walking trials for the three groups of partici- pants. Error bars denote ± one standard error of the mean. Table 3. Mean response time as a function of stimulus distance in the target-directed walk- ing task Distance Participant group Healthy Control a BPPV b UVH c 3 m 3.5 (0.38) 3.5 (0.61) 4.7 (0.66) 6 m 6.0 (0.46) 7.0 (1.2) 8.0 c (1.6) Notes : Response times are expressed in seconds; values in parentheses denote the between-subjects standard error of the mean. a n = 5; b n = 4; c n = 5.</p>
<p>168 J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 (a) (b) Figure 4. Mean within-subject standard deviations in each stimulus condition, calculated across par- ticipants, in (a) target-directed walking and (b) experimenter-guided walking. Error bars denote ± one standard error of the mean. (a) (b) Figure 5. Mean constant (signed) error in each stimulus condition, calculated across participants, in (a) target-directed walking and (b) experimenter-guided walking. Error bars denote ± one standard error of the mean. 3.2.2. Variable Error The mean variable error for each stimulus distance in the target-directed walking task is plotted in Fig. 4(a). An ANOVA on the variable errors showed no effect of Group on mean variable error in the target-directed walking task ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 1 . 47; p = 0 . 268). There was no main effect of target distance ( F [ 1 , 12 ] = 1 . 69; p = 0 . 218). There was no statistically significant Target Distance × Group interac- tion ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 0 . 05; p = 0 . 956). The overall mean variable error for the Healthy Control, BPPV and UVH group were 0.28, 0.37 and 0.48 m, respectively, with cor- responding between-subject standard errors 0.09, 0.10 and 0.08 m. 3.2.3. Constant Error An ANOVA on the constant errors showed no effect of Group ( F [ 1 , 12 ] = 0 . 09; p = 0 . 916). There was a main effect of target distance ( F [ 1 , 12 ] = 9 . 60; MSE = 1.564; p = 0 . 009), with constant errors increasing in the larger stimulus distance. Participants tended to undershoot when attempting to walk to the 6 m target (Fig. 5(a)). There was no statistically significant Target Distance × Group interac- tion ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 0 . 21; p = 0 . 813). The overall mean constant error for the Healthy</p>
<p>J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 169 Control, BPPV and UVH groups were − 0.32, − 0.46 and 0.36 m, respectively, with corresponding between-subject standard errors 0.23, 0.25 and 0.21 m. 3.2.4. Absolute Error An ANOVA on the absolute errors showed no effect of Group ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 1 . 49; p = 0 . 264). There was a significant effect of Target Distance on overall error ( F [ 1 , 12 ] = 23 . 01; MSE = 1.176; p = 0 . 001). Results showed a larger amount of overall error in the 6 m target distance than in the 3 m target distance, averaging 0.78 and 0.38 m, respectively. There was no statistically significant Target Distance × Group interaction ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 1 . 75; p = 0 . 215). The overall mean absolute error for the Healthy Control, BPPV, and UVH groups were 0.45, 0.65 0.65 m, respec- tively, with corresponding between-subject errors 0.10, 0.11 and 0.09 m. 3.3. Experimenter-Guided Walking Estimates The mean indicated walked distances are plotted in Fig. 6. 3.3.1. Variable Error The mean variable error for each stimulus distance in the experimenter-guided walking task is plotted in Fig. 4(b). An ANOVA on the variable errors showed no effect of Group on mean variable error in the experimenter-guided walking task ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 3 . 24; p = 0 . 075). There was no main effect of stimulus distance on response consistency ( F [ 1 , 12 ] = 2 . 24; p = 0 . 160). There was no statistically sig- nificant Distance × Group interaction ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 0 . 113; p = 0 . 894). The overall mean variable error for the Healthy Control, BPPV and UVH groups were 0.54, 0.54 and 0.25 m, respectively, with corresponding between-subject standard errors 0.10, 0.11 and 0.09 m. 3.3.2. Constant Error An ANOVA on the mean constant errors showed a significant effect of Group ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 9 . 45; MSE = 10.997; p = 0 . 003). Pairwise planned contrasts (alpha = Figure 6. Mean indicated distances in the experimenter-guided walking trials for the three groups of participants. Error bars denote ± one standard error of the mean.</p>
