Serveur d'exploration SRAS

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

Risk Perceptions Related to SARS and Avian Influenza: Theoretical Foundations of Current Empirical Research

Identifieur interne : 000543 ( Pmc/Corpus ); précédent : 000542; suivant : 000544

Risk Perceptions Related to SARS and Avian Influenza: Theoretical Foundations of Current Empirical Research

Auteurs : Anja Leppin ; Arja R. Aro

Source :

RBID : PMC:7090865

Abstract

Background

The outbreak of severe acute respiratory syndrome in 2003 and the subsequent emergence of the H5N1 virus have highlighted the threat of a global pandemic influenza outbreak. Planning effective public health control measures for such a case will be highly dependent on sound theory-based research on how people perceive the risks involved in such an event.

Purpose

The present article aims to review theoretical models and concepts underlying current empirical research on pandemic influenza risk perception.

Method

A review was conducted based on 28 empirical studies from 30 articles which were published between 2003 and 2007.

Results

Concepts of risk perception mostly seemed more pragmatic than theory-based and were highly heterogeneous, for instance, in terms of conceptualizing risk perception as an exclusively cognitive or as a cognitive and emotional phenomenon or whether the concept was dominated by expectancy or expectancy and value components. Similarly, the majority of studies investigating risk perceptions and protective behaviors were not model-based.

Conclusions

The current body of knowledge can only provide preliminary insights. Unlike the reviewed studies, which were mostly launched as a rapid response to outbreak situations, future research will have to invest more strongly into theoretical work to provide sounder evidence.