<p>170 J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 0.05) were conducted to directly compare combinations of the groups’ overall er- ror. Results suggest that the effect is attributable to the tendency of the UVH group to underestimate the stimulus distance relative to the other two groups There was an effect of Distance ( F [ 1 , 12 ] = 14 . 89; MSE = 3.161; p = 0 . 002), with greater error for the 6 m stimulus distance. These effects were qualified by a significant interaction between Target Distance × Group ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 3 . 99; MSE = 0.848; p = 0 . 047). Pairwise planned contrasts (alpha = 0.05) were conducted in order to directly compare combinations of each group and each of the stimulus dis- tances so that we could further isolate the source of this interaction. Results showed that this interaction was driven by the UVH group significantly undershooting tar- get distances compared to the two control groups, with this tendency becoming more pronounced in experimenter-guided walking to the 6 m stimulus distance. The BPPV and Healthy control groups did not differ (see Fig. 5(b)). The overall mean constant error for the Healthy Control, BPPV and UVH group were − 0.02, − 0.04 and − 1.74 m, respectively, with corresponding between-subject standard er- rors 0.34, 0.38 and 0.31 m. 3.3.3. Absolute Error The results of analyses on the absolute error for experimenter-guided walking mirrored those of the constant error. An ANOVA on the absolute errors showed an effect of Group on the overall errors in the experimenter-guided walking task ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 10 . 38; MSE = 3.598; p = 0 . 002). Pairwise planned contrasts (alpha = 0.05) of the groups’ overall absolute errors showed that this main effect was primar- ily due to differences between the UVH group and the control groups. There was also a significant effect of Distance on overall error ( F [ 1 , 12 ] = 22 . 402; MSE = 3.716; p = 0 . 001). Results showed that this main effect was attributable to a larger amount of overall error in the 6 m stimulus distance than in the 3 m stimulus dis- tance, averaging 1.4 and 0.73 m, respectively. There was a statistically significant Distance × Group interaction ( F [ 2 , 12 ] = 3 . 94; MSE = 0.645; p = 0 . 048), with larger overall errors in the UVH group when walking to the 6 m stimulus distance than in the 3 m stimulus distance, averaging 2.38 m vs. 1.01 m, respectively. The overall mean variable errors for Healthy Control, BPPV and UVH groups were 0.32, 0.24 and 0.29 m, respectively. 4. Discussion The main result of this study is that, relative to normal controls and patients with BPPV, only patients with UVH show significant deficits in linear path integration on an experimenter-guided walking task. Without vision, UVH patients tended to underestimate the extent of their self-motion when guided along a linear path to an unknown target distance. By contrast, UVH patients performed quite accurately when walking without vision to a previously seen target location. When no loco- motion was required, UVH patients were able to accurately indicate the same target</p>
<p>J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 171 distances; thus, the underestimation in experimenter-guided walking was not sim- ply due to poor verbal calibration. These findings are in agreement with the emerging body of data that suggests non-visual walking deficits in peripheral vestibular defective patients are due to im- pairments in path integration (Borel et al. , 2004; Cohen, 2000; Cohen and Kimball, 2002; Péruch et al. , 1999, 2005). Moreover, we expand on previous findings in that our results suggest that this impairment stems from misperceptions in self-motion, which is attributable to the impaired vestibular afference in UVH (see Schubert and Minor, 2004, for a review of the neurophysiology underlying vestibular hypo- function). Comparable performance on the neuropsychological tasks indicated that cognitive deficits do not account for the systematic underestimation in self-motion. Also, the mean duration to complete the target-directed walking task was simi- lar across groups suggesting these differences were not due to differences in gait velocity, contrary to recent evidence (Cohen and Sangi-Haghpeykar, 2011). Our data complement previous findings that peripheral vestibular dysfunction induces an asymmetrical perception of self-orientation in external space during whole-body rotations (i.e., turn errors) (Borel et al. , 2008; Glasauer et al. , 2002; Péruch et al. , 2005). Taken together, these results highlight the important role of the vestibular system in path integration. Impairment in path integration requires teasing apart error arising from pro- cesses underlying self-motion sensing and updating from other related component processes (e.g., visuospatial processing, working memory). Multimodal (or cross- modal) tasks are useful for separating the various subcomponents of path integra- tion. In contrast to the simple walking