Url:
DOI: 10.1007/s12529-008-9002-8
PubMed: 19214752
PubMed Central: 7090865

Links to Exploration step

PMC:7090865

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI>
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">Risk Perceptions Related to SARS and Avian Influenza: Theoretical Foundations of Current Empirical Research</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leppin, Anja" sort="Leppin, Anja" uniqKey="Leppin A" first="Anja" last="Leppin">Anja Leppin</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="Aff1"></nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aro, Arja R" sort="Aro, Arja R" uniqKey="Aro A" first="Arja R." last="Aro">Arja R. Aro</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="Aff1"></nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">PMC</idno>
<idno type="pmid">19214752</idno>
<idno type="pmc">7090865</idno>
<idno type="url">http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7090865</idno>
<idno type="RBID">PMC:7090865</idno>
<idno type="doi">10.1007/s12529-008-9002-8</idno>
<date when="2009">2009</date>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Corpus">000543</idno>
<idno type="wicri:explorRef" wicri:stream="Pmc" wicri:step="Corpus" wicri:corpus="PMC">000543</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title xml:lang="en" level="a" type="main">Risk Perceptions Related to SARS and Avian Influenza: Theoretical Foundations of Current Empirical Research</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leppin, Anja" sort="Leppin, Anja" uniqKey="Leppin A" first="Anja" last="Leppin">Anja Leppin</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="Aff1"></nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aro, Arja R" sort="Aro, Arja R" uniqKey="Aro A" first="Arja R." last="Aro">Arja R. Aro</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="Aff1"></nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<series>
<title level="j">International Journal of Behavioral Medicine</title>
<idno type="ISSN">1070-5503</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1532-7558</idno>
<imprint>
<date when="2009">2009</date>
</imprint>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">
<sec>
<title>Background</title>
<p>The outbreak of severe acute respiratory syndrome in 2003 and the subsequent emergence of the H5N1 virus have highlighted the threat of a global pandemic influenza outbreak. Planning effective public health control measures for such a case will be highly dependent on sound theory-based research on how people perceive the risks involved in such an event.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Purpose</title>
<p>The present article aims to review theoretical models and concepts underlying current empirical research on pandemic influenza risk perception.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Method</title>
<p>A review was conducted based on 28 empirical studies from 30 articles which were published between 2003 and 2007.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Results</title>
<p>Concepts of risk perception mostly seemed more pragmatic than theory-based and were highly heterogeneous, for instance, in terms of conceptualizing risk perception as an exclusively cognitive or as a cognitive
<italic>and</italic>
emotional phenomenon or whether the concept was dominated by expectancy or expectancy
<italic>and</italic>
value components. Similarly, the majority of studies investigating risk perceptions and protective behaviors were not model-based.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Conclusions</title>
<p>The current body of knowledge can only provide preliminary insights. Unlike the reviewed studies, which were mostly launched as a rapid response to outbreak situations, future research will have to invest more strongly into theoretical work to provide sounder evidence.</p>
</sec>
</div>
</front>
<back>
<div1 type="bibliography">
<listBibl>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, Rd" uniqKey="Smith R">RD Smith</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Savage, L" uniqKey="Savage L">L Savage</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fischhoff, B" uniqKey="Fischhoff B">B Fischhoff</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Slovic, P" uniqKey="Slovic P">P Slovic</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lichtenstein, S" uniqKey="Lichtenstein S">S Lichtenstein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Read, S" uniqKey="Read S">S Read</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Combs, B" uniqKey="Combs B">B Combs</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Slovic, P" uniqKey="Slovic P">P Slovic</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rosenstock, Im" uniqKey="Rosenstock I">IM Rosenstock</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Becker, Mh" uniqKey="Becker M">MH Becker</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rosenstock, Im" uniqKey="Rosenstock I">IM Rosenstock</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rogers, R" uniqKey="Rogers R">R Rogers</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rogers, R" uniqKey="Rogers R">R Rogers</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chaiken, S" uniqKey="Chaiken S">S Chaiken</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Petty, R" uniqKey="Petty R">R Petty</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cacioppo, J" uniqKey="Cacioppo J">J Cacioppo</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Damasio, Ar" uniqKey="Damasio A">AR Damasio</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Forgas, Jp" uniqKey="Forgas J">JP Forgas</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Forgas, Jp" uniqKey="Forgas J">JP Forgas</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schwarz, N" uniqKey="Schwarz N">N Schwarz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Finucane, Ml" uniqKey="Finucane M">ML Finucane</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Holup, Jl" uniqKey="Holup J">JL Holup</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Finucane, Ml" uniqKey="Finucane M">ML Finucane</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Peters, E" uniqKey="Peters E">E Peters</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Slovic, P" uniqKey="Slovic P">P Slovic</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Loewenstein, Gf" uniqKey="Loewenstein G">GF Loewenstein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weber, Eu" uniqKey="Weber E">EU Weber</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hsee, Ck" uniqKey="Hsee C">CK Hsee</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Welch, N" uniqKey="Welch N">N Welch</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Finucane, Ml" uniqKey="Finucane M">ML Finucane</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Alkahani, A" uniqKey="Alkahani A">A Alkahani</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Slovic, P" uniqKey="Slovic P">P Slovic</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnson, Sm" uniqKey="Johnson S">SM Johnson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Slovic, P" uniqKey="Slovic P">P Slovic</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Finucane, Ml" uniqKey="Finucane M">ML Finucane</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Peters, E" uniqKey="Peters E">E Peters</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Macgregor, Dg" uniqKey="Macgregor D">DG MacGregor</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lazarus, R" uniqKey="Lazarus R">R Lazarus</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Folkman, S" uniqKey="Folkman S">S Folkman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Roseman, Ij" uniqKey="Roseman I">IJ Roseman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Antoniou, Aa" uniqKey="Antoniou A">AA Antoniou</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jose, Pe" uniqKey="Jose P">PE Jose</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Irwin, A" uniqKey="Irwin A">A Irwin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wynne, B" uniqKey="Wynne B">B Wynne</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Moscovici, S" uniqKey="Moscovici S">S Moscovici</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kasperson, R" uniqKey="Kasperson R">R Kasperson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kasperson, J" uniqKey="Kasperson J">J Kasperson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct></biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brewer, Nt" uniqKey="Brewer N">NT Brewer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weinstein, Nd" uniqKey="Weinstein N">ND Weinstein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cuite, Cl" uniqKey="Cuite C">CL Cuite</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Herrington, Je" uniqKey="Herrington J">JE Herrington</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brewer, Nt" uniqKey="Brewer N">NT Brewer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chapman, Gb" uniqKey="Chapman G">GB Chapman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gibbons, Fx" uniqKey="Gibbons F">FX Gibbons</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gerrard Mccaul, Kd" uniqKey="Gerrard Mccaul K">KD Gerrard McCaul</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weinstein, Nd" uniqKey="Weinstein N">ND Weinstein</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ajzen, I" uniqKey="Ajzen I">I Ajzen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fishbein, M" uniqKey="Fishbein M">M Fishbein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ajzen, I" uniqKey="Ajzen I">I Ajzen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brown, Sl" uniqKey="Brown S">SL Brown</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Eiser, Jr" uniqKey="Eiser J">JR Eiser</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Miles, S" uniqKey="Miles S">S Miles</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Frewer, Lj" uniqKey="Frewer L">LJ Frewer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cava, Ma" uniqKey="Cava M">MA Cava</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fay, Ke" uniqKey="Fay K">KE Fay</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Beanlands, Hj" uniqKey="Beanlands H">HJ Beanlands</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mccay, Ea" uniqKey="Mccay E">EA McCay</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wignall, R" uniqKey="Wignall R">R Wignall</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rambaldini, G" uniqKey="Rambaldini G">G Rambaldini</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wilson, K" uniqKey="Wilson K">K Wilson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rath, D" uniqKey="Rath D">D Rath</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lin, Y" uniqKey="Lin Y">Y Lin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gold, Wl" uniqKey="Gold W">WL Gold</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kapral, Mk" uniqKey="Kapral M">MK Kapral</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chong, M Y" uniqKey="Chong M">M-Y Chong</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wang, W C" uniqKey="Wang W">W-C Wang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hsieh, W C" uniqKey="Hsieh W">W-C Hsieh</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lee, C Y" uniqKey="Lee C">C-Y Lee</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chiu, N M" uniqKey="Chiu N">N-M Chiu</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yeh, W C" uniqKey="Yeh W">W-C Yeh</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Imai, T" uniqKey="Imai T">T Imai</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Takahashi, K" uniqKey="Takahashi K">K Takahashi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hoshuyama, T" uniqKey="Hoshuyama T">T Hoshuyama</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hasegawa, N" uniqKey="Hasegawa N">N Hasegawa</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lim, M K" uniqKey="Lim M">M-K Lim</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Koh, D" uniqKey="Koh D">D Koh</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tam, Dkp" uniqKey="Tam D">DKP Tam</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lee, S" uniqKey="Lee S">S Lee</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lee, Ss" uniqKey="Lee S">SS Lee</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shiao, Js C" uniqKey="Shiao J">JS-C Shiao</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Koh, D" uniqKey="Koh D">D Koh</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lo, L H" uniqKey="Lo L">L-H Lo</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lim, M K" uniqKey="Lim M">M-K Lim</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Guo, L" uniqKey="Guo L">L Guo</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tang, Csk" uniqKey="Tang C">CSK Tang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wong, C Y" uniqKey="Wong C">C-Y Wong</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tang, Csk" uniqKey="Tang C">CSK Tang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wong, C Y" uniqKey="Wong C">C-Y Wong</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dezwart, O" uniqKey="Dezwart O">O deZwart</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Veldhuijzen, Ik" uniqKey="Veldhuijzen I">IK Veldhuijzen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Elam, G" uniqKey="Elam G">G Elam</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aro, Ar" uniqKey="Aro A">AR Aro</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Abraham, T" uniqKey="Abraham T">T Abraham</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bishop, Gd" uniqKey="Bishop G">GD Bishop</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lau, Jtf" uniqKey="Lau J">JTF Lau</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yang, X" uniqKey="Yang X">X Yang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wong, E" uniqKey="Wong E">E Wong</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tsui, H" uniqKey="Tsui H">H Tsui</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ji, L J" uniqKey="Ji L">L-J Ji</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zhang, Z" uniqKey="Zhang Z">Z Zhang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Usborne, E" uniqKey="Usborne E">E Usborne</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Guan, Y" uniqKey="Guan Y">Y Guan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fielding, R" uniqKey="Fielding R">R Fielding</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lam, Ww" uniqKey="Lam W">WW Lam</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ho, Ey" uniqKey="Ho E">EY Ho</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lam, H" uniqKey="Lam H">H Lam</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hedley, Aj" uniqKey="Hedley A">AJ Hedley</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leung, Gm" uniqKey="Leung G">GM Leung</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brug, J" uniqKey="Brug J">J Brug</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aro, Ar" uniqKey="Aro A">AR Aro</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Oenema, A" uniqKey="Oenema A">A Oenema</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Zwart, O" uniqKey="De Zwart O">O de Zwart</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Richardus, Jh" uniqKey="Richardus J">JH Richardus</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bishop, Gd" uniqKey="Bishop G">GD Bishop</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leung, Gm" uniqKey="Leung G">GM Leung</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ho, L M" uniqKey="Ho L">L-M Ho</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chan, Skk" uniqKey="Chan S">SKK Chan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ho, S Y" uniqKey="Ho S">S-Y Ho</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bacon Shone, J" uniqKey="Bacon Shone J">J Bacon-Shone</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Choy, Ryl" uniqKey="Choy R">RYL Choy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nickell, La" uniqKey="Nickell L">LA Nickell</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Crighton, Ej" uniqKey="Crighton E">EJ Crighton</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tracy, Cs" uniqKey="Tracy C">CS Tracy</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Al Enazy, H" uniqKey="Al Enazy H">H Al-Enazy</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bolaji, Y" uniqKey="Bolaji Y">Y Bolaji</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hanjrah, S" uniqKey="Hanjrah S">S Hanjrah</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kristiansen, Is" uniqKey="Kristiansen I">IS Kristiansen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Halvorsen, Pa" uniqKey="Halvorsen P">PA Halvorsen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gyrd Hansen, D" uniqKey="Gyrd Hansen D">D Gyrd-Hansen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lau, Jtf" uniqKey="Lau J">JTF Lau</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yang, X" uniqKey="Yang X">X Yang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tsui, Hy" uniqKey="Tsui H">HY Tsui</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pang, E" uniqKey="Pang E">E Pang</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leung, Gm" uniqKey="Leung G">GM Leung</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Quah, S" uniqKey="Quah S">S Quah</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ho, L M" uniqKey="Ho L">L-M Ho</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ho, S Y" uniqKey="Ho S">S-Y Ho</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hedley, Aj" uniqKey="Hedley A">AJ Hedley</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lee, H P" uniqKey="Lee H">H-P Lee</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Abbate, R" uniqKey="Abbate R">R Abbate</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Di Giuseppe, G" uniqKey="Di Giuseppe G">G Di Giuseppe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Marinelli, P" uniqKey="Marinelli P">P Marinelli</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Angelillo, If" uniqKey="Angelillo I">IF Angelillo</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lau, Jtf" uniqKey="Lau J">JTF Lau</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yang, X" uniqKey="Yang X">X Yang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tsui, Hy" uniqKey="Tsui H">HY Tsui</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pang, E" uniqKey="Pang E">E Pang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kim, Jh" uniqKey="Kim J">JH Kim</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leung, Gm" uniqKey="Leung G">GM Leung</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lam, T H" uniqKey="Lam T">T-H Lam</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ho, L M" uniqKey="Ho L">L-M Ho</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ho, S Y" uniqKey="Ho S">S-Y Ho</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chan, Bhy" uniqKey="Chan B">BHY Chan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wong, Iol" uniqKey="Wong I">IOL Wong</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hong, S" uniqKey="Hong S">S Hong</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Collins, A" uniqKey="Collins A">A Collins</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Koh, D" uniqKey="Koh D">D Koh</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lim, Mk" uniqKey="Lim M">MK Lim</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chia, Se" uniqKey="Chia S">SE Chia</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ko, Sm" uniqKey="Ko S">SM Ko</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Qian, F" uniqKey="Qian F">F Qian</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ng, V" uniqKey="Ng V">V Ng</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Koh, D" uniqKey="Koh D">D Koh</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Takahashi, K" uniqKey="Takahashi K">K Takahashi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lim, Mk" uniqKey="Lim M">MK Lim</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Imai, T" uniqKey="Imai T">T Imai</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chia, Se" uniqKey="Chia S">SE Chia</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Qian, F" uniqKey="Qian F">F Qian</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lau, Jtf" uniqKey="Lau J">JTF Lau</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yang, X" uniqKey="Yang X">X Yang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tsui, H" uniqKey="Tsui H">H Tsui</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kim, Jh" uniqKey="Kim J">JH Kim</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chang, C S" uniqKey="Chang C">C-S Chang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Du, P L" uniqKey="Du P">P-L Du</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Huang, I C" uniqKey="Huang I">I-C Huang</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weinstein, Nd" uniqKey="Weinstein N">ND Weinstein</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bandura, A" uniqKey="Bandura A">A Bandura</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weinstein, Nd" uniqKey="Weinstein N">ND Weinstein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sandman, Pm" uniqKey="Sandman P">PM Sandman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, Vk" uniqKey="Smith V">VK Smith</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnson, Fr" uniqKey="Johnson F">FR Johnson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Viscusi, Wk" uniqKey="Viscusi W">WK Viscusi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Evans, Wn" uniqKey="Evans W">WN Evans</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tang, Csk" uniqKey="Tang C">CSK Tang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wong, C Y" uniqKey="Wong C">C-Y Wong</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kobbeltveld, T" uniqKey="Kobbeltveld T">T Kobbeltveld</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brun, W" uniqKey="Brun W">W Brun</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnsen, Bh" uniqKey="Johnsen B">BH Johnsen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Eid, J" uniqKey="Eid J">J Eid</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Borkovec, Td" uniqKey="Borkovec T">TD Borkovec</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Robinson, E" uniqKey="Robinson E">E Robinson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pruzinsky, T" uniqKey="Pruzinsky T">T Pruzinsky</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Depree, Ja" uniqKey="Depree J">JA DePree</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Morris, Lw" uniqKey="Morris L">LW Morris</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Davis, Ma" uniqKey="Davis M">MA Davis</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hutchings, Ch" uniqKey="Hutchings C">CH Hutchings</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Harris, P" uniqKey="Harris P">P Harris</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Middleton, W" uniqKey="Middleton W">W Middleton</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Siu, Jy M" uniqKey="Siu J">JY-M Siu</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct></biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lai, Jc" uniqKey="Lai J">JC Lai</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tao, J" uniqKey="Tao J">J Tao</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Beck, U" uniqKey="Beck U">U Beck</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Beck, U" uniqKey="Beck U">U Beck</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wu, Jt" uniqKey="Wu J">JT Wu</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Riley, S" uniqKey="Riley S">S Riley</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fraser, C" uniqKey="Fraser C">C Fraser</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leung, Gm" uniqKey="Leung G">GM Leung</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weber, Eu" uniqKey="Weber E">EU Weber</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hsee, Ck" uniqKey="Hsee C">CK Hsee</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct></biblStruct>
<biblStruct></biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Moscona, A" uniqKey="Moscona A">A Moscona</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Moscona, A" uniqKey="Moscona A">A Moscona</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Singer, Ac" uniqKey="Singer A">AC Singer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nunn, Ma" uniqKey="Nunn M">MA Nunn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gould, Ea" uniqKey="Gould E">EA Gould</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnson, Ac" uniqKey="Johnson A">AC Johnson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barennes, H" uniqKey="Barennes H">H Barennes</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Martinez Aussel, B" uniqKey="Martinez Aussel B">B Martinez-Aussel</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vongphrachanh, P" uniqKey="Vongphrachanh P">P Vongphrachanh</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Strobel, M" uniqKey="Strobel M">M Strobel</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Peltz, R" uniqKey="Peltz R">R Peltz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Avisar Shohat, G" uniqKey="Avisar Shohat G">G Avisar-Shohat</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bar Dayan, Y" uniqKey="Bar Dayan Y">Y Bar-Dayan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Quah, Sr" uniqKey="Quah S">SR Quah</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hin Peng, L" uniqKey="Hin Peng L">L Hin-Peng</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
</listBibl>
</div1>
</back>
</TEI>
<pmc article-type="review-article">
<pmc-dir>properties open_access</pmc-dir>
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="nlm-ta">Int J Behav Med</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="iso-abbrev">Int J Behav Med</journal-id>
<journal-title-group>
<journal-title>International Journal of Behavioral Medicine</journal-title>
</journal-title-group>
<issn pub-type="ppub">1070-5503</issn>
<issn pub-type="epub">1532-7558</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Springer US</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>Boston</publisher-loc>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmid">19214752</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmc">7090865</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="publisher-id">9002</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s12529-008-9002-8</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Review</subject>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>Risk Perceptions Related to SARS and Avian Influenza: Theoretical Foundations of Current Empirical Research</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author" corresp="yes">
<name>
<surname>Leppin</surname>
<given-names>Anja</given-names>
</name>
<address>
<email>aleppin@health.sdu.dk</email>
</address>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="Aff1"></xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Aro</surname>