task employed by Cohen (2000), in both of our walking tasks, the stimulus distance and response are presented in differ- ent modalities. In the target-directed walking task the stimulus distance is specified by direct visual perception of the target location, and distance is indicated using a walked response. In the experimenter-guided walking task, the stimulus distance is supplied via dynamic idiothetic information generated via locomotion (i.e., vestibu- lar, proprioceptive, kinesthetic inputs) and the response is given verbally. Unlike unimodal tasks, which may conceal systematic perceptual errors, multimodal tasks are more sensitive to systematic errors in path integration (Philbeck et al. , 2001, 2004; Sun et al. , 2004b). Our tasks required participants to (1) localize the initial target location (specifi- cally, the target-directed walking and verbal estimation tasks), (2) create and encode the spatial image (Loomis et al. , 2007) in working memory (target-directed and ver- bal estimation tasks) and (3) sense and integrate internally-generated self-motion signals (i.e., vestibular, proprioceptive, kinesthetic and efference copy) over time to yield an estimate of linear displacement (i.e., walked distance) relative to their starting point (target-directed and experimenter-guided walking tasks). Contribu- tions from each of these sub-processes can be identified by comparing performance using a variety of tasks that involve common subsets of each of the components. The verbal distance estimation task draws upon the first two sub-processes and</p>
<p>172 J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 does not require path integration. The fairly accurate verbal estimation of target distances by the UVH group (without locomotion) suggests that our finding of their underestimation of distance walked was not merely due to imprecise localization of target location caused by deficits in distance perception, and/or spatial memory, as described above in factors (1) and (2). UVH patients’ accurate performance on the target-directed walking task also suggests that there were no deficits in maintaining the target location in memory for short durations. Together, these results suggest the participants with UVH were able to localize the initial target location, and encode and maintain the spatial image in working memory (Loomis et al. , 2007). This sug- gests that the underestimation of walked distance found in the experimenter-guided walking task stems from a decreased ability to (3) sense and integrate internally- generated self-motion signals. If the misperception of self-motion during non-visual navigation underlies the systematic errors found in our experimenter-guided walking task, then we would expect similar errors to be manifest in the target-directed walking task. However, patients performed quite accurately on the target-directed walking task. One possi- ble explanation is related to the difference between active and passive locomotion. It has been shown that perception of self-motion is more accurate during active lo- comotion than passive for both angular and linear displacements (Féry et al. , 2004; Jurgens et al. , 1999; Philbeck et al. , 2001; Sun et al. , 2004a). This is reportedly due to differential processing for active versus passive motion signals at the secondary vestibular neurons (Cullen and Roy, 2004). Therefore, it is plausible that the two tasks employed in this study involved two different streams of vestibular process- ing. However, we feel our experimenter-guided walking task is not a passive task in the sense that although our participants did not have prior knowledge of the upcom- ing trajectory, our participants still had to generate a motor output to walk (Philbeck et al. , 2001); this is in contrast to a passive wheelchair transport, where only feed- back information about the movement is available (see Allen et al. , 2004). So, the distinction between active and passive locomotion by itself is not likely to provide a complete explanation of the differences in performance on the two walking tasks. Another non-exclusive hypothesis for the apparent discrepancy between the two tasks involves the response modes and the use of contextual information. Prior to walking in the target-directed task, participants were shown an actual target location. In contrast, for the experimenter-guided walking task, no information regarding the upcoming trajectory (i.e., walking distance) was given. Previous re- search has shown that vision of target locations and salient landmarks while viewing the environment provides contextual information for self-motion updating tasks (Arthur et al. , 2007, 2009; Philbeck et al. , 2001). It has been speculated that specif- ically the anticipation of actively walking to a predetermined location forms the basis of the spatiotemporal framework for incoming sensory signals during goal- directed linear locomotion (Philbeck et al. , 2001). We did not control for the po- tential use of visual cues. In fact, our test environment afforded participants with a highly structured context (well-lit hallway, pictures on the walls). Previous work</p>