<given-names>Arja R.</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="Aff1"></xref>
</contrib>
<aff id="Aff1">
<institution-wrap>
<institution-id institution-id-type="GRID">grid.10825.3e</institution-id>
<institution-id institution-id-type="ISNI">0000000107280170</institution-id>
<institution>University of Southern Denmark, Institute of Public Health, Unit for Health Promotion,</institution>
</institution-wrap>
Niels Bohrs Vej 9, 6700 Esbjerg, Denmark</aff>
</contrib-group>
<pub-date pub-type="epub">
<day>12</day>
<month>2</month>
<year>2009</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="ppub">
<year>2009</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>16</volume>
<issue>1</issue>
<fpage>7</fpage>
<lpage>29</lpage>
<history>
<date date-type="accepted">
<day>29</day>
<month>9</month>
<year>2008</year>
</date>
</history>
<permissions>
<copyright-statement>© International Society of Behavioral Medicine 2009</copyright-statement>
<license>
<license-p>This article is made available via the PMC Open Access Subset for unrestricted research re-use and secondary analysis in any form or by any means with acknowledgement of the original source. These permissions are granted for the duration of the World Health Organization (WHO) declaration of COVID-19 as a global pandemic.</license-p>
</license>
</permissions>
<abstract id="Abs1">
<sec>
<title>Background</title>
<p>The outbreak of severe acute respiratory syndrome in 2003 and the subsequent emergence of the H5N1 virus have highlighted the threat of a global pandemic influenza outbreak. Planning effective public health control measures for such a case will be highly dependent on sound theory-based research on how people perceive the risks involved in such an event.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Purpose</title>
<p>The present article aims to review theoretical models and concepts underlying current empirical research on pandemic influenza risk perception.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Method</title>
<p>A review was conducted based on 28 empirical studies from 30 articles which were published between 2003 and 2007.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Results</title>
<p>Concepts of risk perception mostly seemed more pragmatic than theory-based and were highly heterogeneous, for instance, in terms of conceptualizing risk perception as an exclusively cognitive or as a cognitive
<italic>and</italic>
emotional phenomenon or whether the concept was dominated by expectancy or expectancy
<italic>and</italic>
value components. Similarly, the majority of studies investigating risk perceptions and protective behaviors were not model-based.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Conclusions</title>
<p>The current body of knowledge can only provide preliminary insights. Unlike the reviewed studies, which were mostly launched as a rapid response to outbreak situations, future research will have to invest more strongly into theoretical work to provide sounder evidence.</p>
</sec>
</abstract>
<kwd-group xml:lang="en">
<title>Keywords</title>
<kwd>Severe acute respiratory syndrome (SARS)</kwd>
<kwd>Avian influenza</kwd>
<kwd>Risk perception</kwd>
<kwd>Theory</kwd>
<kwd>Models</kwd>
</kwd-group>
<custom-meta-group>
<custom-meta>
<meta-name>issue-copyright-statement</meta-name>
<meta-value>© International Society of Behavioral Medicine 2009</meta-value>
</custom-meta>
</custom-meta-group>
</article-meta>
</front>
<body>
<sec id="Sec1" sec-type="introduction">
<title>Introduction</title>
<p>Newly emerging respiratory infectious diseases like severe acute respiratory syndrome (SARS) and avian influenza have created novel challenges to traditional nonmedical public health containment strategies [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR1">1</xref>
]. In the initial absence of effective vaccination, measures such as quarantine, travel restrictions, and the promotion of individual protection behaviors such as frequent hand washing, wearing of face masks, or avoiding public gatherings were the measures of choice during the SARS outbreak and will be again should another pandemic influenza, caused, for instance, by H5N1, occur. The effectiveness of such control measures depends fundamentally on the public’s willingness to cooperate, which again is likely to be associated with the level of personal risk people perceive. Sound empirical data about the ways lay persons perceive such risks are therefore essential to develop health communication strategies and successfully devise containment plans. Collecting such a sound database, however, requires first a thorough theoretical understanding of what constitutes lay persons’ risk perceptions of pandemic influenza, how these perceptions emerge, as well as how they influence protective behaviors.</p>
<p>Therefore, as an addition to this special issue’s original contributions on perceptions of SARS and avian influenza risk, the present article will take a more general perspective and explore the theoretical foundations of risk perception research within the specific context of pandemic influenza
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="Fn1">1</xref>
. Specifically, the present article aims to review theoretical models and concepts underlying empirical research on perceptions related to SARS and avian influenza as the two most recent threatening cases of respiratory virus outbreaks. The particular focus of this review will be on how risk perception is conceptualized and also on which models have been chosen by current empirical studies to test for the relationship between SARS or avian influenza risk perception and protective behaviors.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec2">
<title>Current Concepts of Risk Perception</title>
<sec id="Sec3">
<title>The Psychometric Paradigm and Expectancy-Value Models</title>
<p>Concepts of risk have been shaped in the light of different scientific traditions and paradigms. Psychological research on risk perception has mainly been dominated by the
<italic>psychometric paradigm</italic>
on the one hand and the
<italic>subjective expected utility approach</italic>
on the other [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR2">2</xref>
]. The psychometric paradigm [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR3">3</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR4">4</xref>
], wanting to investigate what characterizes risk in the public’s view, has tried to map lay people’s perception of different types of hazards in factorial space, describing the decisive dimensions as
<italic>unknown risk</italic>
and
<italic>dread risk</italic>
, with the latter dimension being the most important determinant of a “risk image.” While
<italic>unknown risk</italic>
describes phenomena which are perceived of as new, nonobservable, unknown to science, and whose effects are delayed,
<italic>dread risk</italic>
has been defined by characteristics such as involuntariness, uncontrollability, fatal consequences, and catastrophic dimensions. Technological hazards, such as nuclear power or genetically modified food, tend to be perceived of as unknown risks and, just like natural disasters, often convey high dread potential. Lifestyle behaviors like smoking, unhealthy eating habits, or car speeding, on the other hand, are usually thought of as observable and well known and thus rank relatively low on the factor
<italic>unknown risk</italic>
. Also, lay persons tend to perceive these behaviors as voluntary, controllable, and as affecting people one at a time rather than simultaneously endangering whole populations. So, in the public’s eye lifestyle factors lack the
<italic>dread potential</italic>
of technological risks and hence often seem more acceptable than the former.</p>
<p>While there is yet no empirical evidence for this, one might speculate that the specific characteristics of disease threats such as SARS or avian influenza would place them conceptually closer to technological risks or to natural disasters than to individual risk behaviors. Just as for technological risks, an initial lack of knowledge about the origin or the life course of a newly emerging virus and the fact that the infection process in itself is unobservable and that visible effects are delayed might produce high rankings on the factor
<italic>unknown risk</italic>
. In addition, pandemic influenza, just like technological and natural hazards, has catastrophic potential, as infection could be fatal, and large numbers of people would be affected at the same time within the same geographical region. Beyond this, global mobility is bound to further add to catastrophic impact by rapid spread of a respiratory virus to distant regions of the world. Also, as specific vaccines need time to be developed and as 100% effective medication is unlikely to be available for a considerable time span, these diseases are difficult to control - all aspects which suggest that pandemic influenza would rank high on the
<italic>dread risk</italic>
factor just like technological risks or natural disasters. On the other hand, it is particularly the issue of control which might make perceptions of pandemic influenza more ambiguous. Like in the case of natural disasters or technological hazards, there is an outside threat, in this case a virus, which might seem difficult to control. However - different from natural or technological hazards - this particular outside agent depends on everyday human behavior to spread, i.e., social contact and hygiene behaviors. Even more than for instance smoking, car speeding, or alcohol consumption, these behaviors are extremely common, well known, and observable, more or less voluntary and—at least theoretically—modifiable or controllable. Objects with these characteristics are rarely perceived as “high risk” by lay persons, as research based on the psychometric paradigm has demonstrated [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR4">4</xref>
]. Thus, at least during the initial phase of an outbreak, there might be a difference between perceived threat of the disease and the risk potential actually attributed to one’s own individual contact behaviors.</p>
<p>This notion needs empirical testing, just as the assumption that the aggregate-level data of the psychometric paradigm are transferable to the individual level. If proven valid, however, this phenomenon might have consequences for planning public health control strategies. To combat disease spread in the initial phases of a pandemic influenza outbreak, where no vaccine is available, control measures will have to rely on influencing the extent (e.g., avoiding public gatherings, closing down schools and work places) and mode (e.g., wearing of face masks, no handshaking, frequent hand washing with soap, etc.) of human contact behaviors. Thus, within a very short time span, social interaction would have to transform from an integral, essential, mostly beneficial, or at least unquestioned part of daily life into risk-fraught, even potentially lethal behavior. How prepared or reluctant people are to make that mental shift, how effective risk perceptions are in initiating protective behaviors without creating panic and problematic social exclusion processes, and which factors influence these processes are key questions for planning containment and communication strategies.</p>
<p>Unlike the psychometric paradigm, which has been developed to describe and investigate aggregate-level characteristics of various types of hazards perceived of as “low risk” or “high risk,” expectancy-value models have focused on how individual risk perception influences decision making and behavior. Based on the subjective expected utility (SEU) approach [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR2">2</xref>
], which posited that people’s choices are a product of assessments of probability and utility/desirability of options, health-related approaches such as the Health Belief Model (HBM) [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR5">5</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR6">6</xref>
] or Protection Motivation Theory (PMT) [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR7">7</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR8">8</xref>
] have defined risk in line with the technical understanding of risk, i.e., as the likelihood of contracting a disease multiplied by disease severity. Together with other types of expectancies, like outcome expectancies (or cost–benefit perceptions) and self-efficacy expectations, these perceptions of risk are then presumed to determine health-protective behaviors such as using health care services, refraining from smoking, or being physically active. These models have found widespread application as well as a considerable degree of empirical support (see below) and might therefore also provide a sound basis for answering questions on how far pandemic influenza risk perceptions can be relied upon as motivating factors for protective behaviors. Beyond a mere focus on risk perception, they can also help reveal how other types of cognition, such as outcome expectancies, about control measures as well as judgements on costs involved in such measures, such as cutting or reducing human contacts, might support or counteract the motivational processes initiated by risk perception.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec4">
<title>Risk as Cognition or Risk as Emotion?</title>
<p>The Health Belief Model and Protection Motivation Theory, however, also have shortcomings which could limit their use for studying pandemic influenza risk perception. One common criticism of these models pertains to their strong focus on conscious-rationalist situational appraisal which falls short of capturing the emotional processes involved in human decision making. Dual-process models have for a long time been suggesting that analytic-central and emotional-heuristic processes work in concert to select and prioritize decisions and behavioral reactions [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR9">9</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR10">10</xref>
], and under certain circumstances emotions may even be the dominant force. Damasio [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR11">11</xref>
] has thus suggested that automated incentive or alarm signals linked to pleasant or unpleasant “gut feelings” often precede cognitive reasoning. In a similar vein, processing theories have emphasized the importance of an emotion–cognition pathway [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR12">12</xref>
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR14">14</xref>
]. Based on these ideas, the “risk as feelings” or “risk as values” approach [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR15">15</xref>
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR17">17</xref>
], just as the notion of an “affect heuristic” [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR18">18</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR19">19</xref>
], have posited that when making judgements individuals draw on two processes of evaluation, which reside in two distinct, if interrelated, systems of decision making. These are an
<italic>analytic-rational system</italic>
based on logical reasoning and probability judgements, on the one hand, and an
<italic>experiential system</italic>
, on the other, where direct automatic visceral reactions influence decision making, based on images, metaphors, and related emotions.</p>
<p>While the HBM or PMT, unlike the original SEU approach, do include an emotional component, i.e., fear, they represent a
<italic>cognitive appraisal framework</italic>
[
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR20">20</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR21">21</xref>
] in assuming that it is mostly cognitions which give raise to emotions, i.e., cognitive risk assessment determines experience of fear. This framework might well apply for behaviors aimed at fending off long-term chronic disease such as screening participation or lifestyle behaviors. In such cases, fear is likely to be less imminent and therefore secondary to more rational reflections about gains and losses related to protective behavior. However, in an acute threat situation like an influenza pandemic, emotional aspects might gain more immediate importance. This is all the more likely if, during the early stages of an outbreak, experts are unable to make more than tentative statements and provide partly contradictory prognoses and recommendations as it happened with SARS. Under these circumstances cognitive risk assessments might be severely hampered by lack of evidence-based information. Consequently, individuals might have little choice but to rely on experiential judgement. However, what role exactly an affect- or emotion-based judgement plays in interaction with risk-related cognitions is still an issue in need of clarification.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec5">
<title>The Social Context of Risk Perception</title>
<p>Another issue which warrants more attention than it has so far received in risk perception research is the social context of risk perception. Health behavior models such as the HBM or PMT have mainly focused on how risk perceptions and other cognitions influence behavior, while the question of how risk perceptions are formed in the first place has met with little attention. Public risk perception is thus mostly conceived of as an aggregate of purely individual assessments - a perspective which neglects cultural and subcultural frames of reference [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR22">22</xref>
]. Slovic [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR4">4</xref>
], in contrast, has emphasized the importance of
<italic>worldviews</italic>
, which can be perceived of as sociocultural attitudes guiding individual risk perception by providing normative orientation. Similarly,
<italic>social representation theory</italic>
[
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR23">23</xref>
] or the
<italic>social amplification of risk framework</italic>
[
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR24">24</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR25">25</xref>
] have emphasized the importance of collective, (sub)cultural–social dynamics which shape individual ways to perceive of and react to risk.</p>
<p>Even more than other types of diseases, infections inherently involve a social dimension due to the social basis of transmission. Risk perception here pertains not only to the self but also to the likelihood of close others being affected. Beyond that, the possibility that oneself might become instrumental in infecting others can evoke anticipations of guilt and blame. Furthermore, anticipation of social exclusion processes affecting those who have become “cases” or those who are facing the frontline combating disease, like health care workers, are bound to become a part of pandemic influenza risk conception and therefore might also have an influence on risk-related behaviors. While such processes can be assumed to be relevant for all societies, it seems probable that such “social risk perceptions” are even stronger in the collectivist societies of Asia than in the more individualist cultures of the West. Factoring in such processes of perceiving and reacting to collective threat is unquestionably an essential element of pandemic influenza preparedness.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec6">
<title>Models of the Risk Perception–Behavior Relationship</title>
<p>In line with the SEU approach, health behavior models assume that high perceived personal risk can be expected to motivate people into action. However, it has also been pointed out that not all empirical studies which have tested this assumption have yielded particularly large effects or have borne out this type of effect at all [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR26">26</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR27">27</xref>
]. This heterogeneous state of empirical findings reflects conflicting underlying psychological processes, but also methodological problems involved in the respective studies.</p>
<p>In terms of psychological processes, one explanation for the diversity of findings is that very high risk perceptions in some population subgroups, instead of mobilizing people into action, might evoke feelings of helplessness which could paralyze rather than provoke protective behaviors. A threat like pandemic influenza, where in the initial absence of a vaccine the efficacy of countermeasures must remain unclear, might be particularly prone to such no-action responses. Vice versa, some people will downplay risk as a kind of coping strategy and thus report lower risk perception which, however, would not necessarily keep them from initiating protective action. Testing such alternative pathways requires the investigation not only of cognitive but also of emotional aspects of or reactions to risk perception.</p>
<p>Also, risk perceptions are not alone in determining protective behaviors. In the absence of positive outcome expectancies for protective behaviors or high self-efficacy, in terms of feeling able to perform these behaviors, high risk perceptions are unlikely to be particularly effective. Another important factor in this context is perceived social norms as outlined by the Theory of Reasoned Action/Theory of Planned Behavior [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR28">28</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR29">29</xref>
]. Normative influences can explain why even those with low personal risk perceptions sometimes adopt protective behaviors. During an outbreak protective behaviors are widely recommended and visibly performed by a great number of people, so that their practice acquires normative status. During the SARS epidemic this might have happened with behaviors such as wearing of face masks, frequent hand washing, or avoidance of public gatherings, where the issue was not only to protect oneself but also to not be perceived as someone who puts others at risk.</p>
<p>Brown [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR30">30</xref>
] has pointed out yet another reasoning process which might cancel out any protective motivational impulses of risk appraisal: Belief modification as a means to reduce cognitive dissonance. In a prospective study on car speeding the author found an inverse relationship between car speeding and perception of accident risk, which he accordingly interpreted as an attempt to reduce dissonance or else as a learning effect signaling that, in the absence of (immediate) negative consequences, risk perceptions might decrease [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR31">31</xref>
]. For pandemic influenza such an effect could occur if people who escape infection during an initial wave get careless, based on the assumption that they could be “immune.” Finding out whether such processes actually take place requires longitudinal process approaches and also in-depth explorations of people’s ways of reasoning about their personal risk perceptions.</p>
<p>Finally, the impact of risk perceptions on behavior is likely to differ depending on the specificity of the risk assessments. Thus, perceived general risk of contracting SARS or avian influenza might relate less strongly to specific types of protective behavior such as not shaking hands, frequently washing hands, etc. than the more specific risks perceived as being involved in these particular behaviors.</p>
<p>From a methodological perspective, Brewer et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR26">26</xref>
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR9">]</xref>
(see also [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR27">27</xref>
]) have recently highlighted another series of problems. One of these is the failure to condition the risk question on not taking action [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR27">27</xref>
]. Not accounting for anticipated or intended positive behavior change can lead to an underestimation of risk because it is likely that some people assess their risk as being low due to already factoring in a change towards anticipated or already accomplished protective behavior. The authors, however, also pointed out that the relevance of this issue depends on whether the precaution is actually expected to eliminate or at least reduce the risk (response efficacy). For investigating pandemic influenza, this type of validity threat might therefore be less imminent than, e.g., for a study on the relationship between perceived risk of “normal” influenza and getting a vaccination. Even so, such effects are unlikely to be nonexistent, so that perceived pandemic influenza risk research should also profit from assessments with and without (anticipated or actual) behavior change.</p>
<p>Interpretational ambiguity can, of course, also result from temporal relations between variables. In cross-sectional studies perceptions of risk for “ordinary influenza” in people who have already been vaccinated are bound to be affected by their expectation of being protected by the vaccination, hence their lowered risk perceptions - which then should not be interpreted as an indication of higher risk perception leading to less protective behavior. Brewer et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR26">26</xref>
] explicitly tried to disentangle the various types of effects by investigating different temporal hypotheses of risk perception about Lyme disease and protective behavior, i.e., getting vaccinated. Based on their findings, they once more emphasized the well-known predicament of cross-sectional studies. Even though often claiming to test a behavior motivation hypothesis, studies based on correlational designs cannot perform more than a test of an
<italic>accuracy hypothesis</italic>
, stating that people who engage in more risk behaviors should also endorse higher risk perceptions. Testing a
<italic>behavior motivation hypothesis</italic>
on the other hand requires a longitudinal design, looking at whether higher risk perception at t1 leads to a stronger increase in protective behavior at t2. Yet another effect is suggested by the
<italic>risk appraisal hypothesis</italic>
, which suggests that risk appraisal will change as a result of behavior change, i.e., it will decrease once positive behavior change has been initiated. The two tests should thus generate inverse correlations. In the first phase, when the behavior motivation model is tested, perceived risk should be positively related to preventive behavior. In the second phase, however, when the risk appraisal model is under investigation, a negative relationship between preventive behavior and risk perception should be expected. Empirical findings of the Brewer et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR26">26</xref>
] study supported these assumptions, showing that higher initial risk judgements concerning Lyme disease encouraged people in favor of vaccination. Taking action subsequently reduced risk perception, indicating also that people who had been vaccinated viewed their risk as lower than those who had not, thereby confirming the accuracy hypothesis. The quite common failure to explicitly distinguish these processes contributes to misinterpretation of findings and misattribution of effects.</p>
<p>The long list of open questions and unsolved issues in risk perception research certainly sets an ambitious research agenda. The purpose of the present study is therefore, first, to take stock of the status quo with regard to the area of pandemic influenza risk, i.e., to investigate the theoretical basis of current empirical research on SARS and avian influenza risk perceptions. Relevant empirical studies will be explored in terms of (1) their conceptual and operational definitions of risk perception and (2) the theoretical models used for investigating the role of risk perceptions as determinants of protective behaviors. The review uses a descriptive–quantitative approach insofar as it not only tries to describe the different concepts and models of risk used in the various studies and also to quantify the amount of studies falling into different categories in order to give a more precise account of the state of theoretical groundwork behind current empirical risk perception research. What the paper does not provide is a review on empirical findings of the current risk perception research itself in terms of degree of risk perception in different populations, or strength of the risk perception–health behavior relationship.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec7">
<title>Method</title>
<p>The analysis is based on 28 empirical studies from 30 publications
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="Fn2">2</xref>
focusing on the threat posed by SARS and avian influenza and the subjective perceptions related to these health hazards (see Table 
<xref rid="Tab1" ref-type="table">1</xref>
). The search for studies published between January of 2003, the year of the SARS outbreak, and October 2007 was conducted with the help of different databases (PUBMED, EMBASE, PSYCLIT, and CINAHL), alternatively combining the term “risk perception” with “SARS,” “avian flu,” “avian influenza,” and “pandemic influenza.” Additionally, reference sections of the papers obtained were searched for further articles. A study was included only if it explicitly referred to “risk perception” or else to subcomponents of the perceived risk concept, i.e., “perceived susceptibility” or “perceived vulnerability.” Thus, studies dealing exclusively with, for instance, SARS-related “distress” or “threat” without explicitly referring to the perceived risk concept were not included in the review.
<table-wrap id="Tab1">
<label>Table 1</label>
<caption>
<p>Overview of characteristics of the reviewed studies</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th>Authors</th>
<th>Disease</th>
<th>Country</th>
<th>Sample</th>
<th>Study design</th>
<th>Research question/study purpose</th>
<th>Theoretical model</th>
<th>Core concept of risk</th>
<th>Risk-related cognitions and emotions</th>
<th>Study of risk perception- behavior-relationship</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td rowspan="8">Abbate et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR50">50</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="8">Avian influenza</td>
<td rowspan="8">Italy</td>
<td rowspan="8">Poultry workers, aged 19–75, from 110 poultry farms in the Campania region, Italy;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 257</td>
<td rowspan="8">Cross-sectional interview survey in the work place, December 2005–March 2006</td>
<td rowspan="8">Evaluation of knowledge, attitudes, and infection control practices</td>
<td rowspan="8">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
<td>
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="7">Modification of work habits; Protection measures, e.g. wearing gloves, face masks, eye protection, hand washing</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived risk for contracting avian influenza for: poultry workers, butchers, veterinarians, hunters</td>
<td>Perceived severity: A1 is a serious disease</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Dichotomous scale: yes–no</td>
<td>Three-point scale: “agree,” “uncertain,” and “disagree”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td>
<italic>Network risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Fear of getting avian influenza</td>
<td rowspan="2">Fear of family–coworkers getting avian influenza</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Ten-point scale: “no fear at all” to “very much fear”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="7">Barennes et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR80">80</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="7">Avian influenza</td>
<td rowspan="7">Laos</td>
<td rowspan="2">Three random samples</td>
<td rowspan="7">Cross-sectional interview survey, April–March 2006</td>
<td rowspan="7">To learn more about Laotian’s knowledge of HPA1 and perceptions of their risk</td>
<td rowspan="7">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="7"></td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="2">
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="6">Protective behaviors, e.g., mask wearing, hand washing</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="3">(1) Market vendors and visitors in an urban setting, mean age = 41;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 461</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived risk for avian influenza at home</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived risk for avian influenza in Laos</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>(2) People living in a semiurban setting, mean age = 34;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 192</td>
<td rowspan="2">Dichotomous scales: yes–no</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>(3) People living in a rural setting, mean age = 38;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 189</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="16">Brug et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR44">44</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="16">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="16">Netherlands</td>
<td rowspan="16">Participants in an internet research panel, aged 19–78;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 373</td>
<td rowspan="16">Cross-sectional internet survey, during outbreak 2003 in an area with no cases</td>
<td rowspan="16">To explore SARS-related perceptions, knowledge, actions, and use of information sources</td>
<td rowspan="16">Precaution Adoption Model</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="16"></td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="15">Protective behaviors, e.g., wearing of face masks, hand washing</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived likelihood of acquiring SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived likelihood of acquiring SARS compared to other persons</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived likelihood of other diseases (e.g., cancer/accidents)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived likelihood of dying from SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Five-point scales: “negligible” to “very large”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Worry about oneself contracting SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Network risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Worry about a family member contracting SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Worry about SARS occurring in the region</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Worry about SARS emerging as a health issue</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Five-point scales: “negligible” to “very large”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="4">Cava et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR32">32</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="4">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="4">Canada</td>
<td rowspan="4">Men and women who had been quarantined during the outbreak; one third = health care workers;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 21</td>
<td rowspan="4">Qualitative study with a semistructured interview guide database, October 2003–March 2004</td>
<td rowspan="4">To explore the experience of being quarantined, particularly the relationship between perceived risk and compliance with the quarantine order</td>
<td rowspan="4">Health Belief Model and Protection Motivation Theory</td>
<td>No standardized questions; narratives</td>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Topics Raised</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="3">Fear and denial with regard to SARS</td>
<td rowspan="3">Compliance with quarantine measures</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Beliefs about level of risk of contracting SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Beliefs about level of risk of spreading SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="4">Chang et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR57">57</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="4">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="4">Taiwan</td>
<td rowspan="4">Nurses from one hospital, aged 29–41;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 330</td>
<td rowspan="4">Cross-sectional questionnaire survey, July–August 2003</td>
<td rowspan="4">To test the hypothesis that susceptibility perceptions moderate the relationship between organizational commitment and intention to leave the profession</td>
<td rowspan="4">None for the risk concept</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="4"></td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="3">Behavioral intention, i.e., intention to quit risky job</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Infection probability due to job: by taking care of a SARS patient, I might become infected</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Five-point scale: “strongly agree” to “strongly disagree”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="17">Chong et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR34">34</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="17">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="17">Taiwan</td>
<td rowspan="17">Health care workers from a large tertiary hospital in Southern Taiwan, aged 21–59;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 1,310</td>
<td rowspan="17">Cross-sectional questionnaire survey, part paper–pencil, part internet-based, early May (initial phase)–June 2003 (repair phase)</td>
<td rowspan="17">To assess the immediate stress and psychological impact of SARS on health workers in a tertiary hospital which was seriously affected by an outbreak and compare data over a period of 6 weeks</td>
<td rowspan="17">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="17"></td>
<td rowspan="17">No</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived risk through job: my job puts me at great risk for exposure to SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived fatality: little chance of survival if infected</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Fear of falling ill with SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Social Aspects</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Fear of passing SARS to others</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Family/friends worried that they might be infected by me</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>People avoid family because of work</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Risk Acceptance</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Accept the risk as part of job</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Other Social–Cognitive Constructs</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Personal control over infection</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Intention to change job</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Five-point scales: “strongly agree” to “strongly disagree”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="6">de Zwart et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR40">40</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="6">Avian influenza</td>
<td rowspan="6">Multicountry comparison study: five European countries and three Asian countries/cities: Denmark, Netherlands, UK, Spain, China, Hong Kong, Singapore</td>
<td rowspan="6">Eight random samples; adult population in the respective countries, aged 18–75;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 3436</td>
<td rowspan="6">Cross-sectional telephone survey, September–November 2005</td>
<td rowspan="6">Investigation of risk perception and efficacy beliefs related to avian influenza</td>
<td rowspan="6">Protection Motivation Theory</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="6"></td>
<td rowspan="6">No</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Vulnerability to SARS: perceived likelihood of becoming infected</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived severity of SARS: risk of dying from SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Risk: multiplication of vulnerability and severity as specified by protection motivation theory</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Five-point scale: “low” to “high”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="10">Fielding et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR43">43</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="10">Avian influenza</td>
<td rowspan="10">Hong Kong</td>
<td rowspan="10">Random sample; general population households, aged 16–95;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 986</td>
<td rowspan="10">Cross-sectional telephone survey, during the peak of outbreak 2004</td>
<td rowspan="10">To determine population knowledge of risk and self-protection practices and estimate population exposure from live chicken sales</td>
<td rowspan="10">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="10"></td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="9">Protective/risk behavior, e.g., buying live chickens, handling chickens</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived likelihood of getting sick from buying live chickens</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Five-point scale: “never–very” to “unlikely” and 0–100% probability estimate</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Buying live chickens is risky to health</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Network risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Friends expressing worry about catching avian influenza</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Five- to seven-point scales</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="5">Hong and Collins [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR53">53</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="5">SARS and influenza</td>
<td rowspan="5">Korea</td>
<td rowspan="5">Adults from the general public, aged 20 to 75, who were approached at random in the city center and shopping malls;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 350</td>
<td rowspan="5">Cross-sectional questionnaire survey, October 2003</td>
<td rowspan="5">To examine how connections between risk perceptions of influenza and SARS affect behavioral intentions for vaccination</td>
<td rowspan="5">State-dependent expected utility framework</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="5"></td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="4">Behavioral intention for influenza vaccination</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived riskiness of SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived riskiness of “normal” influenza</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Five-point scales: “not at all risky” to “extremely risky”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="10">Imai et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR35">35</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="10">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="10">Japan</td>
<td rowspan="10">Health care workers from seven hospitals throughout Japan, mean age 35.6 (SD = 11.2);
<italic>N</italic>
 = 7,463</td>
<td rowspan="10">Cross-sectional questionnaire survey, immediately after SARS epidemic in other Asian countries in 2003</td>
<td rowspan="10">To examine the level of SARS risk perception and anxiety in Japan, the knowledge of preventive measures and perception of infection control measures and relations among these factors</td>
<td rowspan="10">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="10"></td>
<td rowspan="10">No</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Fear of falling ill with SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Risk Acceptance</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Risk accepted as part of job</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Other Social–Cognitive Constructs</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Personal control over infection</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Intention to change job</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Avoidance of patients</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Seven-point scales: “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree,” + “do not know”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="7">Ji et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR42">42</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="7">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="2">(1) Toronto, Canada</td>
<td rowspan="7">University students,
<italic>N</italic>
 = 104 (Chinese background),
<italic>N</italic>
 = 35 (Canadians of European descent)</td>
<td rowspan="7">Cross-sectional questionnaire survey, May–June 2003</td>
<td rowspan="7">To test if Chinese students are more unrealistically optimistic than European-Canadian students</td>
<td rowspan="7">Theory of Optimistic Bias/Unrealistic Optimism</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="7"></td>
<td rowspan="7">No</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="5">(2) Beijing, China</td>
<td>Perceived probability of contracting SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Probability of own chance to become infected as compared to an average person of same gender and age</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived probability that an average person of same age and gender might get infected</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Five-point scales: “much less likely than average” to “much more likely than average”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="10">Koh et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR54">54</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="10">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="10">Singapore</td>
<td rowspan="10">All employees of nine major health care institutions;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 10,511</td>
<td rowspan="10">Cross-sectional mailed questionnaire survey, during outbreak, March–June 2003</td>
<td rowspan="10">Learning about the fears, anxieties, and reactions of health care workers during the SARS epidemic</td>
<td rowspan="10">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>
<bold>Social Aspects</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="10">No</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="2">Close others worried they might be infected by me</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="2">Perceived risk through job: job puts me at great risk of exposure to SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Fear of spreading the disease</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>Social avoidance because of job</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="5">Six-point scales: “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Fear of falling ill with SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Risk acceptance</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Risk accepted as part of job</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Six-point scales: “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="7">Koh et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR55">55</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="7">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="6">(1) Singapore</td>
<td rowspan="6">Health care workers from (1) nine primary hospitals and nine major institutional health care settings in Singapore;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 15,025</td>
<td rowspan="7">Cross-sectional questionnaire survey, May–September 2003</td>
<td rowspan="7">Study of perceptions of risk for SARS infection and preventive measures among health care workers in Singapore, who handled cases of SARS and in Japan, a SARS-free country</td>
<td rowspan="7">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="7"></td>
<td rowspan="7">No</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived risk through job: job puts me at great risk for exposure to SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Fear of falling ill with SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>(2) Japan</td>
<td>(2) Seven tertiary-level hospitals throughout Japan;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 9,978</td>
<td>Six-point scales: “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="3">Kristiansen et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR47">47</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="3">Avian influenza</td>
<td rowspan="3">Norway</td>
<td>(1) Norwegians aged 15–67;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 1,004</td>
<td rowspan="3">Cross-sectional internet-based questionnaire survey</td>
<td rowspan="3">What are people’s perceptions of the mortality risk during an influenza pandemic?</td>
<td rowspan="3">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="3"></td>
<td rowspan="3">No</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="2">(2) Norwegians aged 68+, all randomly chosen from an internet-based panel of 25,000 Norwegians aged 15years +;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 165</td>
<td>
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived probability of death from AI: estimated number of fatalities in Norway in case of an outbreak</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="8">Lau et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR41">41</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="8">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="8">Hong Kong</td>
<td rowspan="8">Random sample; Chinese-speaking residents, aged 18–60;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 457</td>
<td rowspan="8">Cross-sectional telephone survey, December 2003–January 2004</td>
<td rowspan="8">To investigate the general population’s perceptions on susceptibility of contracting SARS via daily contacts with asymptomatic and recovered SARS patients and the diseases’ consequences for the health of recovered patients</td>
<td rowspan="8">Health Belief Model</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="7">Avoidance of recovered patients</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived susceptibility to SARS</td>
<td>Worry about contracting SARS when interacting with recovered patient</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Infectivity of recovered SARS patients, e.g., dining with recovered SARS patient could transmit SARS</td>
<td rowspan="5">Disturbance about SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived consequences/severity: mortality rate for SARS patients;</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Consequences of SARS for health state</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Consequences of SARS for ability to perform job duties</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Dichotomous scales: percent agreement</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="7">Lau et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR48">48</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="7">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="7">Hong Kong</td>
<td rowspan="7">Travelers from mainland China to Hong Kong, aged 18–60;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 839</td>
<td rowspan="7">Cross-sectional questionnaire survey conducted in the exit hall of a custom office of a main border checkpoint, April 2003</td>
<td rowspan="7">To investigate the prevalence of preventive behaviors practiced by cross-border travelers as well as factors associated with these</td>
<td rowspan="7">Health Belief Model</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="7"></td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="6">Protective behaviors, e.g., hand washing, mask wearing</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived risk of contracting SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived risk of Hong Kong residents to contract SARS in different provinces in mainland China</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived fatality of SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Five-point scales: “very low” to “very high”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="5">Lau et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR51">51</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="5">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="5">Hong Kong</td>
<td rowspan="5">Chinese-speaking residents, aged 15–60, traveling back to Hong Kong by air;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 820</td>
<td rowspan="5">Cross-sectional questionnaire survey conducted at airport express bus station and express train platform at Chap Lap Kok International Airport, April 2003</td>
<td rowspan="5">To investigate factors associated with SARS-preventive behaviors</td>
<td rowspan="5">Health Belief model</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
<td>
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="4">Protective behaviors, e.g., mask wearing, avoidance of public places</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Susceptibility to SARS: perceived chance of infection on board</td>
<td>Perceived severity of SARS:</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived chance of infection at destination point</td>
<td>Perceived chance of mortality of SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Five-point scales: “very low” to “very high”</td>
<td>Five-point scales: “very low” to “very high”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="12">Lau et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR56">56</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="12">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="12">Hong Kong</td>
<td>Ten random samples; Chinese-speaking Hong Kong residents, aged 18–60;</td>