<p>J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 173 has validated that previewed landmarks along linear and angular trajectories do fa- cilitate path integration, even when the observer cannot directly sense the objects as they pass (Arthur et al. , 2007, 2009; Israël et al. , 1996; Philbeck and O’Leary, 2005; Philbeck et al. , 2001). However, the contextual information was available to participants in both the target-directed and experimenter-guided tasks. What, then, can account for the differences in UVH patients’ performance on the two walking tasks? One likely explanation is the response phases in the two blind-walking tasks involved different modalities (verbal vs. walking). One major distinction between verbal and walking responses is the frame of reference under- lying the response modes. Verbal responses are egocentric-referenced in that there is no requirement to respond relative to an external location (e.g., target) in the environment. In contrast, walking responses are generally considered exocentric- referenced in that they are explicitly directed to remembered locations external to the body. The difference in the frame of references underlying responses in our two blind-walking tasks could have contributed to the self-motion estimation errors found in the UVH patients. Previous work by Borel et al. (2004) suggest that uni- lateral vestibular deficiencies can lead to high-level adaptive mechanisms, such as switching between exocentric and egocentric frames of reference depending on the availability of salient visual cues in the external environment. Moreover, recent find- ings in healthy participants show that the benefit of spatial memory is particularly likely in spatial updating tasks in which one’s self-location estimate is referenced to external space (Arthur et al. , submitted). In particular, UVH patients may have relied heavily on non-sensory inputs, such as memory of the surrounding environ- ment, as a strategy to compensate for the lack of peripheral vestibular afference in the target-directed walking non-visual task. In this task one’s self-location estimate must be referenced to external space (i.e., walking to a memorized target location), so spatial memory may be beneficial for updating. In contrast, in the experimenter- guided task, they may have abandoned such a strategy and relied on idiothethic cues, presumably because information about the external environment is no longer strictly required for the task. Instead, participants are required to estimate walked stimulus distance using verbal responses relative to a frame of reference centered on their own body. Therefore, in this task, participants may be less inclined to maintain and use the remembered spatial framework to keep track of their orientation during the body rotation. In fact, our finding of increased variability within the UVH group during the target-directed walking task compared to the experimenter-guided walk- ing task may be related to the varied use of such cognitive strategies (see Fig. 4). Thus, our interpretation is that accurate performance in the target-directed walking task may not necessarily imply accurate self-motion updating . Participants’ under- estimation of the extent of self-motion in the experimenter-guided walking task may be due to deficits in the sensing and integrating of self-motion signals. Importantly, the accurate performance during the target-directed walking task in patients with UVH suggests that their deficit does not affect their ability to compensate during active locomotion and walk to a memorized target.</p>
<p>174 J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 The use of adaptive mechanisms and strategies in unilateral vestibular patients has been noted by other researchers (Borel et al. , 2004, 2008; Péruch et al. , 1999). Péruch et al. (1999) tested eight patients with UVH (Ménière’s disease) before and after surgical treatment (vestibular neurotomy) on a non-visual walking task that required participants to navigate to memorized target locations. Interestingly, the experimenters found that patients performed better than the healthy controls, in terms of turn error and latency, before surgery. Additional studies showed that com- pared to controls, unilateral vestibular defective patients performed well on simple active blind-walking tasks (path reproduction and path reversal), yet exhibit linear and angular impairments on more complex navigation task (shortcutting) (Péruch