<td rowspan="12">Ten telephone interview surveys with subsequent cohorts,1–5 before April 2003 (escalating phase), 6–10 April 1 and after (improving phase)</td>
<td rowspan="12">Investigation of the evolution and changes in attitudes, knowledge, and behaviors of Hong Kong residents in response to the public health crisis during the two phases of the epidemic</td>
<td rowspan="12">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="12">No</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>(1)
<italic>N</italic>
 = 80</td>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td>
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>(2)
<italic>N</italic>
 = 156</td>
<td>Susceptibility to infection with SARS: perceived chance of infection for oneself</td>
<td>Perceived severity of SARS (Is SARS fatal?)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>(3)
<italic>N</italic>
 = 156</td>
<td>
<italic>Network risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="9">Dichotomous scale: yes–no</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>(4)
<italic>N</italic>
 = 91</td>
<td>Perceived chance of infection for family members</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>(5)
<italic>N</italic>
 = 66</td>
<td>
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>(6)
<italic>N</italic>
 = 169</td>
<td>Perceived risk of SARS transmission in different places (e.g., public transport, office, regions)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>(7)
<italic>N</italic>
 = 159</td>
<td>Percent “very large or large”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>(8)
<italic>N</italic>
 = 155</td>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>(9)
<italic>N</italic>
 = 165</td>
<td>
<italic>Personal and network risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="2">(10)
<italic>N</italic>
 = 165</td>
<td>Worry about oneself and family contracting SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Percent “much or very much”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="7">Leung et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR45">45</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="7">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="7">Hong Kong</td>
<td>Four random samples; adult Chinese residents, aged 18+; = 
<italic>N</italic>
 = 840</td>
<td rowspan="7">Four telephone surveys, 1 and 1.1: prosp. survey, April 2003 and May–July 2003; 2 and 2.1: prosp. survey, May-July 2003; 3: cross-sect. survey, May-June 2003; 4: cross-sect. survey, Dec. 2003</td>
<td rowspan="7">To aid planning for a possible return of SARS or, more generally, for an outbreak of other infectious diseases</td>
<td rowspan="7">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>1 = 
<italic>N</italic>
 = 840</td>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td>Protective behaviors, e.g., covering mouths when sneezing or coughing, hand washing with soap, face masks, using service utensils for shared food</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>1.1 = 
<italic>N</italic>
 = 480</td>
<td>Perceived likelihood of contracting SARS during the current outbreak</td>
<td rowspan="5">General anxiety (State Trait Anxiety Inventory)</td>
<td rowspan="5"> Health service use</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>2.0 = 
<italic>N</italic>
 = 361</td>
<td>Perceived likelihood of surviving SARS, if infected</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>2.1 = 
<italic>N</italic>
 = 272</td>
<td rowspan="3">Five-point scales: “very unlikely” to “very likely”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>3.0 = 
<italic>N</italic>
 = 706</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>4.0 = 
<italic>N</italic>
 = 2,574</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="5">Leung et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR52">52</xref>
]
<sup>a</sup>
</td>
<td rowspan="5">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="5">Hong Kong</td>
<td rowspan="5">Random sample; adult Chinese residents, aged 18+;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 1,115</td>
<td rowspan="5">Cross-sectional telephone survey, March–April 2003</td>
<td>To learn views and beliefs about and psychological responses to SARS</td>
<td rowspan="5">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="4">To aid public health officials and physicians in planning for a possible expansion of the current outbreak in different parts of the world</td>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td>Protective behaviors, e.g., covering mouths when sneezing or coughing, hand washing with soap, face masks, using service utensils for shared food</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived likelihood of contracting SARS during the current outbreak</td>
<td rowspan="3">General anxiety (State Trait Anxiety Inventory)</td>
<td rowspan="3"> Health service use</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived likelihood of surviving SARS, if infected</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Five-point scales: “very unlikely” to “very likely”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="5">Leung et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR49">49</xref>
]
<sup>a</sup>
</td>
<td rowspan="5">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="5">Hong Kong–Singapore</td>
<td>Random sample adult residents</td>
<td rowspan="5">Cross-sectional telephone surveys, May–June 2003</td>
<td rowspan="5">To learn the populations’ views and beliefs about and psychobehavioral responses to SARS</td>
<td rowspan="5">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>(1) Hong Kong, aged 18+;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 705</td>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="3">Protective behaviors, e.g., covering mouths when sneezing or coughing, hand washing with soap; face masks; using service utensils for shared food</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="3">(2) Singapore, aged 21+;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 1,201</td>
<td>Perceived likelihood of contracting SARS during the current outbreak</td>
<td rowspan="3">General anxiety (State Trait Anxiety Inventory)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived likelihood of surviving SARS, if infected</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Five-point scales: “very unlikely” to “very likely”</td>
<td> Health service use</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="6">Nickell et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR46">46</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="6">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="6">Toronto, Canada</td>
<td rowspan="6">Personnel from one university hospital with three campus sites, 39.3% below 40 years;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 1,983</td>
<td rowspan="6">Cross-sectional questionnaire survey, April 2003</td>
<td rowspan="6">(1) To determine the self-reported psychosocial effects associated with working in a hospital environment during the peak of a disease outbreak, (2) to examine the determinants of these effects</td>
<td rowspan="6">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="6">No</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived death rate for SARS</td>
<td>Concern about personal health</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="3">Three-point scale</td>
<td>
<italic>Network risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Concern about family health</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Three-point scales: “not,” “slightly to somewhat,” “very to extremely”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="4">Peltz et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR81">81</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="4">Avian influenza</td>
<td rowspan="4">Israel</td>
<td>Two random samples</td>
<td rowspan="4">Cross-sectional telephone survey, March 2006 during an avian influenza outbreak</td>
<td rowspan="4">To compare the differences in public interest, sense of knowledge, emotions, and compliance during the early phase of an avian influenza outbreak in an affected area compared with a nationwide population of the same country</td>
<td rowspan="4">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="4">No</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="2">(1) Adults nationwide,
<italic>N</italic>
 = 500</td>
<td>
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="2">Perceived likelihood of avian influenza outbreak to become a widespread disease among humans with a high human-to-human-transmission</td>
<td>Feelings of fear, indifference, stress, and hope as a result of outbreak</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>(2) Adults from affected area,
<italic>N</italic>
 = 103</td>
<td>Five-point scales</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="4">Quah and Hin-Peng [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR82">82</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="4">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="4">Singapore</td>
<td rowspan="4">Random sample; adult population, aged 21+;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 1,202</td>
<td rowspan="4">Cross-sectional telephone survey, during outbreak 2003</td>
<td rowspan="4">To examine preventive practices, perception of self-health, knowledge of SARS, and appraisal of SARS crisis management</td>
<td rowspan="4">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="3">Protective behaviors, e.g., cover mouth when sneezing or coughing, hand washing with soap, face masks, using service utensils for shared food</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived susceptibility to SARS: perceived likelihood of contracting SARS</td>
<td>Anxiety</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Four-point scale: “very likely” to “very unlikely,” plus “do not know”</td>
<td>Three-point scale: “low”–“moderate”–“high”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="6">Rambaldini et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR33">33</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="6">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="6">Toronto, Canada</td>
<td rowspan="6">Random sampling with saturation; medical house staff from university hospitals caring for SARS patients;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 17</td>
<td rowspan="6">Qualitative study based on grounded theory, semistructured telephone interviews May and June 2003</td>
<td rowspan="6">To explore the perceptions, attitudes, and experiences of medical house staff at hospitals caring for patients with SARS</td>
<td rowspan="6">None</td>
<td>No standardized items; narratives</td>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="6">No</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Topics Raised</bold>
</td>
<td>Fear and worry</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="4">Concerns about individual safety, close others’ safety</td>
<td>
<bold>Social Aspects</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Concern about spreading disease to loved ones</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Social isolation</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Risk Acceptance</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="15">Shiao et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR37">37</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="15">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="15">Taiwan</td>
<td rowspan="15">Nurses in one community hospital, one secondary, and two tertiary referral hospitals, aged 20–52;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 753</td>
<td rowspan="15">Cross-sectional telephone survey, May 2003, after a SARS outbreak in April 2003</td>
<td rowspan="15">To determine the perceptions of risk of SARS infection in nurses and factors related to nurses’ consideration of leaving their job because of the SARS outbreak</td>
<td rowspan="15">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="14">Behavioral intention to leave job</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived risk through job: job puts me at great risk of exposure to SARS</td>
<td rowspan="2">Perceived fatality: poor chance of survival if infected</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Network risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>People close to me are at high risk of getting SARS because of my job</td>
<td>My chances of dying from SARS are higher than from cancer</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td>My chances of dying from SARS are higher than from traffic accidents</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td>
<bold>Social Aspects</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Fear of falling ill with SARS</td>
<td>People avoid me because of my job</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Social Aspects</bold>
</td>
<td>People avoid family members because of my job</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Close others worried for my health</td>
<td>
<bold>Risk Acceptance</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Family believes I have a high risk of getting SARS</td>
<td rowspan="5">Should not be looking after SARS patients</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Close others worried they might get infected through me</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Other Social–Cognitive Constructs</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Personal control over infection</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Seven-point scales: “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree,” + “do not know”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="13">Tam et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR36">36</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="13">Avian influenza</td>
<td rowspan="13">Hong Kong</td>
<td rowspan="13">Members of local nursing associations, 50% below 45 years;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 999</td>
<td rowspan="13">Cross-sectional mail questionnaire January–February 2006</td>
<td rowspan="13">To test the assumption that professional experience with SARS would affect health care workers attitude and preparedness towards the impending avian influenza epidemic</td>
<td rowspan="13">None</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="13"></td>
<td rowspan="13">No</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived likelihood of an avian influenza outbreak in Hong Kong</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Fear of falling ill with avian influenza</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Risk Acceptance</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Risk accepted as part of job</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Other Social–Cognitive Constructs</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Personal control over infection</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Contemplation of job change</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Avoidance attitude towards patients</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Six-point scales</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="4">Tang and Wong [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR39">39</xref>
]</td>
<td rowspan="4">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="4">Hong Kong</td>
<td rowspan="4">Random sample, Chinese community residents, aged 60 and older;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 354</td>
<td rowspan="4">Cross-sectional telephone survey, March–April 2003</td>
<td rowspan="4">To test the assumption that perceived health threats and efficacy beliefs were the two core dimension of motivation factors in relation to the practice of SARS-preventive behaviors</td>
<td rowspan="4">Health Belief Model and Theory of Planned Behavior</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="4"></td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td>Protective behaviors, e.g., washing hands with soap, building up body immunity with diet, exercise, rest, ensuring good indoor ventilation, face masks</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived vulnerability with regard to contracting SARS (three items): vulnerability to contracting SARS; knowing or having had contact with infected persons; having respiratory infection symptoms</td>
<td rowspan="2"> Health service use</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Four-point scales</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="8">Tang and Wong [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR38">38</xref>
]
<sup>b</sup>
</td>
<td rowspan="8">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="8">Hong Kong</td>
<td rowspan="8">Random sample; adult Chinese residents, aged 18+;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 1,329</td>
<td rowspan="8">Cross-sectional telephone survey, March–April 2003</td>
<td rowspan="8">To determine rates of SARS-preventive behavior in adult Chinese with different demographic background; test efficacy of HBM in predicting the practice of target preventive behavior</td>
<td rowspan="8">Health Belief Model</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="8"></td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="7">Wearing face masks</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived vulnerability to contracting SARS (three items): vulnerability to contracting SARS; knowing or having had contact with infected persons; having respiratory infection symptoms</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Dichotomous scales: yes–no + sum score of three items</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<bold>Emotional Dimension</bold>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>
<italic>Personal Risk + Universal/Community risk</italic>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Perceived severity (two items): fearfulness of SARS; worry about Hong Kong becoming a quarantine city</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Four-point scales: “not at all worried” to “very worried”</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="4">Tang and Wong [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR63">63</xref>
]
<sup>b</sup>
</td>
<td rowspan="4">SARS</td>
<td rowspan="4">Hong Kong</td>
<td>Random sample, adult Chinese residents, aged 18+;</td>
<td>Longitudinal telephone survey with two measurement points</td>
<td>To identify psychosocial factors associated with SARS-preventive behaviors and to assess whether preventive health behaviors increased after launching SARS community prevention activities</td>
<td rowspan="4">Health Belief Model, Theory of Reasoned Action/Theory of Planned Behavior, Self Efficacy Theory</td>
<td>
<bold>Cognitive Dimension</bold>
</td>
<td rowspan="4"></td>
<td>Yes</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td> (1)
<italic>N</italic>
 = 1,002</td>
<td>(1) March 17–18, 2003</td>
<td rowspan="3"></td>
<td>
<italic>Personal risk</italic>
</td>
<td rowspan="3">Protective behaviors, e.g., washing hands with soap, building up body immunity with diet, exercise, rest, ensuring good indoor ventilation, face masks</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td rowspan="2"> (2)
<italic>N</italic>
 = 1,329</td>
<td rowspan="2">(2) March 29–April 1, 2003</td>
<td>Perceived vulnerability to SARS</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td>Four-point scale</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
<table-wrap-foot>
<p>
<sup>a</sup>
For the purpose of the current review Leung et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR52">52</xref>
] and Leung et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR49">49</xref>
] were counted as one study only as they seemed to refer to partly the same data base</p>
<p>
<sup>b</sup>
For the purpose of the current review Tang and Wong [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR38">38</xref>
] and Tang and Wong [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR63">63</xref>
] were counted as one study only as they seemed to refer to partly the same data base</p>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
</p>
<sec id="Sec8">
<title>Categorization Procedures</title>
<p>Exploring risk concepts was made difficult by the fact that the large majority of studies provided no explicit theoretical definition of how risk perception was conceived of. As a consequence, risk concepts had to be traced back to and deduced from the operational measures used. Descriptions of these assessments, however, were not always sufficiently comprehensive or particularly precise, and the terminology used in the introductory or discussion sections of the articles were not always consistent with the measures actually applied and/or compatible with conventional definitions, all of which contributed to the complexity of the classification procedure. This multifaceted picture was further complicated by the fact that many studies, besides explicitly introducing measures of risk perception, additionally assessed aspects which could be considered as risk-related and/or which actually were considered integral parts of risk perception by some other studies. While, for example, some authors included fatality ratings in their core concepts of risk, others presented these as independent aspects. Similarly, some studies defined emotions, such as fear or worry about SARS, within the boundaries of their risk assessment, while others also included such “emotion items” but placed them beyond the narrower realm of risk perception by labeling them as separate features.</p>
<p>Classification of risk concepts, as documented in Table 
<xref rid="Tab1" ref-type="table">1</xref>
as well as in the following text, was thus carried out in two different ways. One was noting what the authors themselves explicitly referred to as their measures of risk perception, i.e., what they labeled as or listed among their risk perception measures. These will be referred to as the “core concepts” of risk perception. The other pertained to additional risk-related aspects measured in the studies. Table 
<xref rid="Tab1" ref-type="table">1</xref>
, therefore, besides listing the explicit operational definitions of risk perception in one column, contains a separate column listing those related aspects for each study.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec9" sec-type="results">
<title>Results</title>
<sec id="Sec10">
<title>Study Characteristics</title>
<p>The major amount of the reviewed studies (75%;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 21) dealt with SARS, only 25% (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 7) focused on the relatively newer phenomenon of avian influenza. Most of the SARS studies (90%;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 19) were carried out in 2003, with the large majority taking place during the outbreak in East–Southeast Asia–Canada in the first half of that year, while the avian influenza studies were conducted at later points, between 2004 and 2006. As might be expected, most studies were carried out in those parts of the world which had been affected most, with Asia clearly in first place. Thirty-two percent (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 9) of the studies originated from Hong Kong, another 36% (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 10) came from either Singapore, Taiwan, Japan, Korea, or Laos or compared samples from several of these countries. Fourteen percent (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 4) came from Canada and another 18% (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 5) from Western Europe and Israel (see Table 
<xref rid="Tab1" ref-type="table">1</xref>
).</p>
<p>Fifty percent (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 14) of the studies were based on samples from general adult populations, while the other 50% dealt with more selective groups. Mostly, these were persons at particular risk for SARS or avian influenza, such as health care workers from hospitals where SARS patients had been treated, travelers returning from mainland China to Hong Kong during the SARS outbreak, or poultry workers. Thirty-nine percent (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 11) of the studies used random sampling, 61% (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 17) purposive methods to recruit their participants.</p>
<p>The large majority of studies (93%;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 26) was based on quantitative survey research designs, using mostly either standardized telephone interviews or paper–pencil approaches or internet-based surveys in a few cases. Only two studies used a qualitative approach (7%) [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR32">32</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR33">33</xref>
]. Almost all studies were cross-sectional (89%;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 25). Only 11% (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 3) were based on a longitudinal design or used cohorts recruited at different points in time.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec11">
<title>Concepts of Risk Perception</title>
<p>Not surprisingly, the analysis revealed that the measurement concepts of risk perception varied widely (see Table 
<xref rid="Tab1" ref-type="table">1</xref>
). The majority of the studies (71%;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 20) relied on a few single items for their core risk measures asking for
<italic>perceived risk</italic>
or
<italic>perceived likelihood of contracting SARS</italic>
/
<italic>avian influenza</italic>
, for
<italic>perceived likelihood of a SARS</italic>
/
<italic>avian influenza outbreak</italic>
and/or
<italic>perceived fatality of the disease</italic>
, partly added by ratings of
<italic>fear or worry</italic>
with regard to the disease. Some studies, all of which investigated risk perceptions in health care workers, named a more diverse range of items under the risk perception label. Besides direct assessments of risk and/or fear of getting SARS, these included for instance perceptions of personal control, worry of close others about getting infected, and/or avoidance of patients due to infection risk or intention to change jobs [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR34">34</xref>
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR36">36</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR37">37</xref>
]. However, in these studies, too, all measures were used on a single-item basis. Two publications employed aggregate measures of perceived vulnerability (vulnerability, contact with infected persons, having respiratory symptoms) and/or perceived severity (fearfulness of SARS and worry about one’s own city becoming a quarantine area [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR38">38</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR39">39</xref>
]; see Table 
<xref rid="Tab1" ref-type="table">1</xref>
).</p>
<p>A little more than one third of the studies (36%;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 10) explicitly referred to a theoretical model, mostly the HBM (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 6), which also provides a definition of perceived risk (for further details, see the paragraph on the risk perception-protective behaviour relationship below). However, only few studies explicitly laid out their definition of risk perception in the introductory section of their papers [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR32">32</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR40">40</xref>
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR42">42</xref>
].</p>
<p>Further information on the notions of risk perception came from the exploration of the studies’ assessment measures. This analysis yielded a series of categories which were then used to classify the concepts. These categories, intended to capture the major communalities and differences between the approaches rather than to represent the numerous singularities of the different studies, were: (1)
<italic>psychological processes or dimensions</italic>
involved in risk perception (cognitive only versus cognitive plus emotional), (2)
<italic>type of judgement</italic>
(expectancy only versus expectancy × value), (3)
<italic>objects of risk perception</italic>
(self, network, community/society), (4)
<italic>type of risk</italic>
(risk of SARS/avian influenza, risk of other health hazards, social risks), (5)
<italic>risk situations</italic>
(general versus situation-specific risks), (6)
<italic>risk reference points on the health</italic>
<italic>disease continuum</italic>
(exposure to a virus, contraction of a virus, falling ill), and (7)
<italic>risk acceptance</italic>
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="Fn3">3</xref>
. Below, findings will be presented in line with these categories.</p>
<sec id="Sec12">
<title>Processes and Dimensions of Risk Perception: Cognitions and/or Emotions</title>
<p>The distinction here refers to the different processes involved in risk experience and the resulting dimensions of the concept, i.e., whether risk was assessed in purely cognitive terms, such as asking for assessments of
<italic>likelihood to contract SARS</italic>
or
<italic>perceived probability of dying from SARS</italic>
, and/or whether risk was also measured as an emotional phenomenon, such as experience of
<italic>fear</italic>
or
<italic>anxiety</italic>
of SARS or avian influenza. A major problem with categorizing the concepts was due to the fact that a considerable number of studies (31%,
<italic>N</italic>
 = 8) relied on the generic term “risk” for assessment (e.g., “My job puts me at great risk”) mostly without adding any further differentiated rating. As the authors themselves most often seemed to treat these measures as equivalent to assessments of
<italic>likelihood of contracting SARS</italic>
/
<italic>avian influenza</italic>
, they were counted among the cognitive concepts.</p>
<p>Based on this assumption, 61% (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 16) of the quantitative studies chose a one-dimensional approach, the large majority of which again defined risk perception in cognitive terms only (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 15). Thirty-nine percent (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 10) included a cognitive as well as an emotional dimension in their core risk concepts, one of which, however, explicitly referenced the emotional assessment not to the self but asked respondents to estimate the worry about avian influenza felt by friends [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR43">43</xref>
]. Terms used for the assessment of risk-related emotions ranged from the relatively mild
<italic>concern about contracting SARS</italic>
/
<italic>avian flu</italic>
and
<italic>worry about contracting SARS</italic>
/
<italic>avian flu</italic>
to the more intense
<italic>fear</italic>
, which was used most often, and
<italic>anxiety</italic>
(see Table 
<xref rid="Tab1" ref-type="table">1</xref>
). Of the two qualitative studies, one defined risk perceptions in cognitive terms but reported also on the emotional ramifications of the risk experience [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR32">32</xref>
]; the other focused on the emotional connotations of risk in terms of
<italic>concern</italic>
,
<italic>fear</italic>
, and
<italic>worry</italic>
[
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR33">33</xref>
].</p>
<p>Despite the overall dominance of the cognitive perspective on risk perception, it needs to be noted that of those studies which explicitly tapped risk perception in purely cognitive terms, six (23% of the 26 quantitative studies) actually also assessed emotional aspects. This, however, was done without explicitly labeling these measurements as indicators of risk perception, i.e., these were not parts of the core risk perception concepts. From this slightly wider perspective, more than half (61%;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 16) of the reviewed studies took account of cognitive as well as emotional aspects of risk (be it as an explicit part of risk perception or as a related concept); however, only very few explicitly dealt with the associations between these cognitive and emotional dimensions of risk. Thus, only three of the quantitative studies (11%) gave an account of the empirical relationship between measures of cognitive risk assessment and fear/anxiety [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR44">44</xref>
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR46">46</xref>
], with, for instance, one study reporting a correlation of
<italic>r</italic>
 = 0.64 for their measures of perceived risk and worry about contracting SARS [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR44">44</xref>
]. No study provided an extended discussion about the ways in which these aspects might relate to and influence each other.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec13">
<title>Types of Judgements: Expectancy and Value Components of Risk Perception</title>
<p>The single most frequently used assessment was
<italic>perceived likelihood</italic>
or
<italic>probability of contracting the disease</italic>
or
<italic>perceived likelihood of an outbreak</italic>
or variations of such measures which were included in 58% (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 15) of the core concepts represented in the quantitative studies. Eight studies (31%) relied exclusively on this approach. No study took account of this type of assessment as a “side aspect,” i.e., explicitly placed it beyond the risk label. In contrast, another type of probability rating, i.e.,
<italic>fatality ratings</italic>
or
<italic>ratings on probability of death/survival</italic>
was apparently considered less central. While 46% (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 12) of the studies included a measurement of
<italic>perceived fatality</italic>
, only 35% (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 9) mentioned this aspect in their core construct of risk, two of which defined risk exclusively as
<italic>perceived fatality</italic>
[
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR46">46</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR47">47</xref>
]. None of the reviewed studies conditioned their probability assessments on anticipated or actual behavior change.</p>
<p>Compared to the expectancy component, the value or severity element of risk seemed distinctly underrepresented, as the overwhelming majority of studies did not request explicit ratings of severity from their participants (e.g., “How severe or serious is SARS?”). If, however, all the studies measuring fatality as part of their core risk construct were also counted as assessing severity (35%;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 9), the picture seemed a little less skewed. Yet, even then, only seven, i.e., 27%, of the quantitative studies assessed both
<italic>likelihood of contracting</italic>
as well as
<italic>severity/fatality</italic>
as parts of their core risk concept [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR38">38</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR40">40</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR41">41</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR44">44</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR45">45</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR48">48</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR49">49</xref>
]. When those studies which assessed both likelihood as well as severity/fatality outside of their explicit risk measures were included [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR37">37</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR50">50</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR51">51</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR56">56</xref>
], the percentage rose to 42% (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 11). However, only one study used the multiplicative term between the two ratings, as specified by expected utility theory and its health-related applications such as the HBM or PMT [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR40">40</xref>
].</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec14">
<title>Objects of Risk Perception: The Self, Network, and Community</title>
<p>Another obvious difference in conceptualizing risk perception was related to the risk object chosen, i.e., who the risk was assessed for.
<italic>Universal risk</italic>
, which could also be perceived of as
<italic>societal or community risk</italic>
, refers to a global perception of outbreak probability or general fatality rates in case of an outbreak in a specific area.
<italic>Personal risk perception</italic>
and
<italic>network risk perception</italic>
, on the other hand, are estimations of one’s own and close others’ very specific probability to contract SARS or avian influenza or to die from this disease as, for instance, specified in the HBM or PMT. Forty-six percent (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 12) of the 26 quantitative studies focused on the personal perspective alone, i.e., assessed the risk for the self without tapping universal risk in their core concepts, while 27% (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 7) measured solely universal or community sense of risk, and another 27% (N = 7) looked at both aspects. Assessment of network risk was less common and occurred exclusively in combination with either of the other two measures. Five of the quantitative studies (19%) asked respondents to rate
<italic>risk for close others</italic>
, either within or beyond their core risk concepts, and both qualitative studies also reported concerns of respondents about the risk/safety of their loved ones.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec15">
<title>Type of Risk: Different Health Risks and Social Risks Accompanying Health Risks</title>
<p>Three, i.e., 11%, of the quantitative studies included measures which, besides asking for risk related to SARS or avian influenza, also tapped risk related to other diseases or requested direct comparisons between perceived risks for different diseases [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR37">37</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR44">44</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR53">53</xref>
].</p>
<p>Another three studies (11%), all of which were looking at perceived risk in health care workers, also included some measure on
<italic>social aspects of risk</italic>
, such as
<italic>perceiving a risk of spreading the disease to others</italic>
[
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR34">34</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR54">54</xref>
] and/or on whether people believed that their
<italic>close others felt worried about their health due to the respondent</italic>
<italic>s job in the health care sector</italic>
[
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR34">34</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR37">37</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR54">54</xref>
]. This issue of perceived risk to transmit the disease and ensuing role conflicts between professional and family obligations was also raised by the qualitative studies (“All residents expressed concern about the potential risk…they posed to their loved ones” [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR33">33</xref>
], p. 382) [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR32">32</xref>
].</p>
<p>The same studies raised another issue which might be relevant in this regard, even if it was not explicitly claimed as part of an anticipated “risk” because it mostly referred to actual past experiences, was that of possible
<italic>social isolation and stigmatization due to having a job in the health care sector</italic>
[
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR34">34</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR37">37</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR54">54</xref>
]. Again, one of the qualitative studies also reported that respondents raised the issue of felt isolation due to their exposure to the virus (“The cancellation of rounds and the restrictions placed on social interactions with other health care workers created a sense of isolation” [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR33">33</xref>
], p. 383).</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec16">
<title>Situational Specificity of Risk Appraisal: General Risks and Specific Risks</title>
<p>Variance occurred also in terms of the generality or specificity of risk assessments. Forty-two percent (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 11) of the quantitative studies phrased their questions in unconditional/absolute terms (e.g., “likelihood of contracting SARS”), whereas 58% (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 15) asked for qualified/conditional statements (e.g., “likelihood of contracting SARS in place x” or “likelihood of contracting SARS when doing y”) or included both aspects. In particular, these ratings pertained, for instance, to the probability of contracting SARS in different places within a city, such as public places, offices, or public transport [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR56">56</xref>
], when traveling on board of a plane [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR51">51</xref>
], when interacting with recovered patients [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR41">41</xref>
], or risk from specific behaviors such as buying live chickens [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR43">43</xref>
]. Specified assessments were also requested in most of the studies which investigated risk perceptions in “professional risk groups,” such as poultry workers or - primarily - employees in the health care sector (23%;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 6) [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR34">34</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR37">37</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR50">50</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR54">54</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR55">55</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR57">57</xref>
]. Both qualitative studies as well reported on risk perceptions related to a job in the health care professions [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR32">32</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR33">33</xref>
]. Yet another differentiation occurred within the group of studies which were conducted outside of acute outbreak areas and/or during times without an outbreak. While most studies - just like those conducted in outbreak areas - asked for general risk assessments, an alternative was to explicitly link the measurement to outbreak occurrence (i.e., likelihood of contracting or dying from the disease,
<italic>if</italic>
one’s own country were hit by an outbreak [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR47">47</xref>
]).</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec17">
<title>Risk Reference Points on the Personal Health–Disease Continuum: Risk of Exposure, Risk of Infection, and Risk of Illness</title>
<p>Differences in the core risk concepts also became evident in terms of how perceived risk was specified in relation to the reference points along the personal health–disease continuum. The most common choice among the studies which in any form assessed personal risk was to ask for
<italic>likelihood</italic>
(or else risk or fear) of
<italic>infection</italic>
or for
<italic>contracting</italic>
or
<italic>getting</italic>
SARS/avian flu (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 10; 38% of all quantitative studies). A few studies (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 4; 15%), however, in slightly different terms asked for ratings of risk of
<italic>exposure</italic>
to SARS/avian flu [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR34">34</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR37">37</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR54">54</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR55">55</xref>
], and instead or in addition requested respondents to indicate their fear or likelihood of
<italic>falling ill</italic>
or fear of
<italic>getting</italic>
sick (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 7; 27%) [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR34">34</xref>
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR36">36</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR37">37</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR43">43</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR54">54</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR55">55</xref>
].</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec18">
<title>Risk Acceptance</title>
<p>Finally, five (19%) of the quantitative studies introduced yet another feature (four of them explicitly as part of the risk construct), asking health care workers whether they accepted risk as part and parcel of the job [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR34">34</xref>
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR37">37</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR54">54</xref>
]. Rambaldini et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR33">33</xref>
] in their qualitative study on health professionals’ reactions to the SARS outbreak also reported on this aspect of risk perception.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec19">
<title>The Risk Perception–Protective Behavior Relationship</title>
<p>More than half, i.e., 57% (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 16) of the 28 reviewed studies investigated the role of risk perception for behaviors or behavioral intentions aimed at avoiding infection. Mostly, these were individual hygiene or lifestyle behaviors or behaviors related to handling live chickens (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 11; 39%). A few studies (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 3; 11%) dealt with social avoidance behaviors/intentions, such as health care workers intentions to leave their job [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR37">37</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR57">57</xref>
] or tendencies in the general public to avoid recovered SARS patients [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR41">41</xref>
]. One study investigated behavioral intentions for influenza vaccination as a result of SARS risk perception [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR53">53</xref>
] and another compliance with quarantine [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR32">32</xref>
].</p>
<p>The remainder of the studies (43%;
<italic>N</italic>
 = 12) was either of a purely descriptive–epidemiological nature or had their focus on potential determinants of risk perception. Of these, only two (17%) explicitly based their risk concepts on a theoretical model, i.e., the PMT [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR40">40</xref>
] and Weinstein’s [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR58">58</xref>
] Optimistic Bias concept [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR42">42</xref>
]. This low percentage of model-based research increased to 50% (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 8) in the subgroup of studies which investigated the potential role of risk perceptions as determinants of health-related behaviors or behavioral intentions (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 16). Of the seven quantitative studies which were model-based, three used the HBM [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR41">41</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR48">48</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR51">51</xref>
]; two further studies [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR38">38</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR39">39</xref>
] named the HBM as well as the Theory of Reasoned Action/Theory of Planned Behavior [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR28">28</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR29">29</xref>
] and Bandura’s Self Efficacy Theory [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR59">59</xref>
] as the basis of their work. Another choice presented by one of the studies [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR44">44</xref>
] was the Precaution Adoption Model [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR60">60</xref>
], and one further study [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR53">53</xref>
] based its research on a state-dependent expected utility framework [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR61">61</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR62">62</xref>
]. In addition, the HBM and PMT were claimed by one of the qualitative studies, which explored the association between risk perception and compliance with quarantine [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR32">32</xref>
]. In three of the quantitative studies all or most of the variables introduced by the respective model were tested within a multivariate analysis [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR39">39</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR48">48</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR51">51</xref>
], while the other studies used major components of the theories and/or tested bivariate relationships between separate model components and protective behaviors. Only one of the model-based studies had a longitudinal design [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR63">63</xref>
].</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec20" sec-type="discussion">
<title>Discussion</title>
<p>In the following section, the theoretical concepts of risk which emerged from the reviewed articles as well as the role risk perceptions play for protective behaviors will be summarized and examined. Strengths and deficits of current empirical risk perception research in terms of their theoretical foundations will be pointed out and conclusions will be drawn regarding implications for future research and also for practical purposes such as the development and design of public health risk messages.</p>
<sec id="Sec21">
<title>Concepts of Risk</title>
<p>The majority of the reviewed studies were launched as a rapid response to the SARS outbreak in 2003 or as an immediate reaction to the first human cases of avian influenza in some Asian countries. The purpose was thus socioepidemiological in most cases, aimed at assessing psychological reactions towards the SARS or avian influenza threat in general populations or in specific subgroups, such as health care workers, to predict adoption of protective behaviors and thereby to provide data for future intervention planning. This predominantly practice-oriented intent might explain the pervasive lack of conceptual elaboration on the perceived risk concept that was encountered. In fact, only few studies explicitly defined or theoretically explained their notion of perceived risk. Instead, it was mainly the operational level, i.e., the measurement instruments, which provided indications about the underlying notions of risk perception.</p>
<p>The concepts emerging this way turned out to be quite heterogeneous - and this finding related to a whole number of features characterizing the risk concept, including: the
<italic>psychological processes or dimensions</italic>
involved in risk perception (cognitive only versus cognitive plus emotional),
<italic>type of cognitive judgement</italic>
(expectancy only versus expectancy × value),
<italic>objects of risk perception</italic>
(self, network, community/society),
<italic>type of risk</italic>
(SARS/avian influenza risk, other health risks, social risks),
<italic>risk situations</italic>
(general versus situation-specific risks),
<italic>risk reference points on the health</italic>
<italic>disease continuum</italic>
(exposure to a virus, contraction of a virus, falling ill), and
<italic>risk acceptance</italic>
. The variance which occurred on all these dimensions clearly indicated that behind the common claim to investigate “risk perception” there was only very limited agreement on what exactly constitutes such perception of risk and how it should be measured. This, however, suggests that the empirical findings from the different studies on risk perception might be extremely difficult to compare and evaluate within a common context.</p>
<p>One of these differences, which actually touched the very core of the construct, became evident with regard to the choice of defining risk perception as an exclusively
<italic>cognitive</italic>
or as a
<italic>cognitive</italic>
<italic>emotional</italic>
phenomenon. While it seems plausible that a pandemic influenza outbreak constitutes a highly uncertain, ambiguous, and threatening situation, where lay people tend to fall back on immediate, visceral, and emotionally loaded concepts and images rather than rational probability assessments, this notion needs empirical testing: at different points in time, before and during an outbreak, and with different subgroups within general populations. However, only a little more than half of the reviewed studies looked not only at cognitive risk assessment but also at SARS- or avian-influenza-related emotions. Only about one third of the quantitative studies explicitly perceived of this emotion-based judgement as a dimension of risk perception and not as some external aspect. And whatever the concept - “risk
<italic>as</italic>
feelings” or “risk
<italic>and</italic>
feelings” [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR19">19</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR64">64</xref>