et al. , 2005). The authors concluded that the proprioceptive cues compensated for vestibular loss during the simple active blind-walking task. This raises the issue that some tasks, such as the target-directed walking task, may be less sensitive to deficits in path integration. Together with our findings, these data suggest that the perfor- mance of patients with UVH is task-dependent. Accurate performance on certain goal-directed tasks may in fact be due to compensatory mechanisms. This process of compensation for abnormal vestibular input can be one of sensory substitution (e.g., proprioception, kinesthesia, efference copy) coupled with non-sensory (cog- nitive) strategies, such as the use of spatial memory, that can be used to substitute for abnormal vestibular afferent signals (Borel et al. , 2008). Further exploration on tasks that limit sensory input to vestibular information with a greater variety of stimulus distances would yield important insights into this issue. Notably, our healthy control group was strikingly accurate during the experi- menter-guided walking task, which contrasts with previous work (Peruch et al. , 2005; Philbeck et al. , 2004). Using similar blind-walking tasks, previous studies have shown an underestimation of linear displacement in neurologically-intact and UVH participants (Peruch et al. , 2005; Philbeck et al. , 2004). It is unclear from our study exactly what type of cues and/or cognitive strategies participants in the control group may have used to compensate for the lack of real-time visual input. Further investigation is required to fully illuminate this issue. To our knowledge, this is the first study to use an extensive neuropsychological battery to assess cognitive functions in conjunction with path integration for pa- tients with vestibular hypofunction. We found no evidence that patients with UVH are at a cognitive disadvantage; neither did their cognitive functioning explain dif- ficulty in estimating walked distance in the dark. However, the extent to which our preliminary results can be informative on the contribution of vestibular afferent sig- nals to self-motion sensing in normal and patient populations is limited. Admittedly, more extensive testing is required to confirm our current findings. Therefore, read- ers should be cautious about the extent to which these results from the two patient populations tested can be applied to the patient population at large. In the future, studies investigating the link between vestibular dysfunction and path integration deficits will need to focus on more standardized neuropsychological tests and non- visual walking tasks in a larger patient population with adequate control groups.</p>
<p>J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 175 Future studies should also test a broader range of stimulus and response distances to increase the sensitivity of detecting path integration deficits. Furthermore, a pas- sive wheelchair conveyance task (e.g., Allen et al. , 2004) with continuous blind pointing (e.g., Siegle et al. , 2009) to investigate linear path integration in UVH would corroborate these data. Acknowledgements This work has not been previously published elsewhere in any medium (paper or electronic), nor has the content been simultaneously submitted for publication in any form. This work was supported in part by NIH Postdoctoral Fellowship Grant to JCA and National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Dis- orders (K23-007926) Grant to MCS. The authors thank the anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments and contributions. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Michael C. Schubert at: Department of Otolaryngol- ogy, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA (e-mail: mschube1@jhmi.edu). References Allen, G. L., Kirasic, K. C. and Rashotte, M. A. (2004). Aging and path integration skill: kinesthetic and vestibular contributions to wayfinding, Percept. Psychophys. 66 , 170–179. Arthur, J. C., Philbeck, J. W. and Chichka, D. (2007). Spatial memory enhances the precision of angular self-motion updating, Exper. Brain Res. 183 , 557–568. Arthur, J. C., Philbeck, J. W. and Chichka, D. (2009). Non-sensory inputs to angular path integration, J. Vestib. Res. 19 , 111–125. Avni, R., Elkan, T., Dror, A. A., Shefer, S., Eilam, D., Avraham, K. B. and Mintz, M. (2009). Mice with vestibular deficiency display hyperactivity, disorientation and signs of anxiety, Behav. Brain Res. 202 , 210–217. Barlow, J. S. (1964). Inertial navigation as a basis for animal navigation, J. Theoret. Biol. 6 , 76–117. Benedict, R. H. B. (1997). Brief Visuospatial MemoryTest-Revised . Psychological Assessment Re- sources, Odessa, FL, USA. Benedict, R. H. B., Schretlen, D., Groninger, L., Dobraski, M. and Shpritz, B. (1996). Revision of the brief visuospatial memory test: studies of normal performance, reliability, and validity, Psycholog. Assess. 