] - neither of the studies provided further theoretical elaboration on why fear or worry were perceived of as part of risk perception or seen as separate features, just as there was little interest in the empirical relations between cognitive and emotional aspects of risk perception. At this point, empirical research on SARS and avian influenza risk perception can thus provide few insights into the issue of whether emotions result from cognitive risk assessment and/or precede such cognitions or how cognitive and emotional processes interact in creating notions about “risk” and influence behavior. While the mostly social–epidemiologically oriented studies which formed the basis for the current review were not designed and intended to investigate such issues, future research should be directed towards this area as findings would not only be relevant for theory development but also for the ways in which health risk messages will have to be framed and designed in case of future outbreaks.</p>
<p>Another notable issue was the considerable heterogeneity in the operational terms chosen to measure risk affect, which ranged from the relatively mild
<italic>concern</italic>
and
<italic>worry</italic>
to
<italic>fear</italic>
or
<italic>anxiety</italic>
. Affirmative responses will probably be stronger for
<italic>worry</italic>
or
<italic>concern</italic>
than for
<italic>anxiety</italic>
, which makes these findings difficult to compare and evaluate. Also, while
<italic>worry</italic>
and
<italic>concern</italic>
can be perceived of as cognitive aspects of emotion [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR65">65</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR66">66</xref>
],
<italic>anxiety</italic>
and particularly
<italic>fear</italic>
, might tap more strongly into an affective-visceral dimension. These aspects are likely to relate differently and interact differently with cognitive probability ratings of disease [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR66">66</xref>
], but that notion, too, requires empirical testing.</p>
<p>Another difference concerning the essence of the risk paradigm became apparent in the way in which the studies measured the
<italic>type of risk judgements</italic>
, i.e., whether they assessed risk in terms of the
<italic>expectancy</italic>
and/or the
<italic>value component</italic>
. What came as a particular surprise was that only few studies actually relied on the theoretically established “probability × severity function” which posits that people’s choices are a product of assessments of probability and utility/desirability of options [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR2">2</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR5">5</xref>
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR8">8</xref>
]. First, quite a number of studies did not use a differentiated concept of risk at all but simply asked respondents to rate their
<italic>risk to contract SARS</italic>
. While this type of assessment, particularly when related to terms like “contracting” or “getting” a disease, can be considered predominantly cognitive and conceptually close to the “likelihood of contracting–measure,” there is also plausibility in the assumption that the term
<italic>risk</italic>
inherently entails severity aspects and/or is likely to incur more negative emotional connotations than the more neutral
<italic>likelihood</italic>
and is therefore conceptually closer to “fear.” This is also supported by psychometric paradigm-related research, suggesting that lay people tend to judge events with low probability but high catastrophic potential such as nuclear accidents as “high (dread) risk” [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR3">3</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR4">4</xref>
].</p>
<p>Furthermore, while probability of infection or of contracting disease was clearly the most common notion of risk perception, the value - or severity - component met with considerably less interest. However, a substantial number of studies included measures of fatality and it might be argued that fatality ratings
<italic>are</italic>
in fact assessments of value (i.e., severity) rather than measures of expectancy. Indeed, in many studies which used
<italic>fatality measures</italic>
, these were explicitly introduced as
<italic>measures of severity of disease</italic>
. Such a claim certainly has some degree of content validity, since most people would consider diseases they believe to be potentially fatal also as serious, even if from a purely structural–systematic perspective, probability of death ratings clearly refer to an expectancy-, not to a value-element. However, even when these ratings were counted as measuring severity, it seemed that severity was not generally considered to be an integral part of the risk concept, but was more often presented as a separate entity. This was also emphasized by the fact that only one study actually used the multiplicative term specified by the HBM and PMT [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR40">40</xref>
], i.e., the product of probability and severity. All these deviations from standard definitions of risk occurred mostly without any explanation of the rationale behind them — even in cases where models such as the HBM or PMT had been referred to in the theory sections of the papers. Testing the effects of subcomponents and their possible interactions, however, might yield essential information for effective risk communication, for instance, in terms of being able to specifically target unrealistically low perceptions of occurrence likelihood versus underestimations of severity.</p>
<p>Further variance was encountered in terms of the
<italic>objects risk was rated for</italic>
- a problem that Brewer et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR26">26</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR27">27</xref>
] have pointed out and which also became apparent in the present review. The most common assessment related to the self (
<italic>personal risk</italic>
), but some studies also measured risk perceptions for close others (
<italic>network risk</italic>
) and/or for the society/community as a whole (
<italic>universal or societal</italic>
/
<italic>community risk</italic>
). While network risk was commonly measured in addition to personal risk, societal/community risk was most often assessed
<italic>instead</italic>
of personal risk. While for respiratory infections risk of self and risk of others - due to “social contagiousness” - are bound to be correlated, they are also liable to differ. Judgements on personal likelihood to contract infectious disease are subject to considerations of individual immune competence and host resistance. Since diseases such as SARS and avian influenza are still relatively new and unknown in the wider population, people are likely to refer to personal experiences with diseases which might seem to bear some resemblance to “normal” flu or other infectious diseases. Based on differing individual experiences with susceptibility or immune competence, there could be considerable divergence between personal ratings and those for the population in general. In addition, tendencies towards an optimistic bias [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR58">58</xref>
] should influence personal ratings to be generally lower than those for the overall population. Harris and Middleton [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR67">67</xref>
], who investigated risk perceptions for various health hazards in students, showed a gradient for most of the rated conditions depending on who the rating was made for: from lowest ratings for the self to slightly higher ones for very close others, even higher ones for acquaintances, to highest scores for unknown others. Even if such tendencies should be less pronounced for respiratory infections such as SARS, which - compared to lifestyle disorders - tend to be “equal-opportunity diseases,” subjective concepts about individual susceptibility or resistance capacity might produce quite different results depending on who the risk is assessed for. In any case, if the goal is to determine how risk perception influences protective behaviors, personal risk assessment should be the first choice, while the other perspectives - while also important - should provide additional rather than surrogate insights.</p>
<p>Surprisingly few studies extended their view from the health dimension of SARS or avian influenza risk to other types of risks. Thus, only three studies asked their respondents to also rate other disease risks or to make direct comparisons between risk of SARS and other risks [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR37">37</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR44">44</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR53">53</xref>
]. Such comparisons could provide important insights, as the motivational potential of risk perceptions in case of an outbreak depends not only on their absolute but also their relative strength when compared to other health concerns. Relative importance of pandemic influenza risk, however, also relates to
<italic>other types of risk</italic>
which might be involved in a pandemic influenza hazard such as, for instance, social risks. Especially studies looking at health care workers did assess not only personal health risks but also the perceived risk of
<italic>spreading the disease to family and friends</italic>
and/or
<italic>issues of stigmatization</italic>
and
<italic>social isolation</italic>
[
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR32">32</xref>
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR34">34</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR37">37</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR54">54</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR55">55</xref>
]. While in the reviewed studies stigmatization and social isolation were mostly assessed in terms of actual experience, it is plausible to assume that anticipations of such processes (“social risk cognitions”) are relevant for shaping the sum of pandemic influenza risk connotations and thereby influences behavioral responses. Above and beyond risk to one’s personal health, anticipations of being responsible for spreading the virus to loved ones or being socially excluded because of one’s job might detract considerably from readiness to accept risk and might interfere with professional behavior in health care workers. This issue was also explicitly reflected in the concept of
<italic>risk acceptance</italic>
, which was brought up by a few studies [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR33">33</xref>
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR36">36</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR37">37</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR54">54</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR55">55</xref>
]. How professional groups who have a key role within containment plans balance personal and social risk expectations with their professional roles, how they weigh the risk of getting infected and in turn infecting others against the risk of jeopardizing their professional identity, is a concern which needs to be faced and dealt with in the course of effective intervention planning. Otherwise, a sudden lack of readiness of professionals will disrupt care for those who have already become patients or get in the way of public health containment measures and endanger collective protective action.</p>
<p>Social considerations are, of course, not restricted to professionals, as Yiang et al. in a contribution to the current issue, have shown. They investigated determinants of risk perception within Chinese communities in the Netherlands and the UK, pointing out the role of social costs of infection due to anticipated stigmatization processes. Similarly, a recent qualitative study on Hong Kong SARS survivors [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR68">68</xref>
] showed that stigmatization and discrimination was a pervasive and traumatic experience, a finding which led the author to conclude that anticipations of such social exclusion processes might well endanger the effectiveness of public health measures should there be another major outbreak (see also [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR69">69</xref>
]). Particularly in the early stages of an outbreak, when people are perhaps anyway more ready to interpret ambiguous respiratory symptoms as indications of “normal” flu than of a life-threatening disease, they might thus deliberate and delay, hoping that the symptoms will turn out harmless and go away, rather than take the risk of being prematurely identified as a carrier of a potentially lethal virus. Social risk perceptions such as these could easily cancel out motivating effects of perceived disease risk. Thus, the so far most commonly used one-dimensional notion of SARS/avian influenza risk as a “mere” health hazard might fall short of grasping the whole of what makes up lay persons’ or health professionals’ ideas about pandemic influenza risk.</p>
<p>Another angle which remains to be investigated by future studies is the relative relevance of health risk perceptions compared to perceptions of other types of life risks. In one of the original contributions to the current issue, Voeten et al. reported lower avian influenza risk perceptions in a sample of Chinese residents in the UK and the Netherlands in comparison to the general populations in these countries. According to the authors, an explanation for this rather surprising finding might be found in studies such as the one by Lai and Tao [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR70">70</xref>
] who showed that Chinese people in general seem to be far less concerned about disease risks than about risks that threaten, for instance, national stability and economic development. In the same vein, two of the reviewed studies [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR40">40</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR42">42</xref>
] reported lower risk perceptions in Asian as compared to European or North American samples. While this variation may largely be due to social–cultural differences, socioeconomical considerations may also play a part. Thus, people living under economically precarious conditions may place a relatively lower value on health consequences, at least as long as these remain speculative and distant when compared to other more immediate economic daily hazards. This might be specifically relevant for groups such as poultry workers who in case of an avian influenza outbreak will find themselves between a rock and a hard place when evaluating health vs. economic risk. Vice versa, as an example from the other end of the socioeconomic spectrum, globally mobile executives might at some point have to face the “bads” of globalized risk society as the almost inevitable flip side of the “goods” [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR71">71</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR72">72</xref>
], having to weigh the risk of health damage against that of economic losses due to restricting travel. Future research will have to take up the challenge to understand the processes by which pandemic influenza risk is grounded in the (sub)cultures and social structures of different countries and their respective worldviews [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR4">4</xref>
] in order to effectively tailor communication strategies to specific groups, particularly those who might face a heightened risk of exposure to influenza viruses while at the same time facing economic or social risks, like for example poultry farmers and vendors in the case of H5N1.</p>
<p>As for the
<italic>generality or specificity of risk situations</italic>
, a little less than half of the studies asked for general risk assessments, i.e., overall probability to contract SARS or general fatality rates, while a bit more than half qualified their assessments in one way or another. What was requested were, for instance, risk ratings for specific regions or ratings for situational circumstances, such as probability of infection when traveling or due to working in a specific job such as in the health care sector. Particularly, behavior-specific risk ratings should be expected to relate more closely to particular protective behaviors than general risk perception. In an outbreak situation specific assessments can help identify “critical areas of risk perception” where people perceive unrealistically low or high risk and which might warrant intervention. Vice versa, general risk expectations provide an interpretational background for behavior-specific risk perceptions because, even when specific risks are perceived of as not particularly high, this does not necessarily imply that risk perception in general is also low and vice versa. Therefore, both types of risk perception are essential and should not be treated as surrogates.</p>
<p>Yet another type of qualifying assessments became apparent in the small group of studies which were conducted outside of acute outbreak areas. It should make a difference whether respondents are asked for general statements (“probability of contracting SARS/avian influenza”) or whether such ratings are conditioned on the case of an outbreak (“probability of contracting SARS/avian influenza
<italic>if</italic>
an outbreak occurred in the country/region”). The latter should yield higher risk perceptions than those without qualification, as in the former case the disease has yet to arrive and might seem not only geographically but also psychologically more removed. In general, hypothetical risk perceptions such as these are hard to compare to “real” risk perceptions under outbreak conditions and do not refer to exactly the same phenomenon. Unlike chronic diseases, which develop gradually, pandemic influenza tends to be a more binary or “on–off phenomenon”, which sets quite distinct frames for risk perception under conditions of outbreak versus no outbreak. It would be profitable to study how hypothetical risk perceptions in a non-outbreak situation develop under conditions of imminent threat and whether perceptions of hypothetical risk can be extrapolated to actual risk perception. This, of course, would require introduction of long-term continuous surveillance studies.</p>
<p>Finally, variance also occurred with regard to the
<italic>reference point chosen for assessment on the health</italic>
<italic>illness continuum</italic>
. While most studies measured “likelihood of contracting SARS/avian influenza,” there were also some which asked for ratings of “exposure to SARS” or “falling ill with SARS/avian influenza.” Such diverse semantics could evoke different connotations. Exposure occurs prior to actual contraction or acquisition. Health care workers in an outbreak area might perceive a considerably higher likelihood of exposure to pandemic influenza than of actually contracting the disease because they might hope that immune competence and control measures would protect them from actually being infected. And while as a rule contracting the virus will also mean falling ill with the disease, the latter expression seems to indicate a step forward on the semantic continuum and to evoke stronger emotional connotations than a more neutral rating on probability to contract pandemic influenza.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec22">
<title>The Risk Perception–Protective Behavior Relationship</title>
<p>As outlined in the introductory section of the paper, much of the research work on the health risk perception–protective behavior relationship has been characterized by conceptual as well as methodological problems, leading to substantial interpretational ambiguity. The specific set of papers on SARS and avian influenza risk perception reviewed here seems to make no exception in this regard. Just as there was little theoretical elaboration on the risk concept itself, only a minority of the subgroup of studies which investigated the possible predictive role of risk perception for protective behaviors could actually be considered model-based. The current review showed that, of the 16 studies which were dealing with this issue, only half referred to any theoretical concept or model, most often the HBM. Of these, again only less than half actually tested the full respective model.</p>
<p>The resulting lack of opportunity for studying the complexity of the decision-making process is bound to also affect the extent to which findings for the relationship between risk perception and protective behaviors can be interpreted. To obtain a comprehensive picture of how decisions about protective behaviors are made, risk perceptions need to be tested in comparison to and/or in concert with other cognitions, such as those specified by the HBM or PMT, i.e., perceived efficacy of countermeasures (outcome expectancies) or the costs and benefits associated with behavioral change. For instance, the social aspects of pandemic influenza risk discussed above could also be conceptualized in terms of costs associated with risk-related behavior, as specified by the HBM. Thus, as outlined above, the cost factor “anticipated stigmatization” (or “social risk perceptions”) might neutralize mobilizing effects of health-related risk perceptions, for instance by precluding disclosure of symptoms. Also, to answer the question “how high is high enough” with regard to risk perception and “how high is too high” because helplessness sets in, risk cognitions would have to be jointly investigated with their emotional connotations and also together with outcome expectancies about control measures as well as self-efficacy expectancies. Taking action is bound to depend not only on motivating factors such as risk perception but also on enabling features like perceived availability of effective behavioral options or perceived social norms. Also, the construct of
<italic>risk acceptance</italic>
introduced by a few studies might further contribute to successfully predicting protective behaviors. Even high health risk perceptions in terms of likelihood to contract an infection might fail to have mobilizing effects if the related social risks are given greater weight, so that the health risk, even if high, is tolerated. Thus, a more systematic application of multifactor models would allow for far more complex insights into the workings of risk perceptions in shaping behavior than the most often used tests of bivariate relationships alone.</p>
<p>In addition, the utility of the data presented by the reviewed studies might be constrained by design deficits of the studies and ensuing validity problems. Despite their claim to investigate the motivating effects of risk perception on health-protective behaviors (
<italic>behavior motivation model</italic>
), the overwhelming majority of the studies which looked at risk perception–protective behavior relationships actually investigated
<italic>risk perception</italic>
<italic>behavior accuracy models</italic>
[
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR26">26</xref>
]. That is, due to the dominating correlational study designs, positive relations between risk perception and behaviors, like hand washing, wearing of face masks, or being careful in handling live chickens, indicate little more than the fit between perceptions of own protective behavior and risk (
<italic>accuracy hypothesis</italic>
), as people who protect themselves are also more likely to perceive less personal risk
<italic>because</italic>
of these protective behaviors. This ambiguity was exacerbated by the fact that no study attempted to compare behavior-conditioned and behavior-unconditioned risk perceptions, i.e., looked at risk perceptions with or without anticipated or past behavior change, as suggested by Brewer et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR26">26</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR27">27</xref>
]. Not taking this precaution can lead to dilution of risk perception effects, as some respondents might already factor in behaviour change when estimating their personal risk.</p>
<p>On the other hand, one could argue that pandemic influenza constitutes a special case. The investigation of this type of hazard might make studies less susceptible to internal validity threats than either studies on chronic disease or on types of acute disease which can be offset by vaccination, as in the Lyme disease example used by Brewer et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR26">26</xref>
]. Unlike the individual long-term development of chronic disease (e.g., coronary heart disease), where there is an extended time-frame for behavior-change, an outbreak situation creates a massive, collective, and acute threat, i.e., it has “dread potential” in terms of the psychometric paradigm, and people tend to feel more out of control [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR3">3</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR4">4</xref>
]. It seems likely that in the first stage of an outbreak risk perception is strongly influenced by the nature of the hazard and the way it is communicated by media coverage of case and fatality incidents. That impact might be more overwhelming than any effect which countermeasures might have on risk perception. In fact, the few longitudinal or cohort studies in this review found that during the initial phases of the SARS outbreak in 2003 there was a steady increase in risk perception, which only leveled off in later phases [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR45">45</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR56">56</xref>
]. Moreover, unlike other types of infectious diseases, which can be neutralized by almost 100% effective vaccinations, in case of pandemic influenza people cannot be certain about the effectiveness of preliminary vaccines or behavioral containment measures, so that it is unlikely that even strict adherence to protective behaviors will nullify any notion of risk. However, while these characteristics of pandemic influenza risk might lessen interpretational ambiguities, it will not eliminate them, particularly when studies are conducted in the latter phases of an outbreak. As consistent behavior change is more likely to have occurred by then, it is also more likely that behavior motivation effects are wiped out or reduced by appraisal effects (lower risk perceptions due to higher protective behaviors). Studying the change of pandemic influenza risk perceptions and their “true” influence on protective behaviors over time therefore remains an important objective, requiring not only studies with longitudinal designs, starting after potential future outbreaks, but also long-term surveillance studies which are initiated before an actual outbreak occurs.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec23">
<title>Limitations</title>
<p>It certainly needs mentioning that the classification of studies into the different categories can be disputed in some regards, due to the problems outlined in the “
<xref rid="Sec7" ref-type="sec">Method</xref>