8 , 145–153. Benton, A. L., Varney, N. R. and Hamsher, K. S. (1978). Visuospatial judgement, Arch. Neurol. 35 , 364–367. Beritoff, J. (1965). Neural Mechanisms of Higher Vertebrate Behavior . Little, Brown, Boston, MA, USA. Borel, L., Harlay, F., Lopez, C., Magnan, J., Chays, A. and Lacour, M. (2004). Walking performance of vestibular-defective patients before and after unilateral vestibular neurotomy, Behav. Brain Res. 150 , 191–200. Borel, L., Lopez, C., Péruch, P. and Lacour, M. (2008). Vestibular syndrome: a change in internal spatial representation, Clin. Neurophysiol. 38 , 375–389. Brandt, J. and Benedict, R. H. B. (2001). Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R) . Psycho- logical Assessment Resources, Lutz, FL, USA.</p>
<p>176 J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 Cattaneo, D., Regola, A. and Meotti, M. (2006). Validity of six balance disorders scales in persons with multiple sclerosis, Disabil. Rehabilit. 28 , 789–795. Cohen, H. S. (2000). Vestibular disorders and impaired integration along a linear trajectory, J. Vestib. Res. 10 , 7–15. Cohen, H. S. and Kimball, K. T. (2000). Development of the vestibular disorders activities of daily living scale, Arch. Otolaryngol. — Head and Neck Surg. 126 , 881–887. Cohen, H. S. and Kimball, K. T. (2002). Improvements in path integration after vestibular rehabilita- tion, J. Vestib. Res. 12 , 47–51. Cohen, H. S. and Sangi-Haghpeykar, H. (2011). Walking speed and vestibular disorders in a path integration task, Gait Posture 33 , 211–213. Cullen, K. E. and Roy, J. E. (2004). Signal processing in the vestibular system during active versus passive head movements, J. Neurophysiol. 91 , 1919–1933. Enloe, L. J. and Shields, R. K. (1997). Evaluation of health-related quality of life in individuals with vestibular disease using disease-specific and general outcome measures, Phys. Therapy 77 , 890– 903. Etienne, A. S., Maurer, R. and Séguinot, V. (1996). Path integration in mammals and its interaction with visual landmarks, J. Exper. Biol. 199 , 201–209. Féry, Y.-A., Magnac, R. and Israël, I. (2004). Commanding the direction of passive whole-body rota- tions facilitates egocentric spatial updating, Cognition 91 , B1–B10. Glasauer S., Amorim, M. A., Viaud-Delmon, I. and Berthoz, A. (2002). Differential effects of labyrinthine dysfunction on distance and direction during blindfolded walking of a triangular path, Exper. Brain Res. 145 , 489–497. Grimm, R. J., Hemenway, W. G., Lebray, P. R. and Black, F. O. (1989). The perilymph fistula syn- drome defined in mild head trauma, Acta Otolaryngologica (Stockh) Supplementum 464 , 1–40. Guidetti, G., Monzani, D., Trebbi, M. and Rovatti, V. (2008). Impaired navigation skills in patients with psychological distress and chronic peripheral vestibular hypofunction without vertigo, Acta Otorhinolaryngologica Italica 28 , 21–25. Hanes, D. A. and McCollum, G. (2006). Cognitive–vestibular interactions: a review of patient diffi- culties and possible mechanisms, J. Vestib. Res. 16 , 75–91. Herdman, S. J. and Whitney, S. L. (2000). Treatment of vestibular hypofunction, in: Vestibular reha- bilitation , S. L. Wolf (Ed.), pp. 387–423. F.A. Davis, Philadelphia, PA, USA. Highstein, S. M. (1996). How does the vestibular part of the inner ear work?, in: Disorders of the Vestibular System , R. W. Baloh and G. M. Halmagyi (Eds), pp. 3–11. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK. Hillier, S. L. and Hollohan, V. (2007). Vestibular rehabilitation for unilateral peripheral vestibular dysfunction, Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 4 , CD005397. Hilton, M. and Pinder, D. (2004). The Epley (canalith repositioning) manoeuvre for benign paroxys- mal positional vertigo, Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2 , CD003162. Hooper, H. E. (1958). The Hooper Visual Organization Test Manual . Western Psychological Services, Los Angeles, CA, USA. Israël, I. and Berthoz, A. (1989). Contribution of the otoliths to the calculation of linear displacement, J. Neurophysiol. 62 , 247–263. Israël, I., Bronstein, A. M., Kanayama, R., Faldon, M. and Gresty, M. A. (1996). Visual and vestibular factors influencing vestibular ‘navigation’, Exper. Brain Res. 112 , 411–419. Israël, I., Grasso, R., Georges-François, P., Tsuzuku, T. and Berthoz, A. (1997). Spatial memory and path integration studied by self-driven passive linear displacement. I. Basic properties, J. Neuro- physiol. 77 , 3180–3192.</p>
<p>J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 177 Jacobson, G. P., Hunter, L. and Balzer, G. (1991). Balance function test correlates of the dizziness handicap inventory, J. Amer. Acad. Audiol. 2 , 253–260. Jacobson, G. P. and Newman, C. W. (1990). The development of the dizziness handicap inventory, Arch. Otolaryngol. — Head and Neck Surg. 116 , 424–427. Jurgens, R., Boss, T. and Becker, W. (1999). Estimation of self-turning in the dark: comparison be- tween active and passive rotation, Exper. Brain Res. 128 , 491–504. Kallie, C. S., Schrater, P. R. and Legge, G. E. (2007). Variability in stepping direction explains the veering behavior of blind walkers, J. Exper. Psychol.: Human Percept. Perform. 33 , 183–200. Loomis, J. M., Da Silva, J. A., Fujita, N. and Fukusima, S. S. (1992). Visual space perception and visually directed action, J. Exper. Psychol.: Human Percept. Perform. 18 , 906–921. Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., Avraamides, M., Lippa, Y. and Golledge, R. G. (2007). Functional equivalence of spatial images produced by perception and spatial language, in: Spatial Processing in Navigation, Imagery and Perception , F. Mast and L. Jäncke (Eds), pp. 29–48. Springer, New York, NY, USA. Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., Golledge, R. G. and Philbeck, J. W. (1999). Human navigation by path integration, in: Wayfinding Behavior: Cognitive Mapping and Other Spatial Processes , R. G. Golledge (Ed.), pp. 125–152. John Hopkins Press, Baltimore, MD, USA. Lopez, M. N., Lazar, M. D. and Oh, S. (2003). Psychometric properties of the Hooper Visual Orien- tation Test, Assessment 10 , 66–70. Mittelstaedt, M. L. and Mittelstaedt, H. (1980). Homing by path integration in a mammal, Naturwis- senschaften 67 , 566–567. Mittelstaedt, M. L. and Mittelstaedt, H. (2001). Idiothetic navigation in humans: estimation of path length, Exper. Brain Res. 139 , 318–332. Péruch, P., Borel, L., Gaunet, F., Thinus-Blanc, G., Magnan, J. and Lacour, M. (1999). Spatial perfor- mance of unilateral vestibular defective patients in nonvisual vs. visual navigation, J. Vestib. Res. 9 , 37–47. Péruch, P., Borel, L., Magnan, J. and Lacour, M. (2005). Direction and distance deficits in path inte- gration after unilateral vestibular loss depend on task complexity, Cognit. Brain Res. 25 , 862–872. Philbeck, J. W., Behrmann, M., Levy, L., Potolicchio Jr., S. J. and Caputy, A. J. (2004). Path integration deficits during linear locomotion after human medial temporal lobectomy, J. Cognit. Neurosci. 16 , 510–520. Philbeck, J. W., Klatzky, R., Behrmann, M., Loomis, J. and Goodridge, J. (2001). Active control of locomotion facilitates nonvisual navigation, J. Exper. Psychol.: Human Percept. Perform. 27 , 141– 153. Philbeck, J. W. and Loomis, J. M. (1997). Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions, J. Exper. Psychol.: Human Percept. Perform. 23 , 72–85. Philbeck, J. W. and O’Leary, S. (2005). Remembered landmarks enhance the precision of path inte- gration, Psicologica 26 , 7–24. Riccio, C. A. and Hybd, G. W. (1992). Validity of Benton’s judgment of line orientation test, J. Psy- choeducat. Assess. 10 , 210–218. Russell, N. A., Horii, A., Liu, P., Smith, P. F., Darlington, C. L. and Bilkey, D. K. (2003). Bilateral peripheral vestibular lesions produce long-term changes in spatial learning in the rat, J. Vestib. Res. 13 , 9–16. Schmitz-Hübsch, T., du Montcel, S. T., Baliko, L., Berciano, J., Boesch, S., Depondt, C., Giunti, P., Globas, C., Infante, J. and Kang, J. S. (2006). Scale for the assessment and rating of ataxia: devel- opment of a new clinical scale, Neurology 66 , 1717–1720.</p>
<p>178 J. C. Arthur et al. / Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012) 155–178 Schubert, M. C. and Minor, L. B. (2004). Vestibulo-ocular physiology underlying vestibular hypo- function, Phys. Therapy 84 , 373–385. Shapiro, A. M., Benedict, R. H. B., Schretlen, D. and Brandt, J. (1999). Construct and concurrent validity of the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised, Clin. Neuropsychol. 13 , 348–358. Siegle, J. H., Campos, J. L, Mohler, B. J., Loomis, J. M. and Bülthoff, H. H. (2009). Measurement of instantaneous perceived self-motion using continuous pointing, Exper. Brain Res. 195 , 429–444. Smith, P. F., Darlington, C. L. and Zheng, Y. (2010). Move it or lose it: is stimulation of the vestibular system necessary for normal spatial memory? Hippocampus 20 , 36–43. Smith, P. F., Zheng, Y., Horii, A. and Darlington, C. L. (2005). Does vestibular damage cause cognitive dysfunction in humans? J. Vestib. Res. 15 , 1–9. Stackman, R. W. and Herbert, A. M. (2002). Rats with lesions of the vestibular system require a visual landmark for spatial navigation, Behav. Brain Res. 128 , 27–40. Sun, H.-J., Campos, J. L. and Chan, G. S. (2004a). Multisensory integration in the estimation of relative path length, Exper. Brain Res. 154 , 246–254. Sun, H.-J., Campos, J. L., Young, M., Chan, G. S. and Ellard, C. G. (2004b). The contributions of static visual cues, nonvisual cues, and optic flow in distance estimation, Perception 33 , 49–65. Thomson, J. A. (1983). Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion?, J. Exper. Psychol.: Human Percept. Perform. 9 , 427–443. Wallace, D. G., Hines, D. J., Pellis, S. M. and Whishaw, I. Q. (2002). Vestibular information is required for dead reckoning in the rat, J. Neurol. 22 , 10009–10017. Weschler, D. (1997). Weschler Memory Scale — III Manual . Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX, USA. Woods, S. P., Scott, J. C., Conover, E., Marcotte, T. D. and Heaton, R. K. (2005). Test–retest reliability of component process variables within the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised, Assessment 12 , 96–100. Zheng, Y., Darlington, C. L. and Smith, P. F. (2006). Impairment and recovery on a food foraging task following unilateral vestibular deafferentation in rat, Hippocampus 16 , 368–378. Zheng, Y., Goddard, M., Darlington, C. L. and Smith, P. F. (2009). Long-term deficits on a foraging task after bilateral vestibular deafferentation in rats, Hippocampus 19 , 480–486.</p>
</body>
</article>
</istex:document>
</istex:metadataXml>
<mods version="3.6">
<titleInfo>
<title>Linear Path Integration Deficits in Patients with Abnormal Vestibular Afference</title>
</titleInfo>
<titleInfo type="alternative" contentType="CDATA">
<title>Linear Path Integration Deficits in Patients with Abnormal Vestibular Afference</title>
</titleInfo>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Michael C.</namePart>
<namePart type="family">Schubert</namePart>
<role>
<roleTerm type="text">author</roleTerm>
</role>
</name>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Joeanna C.</namePart>
<namePart type="family">Arthur</namePart>
<role>
<roleTerm type="text">author</roleTerm>
</role>
</name>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Mark</namePart>
<namePart type="family">Shelhamer</namePart>
<role>
<roleTerm type="text">author</roleTerm>
</role>
</name>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Kathleen B.</namePart>
<namePart type="family">Kortte</namePart>
<role>
<roleTerm type="text">author</roleTerm>
</role>
</name>
<typeOfResource>text</typeOfResource>
<genre type="research-article" displayLabel="research-article"></genre>
<originInfo>
<publisher>BRILL</publisher>
<place>
<placeTerm type="text">The Netherlands</placeTerm>
</place>
<dateIssued encoding="w3cdtf">2012</dateIssued>
<dateCreated encoding="w3cdtf">2012</dateCreated>
</originInfo>
<language>
<languageTerm type="code" authority="iso639-2b">eng</languageTerm>
<languageTerm type="code" authority="rfc3066">en</languageTerm>
</language>
<physicalDescription>
<internetMediaType>text/html</internetMediaType>
</physicalDescription>
<subject>
<genre>Keywords</genre>
<topic>path integration</topic>
<topic>Vestibular navigation</topic>
<topic>spatial orientation</topic>
<topic>vestibular hypofunction</topic>
</subject>
<relatedItem type="host">
<titleInfo>
<title>Seeing and Perceiving</title>
<subTitle>A Journal of Multisensory Science</subTitle>
</titleInfo>
<titleInfo type="abbreviated">
<title>SP</title>
</titleInfo>
<genre type="Journal">journal</genre>
<identifier type="ISSN">1878-4755</identifier>
<identifier type="eISSN">1878-4763</identifier>
<part>
<date>2012</date>
<detail type="volume">
<caption>vol.</caption>
<number>25</number>
</detail>
<detail type="issue">
<caption>no.</caption>
<number>2</number>
</detail>
<extent unit="pages">
<start>155</start>
<end>178</end>
</extent>
</part>
</relatedItem>
<identifier type="istex">1CF21DAD37B8234A4A160C65709A413B551FE25B</identifier>
<identifier type="DOI">10.1163/187847612X629928</identifier>
<identifier type="href">18784763_025_02_s003_text.pdf</identifier>
<accessCondition type="use and reproduction" contentType="copyright">© Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, The Netherlands</accessCondition>
<recordInfo>
<recordContentSource>BRILL Journals</recordContentSource>
<recordOrigin>Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, The Netherlands</recordOrigin>
</recordInfo>
</mods>
</metadata>
<serie></serie>
</istex>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Ticri/CIDE/explor/HapticV1/Data/Istex/Corpus
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 003150 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Istex/Corpus/biblio.hfd -nk 003150 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Ticri/CIDE
   |area=    HapticV1
   |flux=    Istex
   |étape=   Corpus
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     ISTEX:1CF21DAD37B8234A4A160C65709A413B551FE25B
   |texte=   Linear Path Integration Deficits in Patients with Abnormal Vestibular Afference
}}

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.23.
Data generation: Mon Jun 13 01:09:46 2016. Site generation: Wed Mar 6 09:54:07 2024