” section. Thus, for instance, to categorize the studies which directly asked for ratings of “risk” (e.g., “My job puts me at great risk”) into the category of cognitive risk judgements, analogous to the measurements of likelihood or probability of infection, can be considered problematic, as the term “risk” is affectively less neutral than “likelihood” or “probability”. Also, in general, the descriptions of measurement instruments in the reviewed articles were not always sufficiently precise to permit completely clear-cut categorization. Similarly, it cannot be ruled out that to some extent the differentiation into “core concepts of risk perception” and “side aspects” or “risk-related aspects” suffered from misclassification. This might, for instance, have happened in cases where authors explicitly labeled their ratings on probability of infection as their measures of risk perception but then also measured severity aspects without further comment on whether this was conceived of as a part of risk perception or not. As most studies provided no theoretical definition or elaboration on how risk perception was conceptualized, it cannot be ruled out that in some cases the apparent distinction in measurement was not a deliberate attempt at conceptual differentiation. Likewise, in some instances where studies were published in rather brief formats, it cannot be excluded that the empirical work actually was theory-based, but, due to extremely short background sections in some articles, this might not have been discernible.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="Sec24" sec-type="conclusion">
<title>Conclusions</title>
<p>In a recent study, Wu et al. [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR73">73</xref>
] put forward an epidemiological model which tested pathways to reduce the impact of pandemic influenza by household interventions. The authors reported that already moderate levels of behavioral compliance would bring about substantial population-level benefits and also that individual decisions which would influence compliance might differ widely. The authors concluded that there is a clear need for psychobehavioral surveillance studies to estimate levels of compliance and to identify factors which would influence the decisions of individuals and households.</p>
<p>The presently reviewed body of studies provides preliminary evidence with regard to one such factor, i.e., risk perception, and is valuable insofar as the studies offer first insights into peoples’ reaction to new or reemerging health threats such as SARS or avian influenza. However, in order to gather more systematic and compelling data, which can be compared and evaluated within the context of others studies, and in order to build a sound evidence base, future studies in the field positively need to carry out more theoretical groundwork which can guide the development and choice of risk perception concepts and measurements as well as study designs. It was one of the most surprising findings of this review that the risk perception label was often used with little explicit explanation of what it stood for, as if risk perception were a self-evident and homogeneous concept. However, closer scrutiny of the studies revealed that this common label actually covered quite different notions — a, which would make it extremely difficult to draw any generalized conclusions from the resulting body of data. In view of this finding, it might be worth striving for a working consensus on indispensable core measures of risk perception, particularly for future surveillance and monitoring studies. As stated above, many of the studies were initiated in an ad hoc fashion as an acute response to outbreaks of SARS and human H5N1 cases and not as an explicit attempt at testing theory. However, the call for model-based risk perception research [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR15">15</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR74">74</xref>
] does not exclusively aim at benefiting theoretical knowledge about what “is” and what determines risk perception. A thorough understanding of how perceptions of risk develop and in what way and by which mechanisms they influence behavior is an indispensable prerequisite for the development of effective health risk messages and for promoting protective behaviors in case of an outbreak. To define the purpose of a study as predominantly pragmatic in terms of wanting to provide data for planning does not imply nonnecessity for theoretical groundwork. Otherwise, the data gathered might be difficult to interpret and of limited value, not at least for the practical purpose they were intended for in the first place.</p>
<p>“The threat of an influenza pandemic is, at present, one of the most significant public health issues our nation and world faces” - so a statement of the US Commissioner of Food and Drugs, Andrew von Eschenbach in a press release from April 2007 [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR75">75</xref>
]. While some medical advances have been made, thus for instance a preliminary vaccine has been developed to counteract human-to-human transmission of H5N1, there is still agreement that an initial outbreak cannot be controlled by medical strategies alone. Apart from the fact that the vaccine offers only limited protection, concerns have also been voiced that the medication which has predominantly been stockpiled so far, i.e., Tamiflu (oseltamivir), might be meeting with increasing resistance [
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR76">76</xref>
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="CR78">78</xref>
]. Also, it is by no means certain that the next epidemic actually will be a H5N1 outbreak, or if another new virus will emerge on the scene. Thus, traditional public health containment strategies will still be a centerpiece of preparedness planning. To provide a more profound basis for this type of planning and to enable effective health communication, research on pandemic influenza risk perception will have to progress from its current preliminary ad hoc stage into a more systematic, methodological, and theory-based endeavor.</p>
</sec>
</body>
<back>
<fn-group>
<fn id="Fn1">
<label>1</label>
<p>The term pandemic influenza will be used as a generic term for life-threatening respiratory infectious diseases with epidemic–pandemic potential, of which SARS and H5NI are only two examples, which might be succeeded by further outbreaks of other viruses.</p>
</fn>
<fn id="Fn2">
<label>2</label>
<p>In two cases, publications clearly seemed to be based on the same study. These were therefore counted as one study each; however, in both cases, for the information in Table 
<xref rid="Tab1" ref-type="table">1</xref>
, all four articles were used.</p>
</fn>
<fn id="Fn3">
<label>3</label>
<p>For the report on the number of studies within different categories of operationalization, it should be noted (1) that the categories are not always mutually exclusive, i.e., as many studies contained more than one measure of risk, the respective studies were also counted in more than one category, (2) that unless otherwise specified the numbers and percentages given pertain to the quantitative studies only (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 26), while results of the qualitative studies (
<italic>N</italic>
 = 2) will be reported separately.</p>
</fn>
</fn-group>
<ref-list id="Bib1">
<title>References</title>
<ref id="CR1">
<label>1.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>RD</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Responding to global infectious disease outbreaks: lessons from SARS on the role of risk perception, communication and management</article-title>
<source>Soc Sci Med</source>
<year>2006</year>
<volume>63</volume>
<fpage>3113</fpage>
<lpage>3133</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.08.004</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16978751</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR2">
<label>2.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Savage</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>The foundations of statistics</source>
<year>1954</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Wiley</publisher-name>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR3">
<label>3.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fischhoff</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Slovic</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lichtenstein</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Read</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Combs</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>How safe is safe enough? A psychometric study of attitudes towards technological risks and benefits</article-title>
<source>Policy Sci</source>
<year>1978</year>
<volume>9</volume>
<fpage>127</fpage>
<lpage>152</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/BF00143739</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR4">
<label>4.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Slovic</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>The perception of risk</source>
<year>2000</year>
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Earthscan</publisher-name>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR5">
<label>5.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rosenstock</surname>
<given-names>IM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Why people use health services</article-title>
<source>Milbank Mem Fund Q</source>
<year>1966</year>
<volume>44</volume>
<issue>Suppl</issue>
<fpage>127</fpage>
<lpage>136</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR6">
<label>6.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Becker</surname>
<given-names>MH</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rosenstock</surname>
<given-names>IM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Ward</surname>
<given-names>WB</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Comparing social learning theory and the health belief model</article-title>
<source>Advances in health education and promotion</source>
<year>1987</year>
<publisher-loc>Greenwich</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>JAI</publisher-name>
<fpage>245</fpage>
<lpage>249</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR7">
<label>7.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rogers</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>A protection motivation theory of fear appeals and attitude change</article-title>
<source>J Psychol</source>
<year>1975</year>
<volume>91</volume>
<fpage>93</fpage>
<lpage>114</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">28136248</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR8">
<label>8.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rogers</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Attitude change and information integration in fear appeals</article-title>
<source>Psychol Rep</source>
<year>1985</year>
<volume>56</volume>
<fpage>182</fpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR9">
<label>9.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Chaiken</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Zanna</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Olson</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Herman</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The heuristic model of persuasion</article-title>
<source>Social influence: the Ontario Symposium</source>
<year>1987</year>
<edition>5</edition>
<publisher-loc>Hillsdale</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Erlbaum</publisher-name>
<fpage>3</fpage>
<lpage>39</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR10">
<label>10.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Petty</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cacioppo</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Berkowitz</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion</article-title>
<source>Advances in experimental social psychology</source>
<year>1986</year>
<publisher-loc>Burlington</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Academic</publisher-name>
<fpage>123</fpage>
<lpage>205</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR11">
<label>11.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Damasio</surname>
<given-names>AR</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain</source>
<year>1994</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Avon</publisher-name>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR12">
<label>12.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Forgas</surname>
<given-names>JP</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Affect in social judgments and decisions: a multiprocess model</article-title>
<source>Adv Exp Soc Psychol</source>
<year>1992</year>
<volume>25</volume>
<fpage>227</fpage>
<lpage>275</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60285-3</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR13">
<label>13.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Forgas</surname>
<given-names>JP</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Mood and judgment: the affect infusion model (AIM)</article-title>
<source>Psychol Bull</source>
<year>1995</year>
<volume>177</volume>
<fpage>39</fpage>
<lpage>66</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.39</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR14">
<label>14.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schwarz</surname>
<given-names>N</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Sorrentino</surname>
<given-names>RM</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Higgins</surname>
<given-names>ET</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Feelings as information: informational and motivational functions of affective states</article-title>
<source>Handbook of motivation and cognition: foundations of social behaviour</source>
<year>1990</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Guilford</publisher-name>
<fpage>527</fpage>
<lpage>561</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR15">
<label>15.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Finucane</surname>
<given-names>ML</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Holup</surname>
<given-names>JL</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Psychosocial and cultural factors affecting the perceived risk of genetically modified food: an overview of the literature</article-title>
<source>Soc Sci Med</source>
<year>2006</year>
<volume>60</volume>
<fpage>1603</fpage>
<lpage>1612</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.08.007</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR16">
<label>16.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Finucane</surname>
<given-names>ML</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Peters</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Slovic</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Schneider</surname>
<given-names>SL</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Shanteau</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Judgment and decision making: the dance of affect and reason</article-title>
<source>Emerging perspectives on judgment and decision research</source>
<year>2003</year>
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
<fpage>326</fpage>
<lpage>364</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR17">
<label>17.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Loewenstein</surname>
<given-names>GF</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Weber</surname>
<given-names>EU</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hsee</surname>
<given-names>CK</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Welch</surname>
<given-names>N</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Risk as feelings</article-title>
<source>Psychol Bull</source>
<year>2001</year>
<volume>127</volume>
<fpage>267</fpage>
<lpage>286</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.267</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11316014</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR18">
<label>18.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Finucane</surname>
<given-names>ML</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Alkahani</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Slovic</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Johnson</surname>
<given-names>SM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The affect heuristic in judgements of risks and benefits</article-title>
<source>J Behav Decis Mak</source>
<year>2000</year>
<volume>13</volume>
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>17</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1002/(SICI)1099-0771(200001/03)13:1<1::AID-BDM333>3.0.CO;2-S</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR19">
<label>19.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Slovic</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Finucane</surname>
<given-names>ML</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Peters</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>MacGregor</surname>
<given-names>DG</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality</article-title>
<source>Risk Anal</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>24</volume>
<fpage>311</fpage>
<lpage>322</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00433.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15078302</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR20">
<label>20.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lazarus</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Folkman</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>Stress, appraisal and coping</source>
<year>1984</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Springer</publisher-name>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR21">
<label>21.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Roseman</surname>
<given-names>IJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Antoniou</surname>
<given-names>AA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jose</surname>
<given-names>PE</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Appraisal determinants of emotion: constructing a more accurate and comprehensive theory</article-title>
<source>Cogn Emot</source>
<year>1996</year>
<volume>10</volume>
<fpage>241</fpage>
<lpage>277</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/026999396380240</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR22">
<label>22.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Irwin</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wynne</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Irwin</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wynne</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Conclusions</article-title>
<source>Misunderstanding science: the public reconstruction of science and technology</source>
<year>1996</year>
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
<fpage>213</fpage>
<lpage>221</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR23">
<label>23.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Moscovici</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Deaux</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Philogene</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Why a theory of social representations</article-title>
<source>Representations of the social</source>
<year>2001</year>
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Blackwell</publisher-name>
<fpage>8</fpage>
<lpage>35</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR24">
<label>24.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kasperson</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kasperson</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The social amplification and attenuation of risk</article-title>
<source>Ann Am Acad Polit Soc Sci</source>
<year>1996</year>
<volume>545</volume>
<fpage>95</fpage>
<lpage>105</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0002716296545001010</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR25">
<label>25.</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="other">Pidgeon RE, Kasperson RE, Slovic P, editors. The social amplification of risk. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2003.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR26">
<label>26.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Brewer</surname>
<given-names>NT</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Weinstein</surname>
<given-names>ND</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cuite</surname>
<given-names>CL</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Herrington</surname>
<given-names>JE</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Risk perceptions and their relation to risk behavior</article-title>
<source>Annals Behav Med</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>27</volume>
<fpage>125</fpage>
<lpage>130</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1207/s15324796abm2702_7</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR27">
<label>27.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Brewer</surname>
<given-names>NT</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Chapman</surname>
<given-names>GB</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gibbons</surname>
<given-names>FX</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gerrard McCaul</surname>
<given-names>KD</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Weinstein</surname>
<given-names>ND</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Meta-analysis of the relationship between risk perception and health behavior: the example of vaccination</article-title>
<source>Health Psychol</source>
<year>2007</year>
<volume>26</volume>
<fpage>136</fpage>
<lpage>145</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0278-6133.26.2.136</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17385964</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR28">
<label>28.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ajzen</surname>
<given-names>I</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The theory of planned behavior</article-title>
<source>Org Behav Human Decis Process</source>
<year>1991</year>
<volume>50</volume>
<fpage>179</fpage>
<lpage>211</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR29">
<label>29.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fishbein</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ajzen</surname>
<given-names>I</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: an introduction to theory and research</source>
<year>1975</year>
<publisher-loc>Reading</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Addison Wesley</publisher-name>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR30">
<label>30.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Brown</surname>
<given-names>SL</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Relationships between risk-taking behaviour and subsequent risk perceptions</article-title>
<source>Br J Psychol</source>
<year>2005</year>
<volume>96</volume>
<fpage>155</fpage>
<lpage>164</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1348/000712605X36703</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15969828</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR31">
<label>31.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Eiser</surname>
<given-names>JR</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Miles</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Frewer</surname>
<given-names>LJ</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Trust, perceived risk, and attitudes toward food technologies</article-title>
<source>J Appl Soc Psychol</source>
<year>2002</year>
<volume>32</volume>
<fpage>2423</fpage>
<lpage>2433</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb01871.x</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR32">
<label>32.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cava</surname>
<given-names>MA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fay</surname>
<given-names>KE</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Beanlands</surname>
<given-names>HJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McCay</surname>
<given-names>EA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wignall</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Risk perception and compliance with quarantine during the SARS outbreak</article-title>
<source>J Nurs Sch</source>
<year>2005</year>
<volume>37</volume>
<fpage>343</fpage>
<lpage>347</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1547-5069.2005.00059.x</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR33">
<label>33.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rambaldini</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wilson</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rath</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lin</surname>
<given-names>Y</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gold</surname>
<given-names>WL</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kapral</surname>
<given-names>MK</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
<article-title>The impact of severe acute respiratory syndrome on medical house staff: a qualitative study</article-title>
<source>J Gen Intern Med</source>
<year>2005</year>
<volume>20</volume>
<fpage>381</fpage>
<lpage>385</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1525-1497.2005.0099.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15963157</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR34">
<label>34.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Chong</surname>
<given-names>M-Y</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wang</surname>
<given-names>W-C</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hsieh</surname>
<given-names>W-C</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lee</surname>
<given-names>C-Y</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Chiu</surname>
<given-names>N-M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Yeh</surname>
<given-names>W-C</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
<article-title>Psychological impact of severe acute respiratory syndrome on health workers in a tertiary hospital</article-title>
<source>Br J Psychiatry</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>185</volume>
<fpage>127</fpage>
<lpage>133</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1192/bjp.185.2.127</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15286063</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR35">
<label>35.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Imai</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Takahashi</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hoshuyama</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hasegawa</surname>
<given-names>N</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lim</surname>
<given-names>M-K</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Koh</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>SARS risk perceptions in healthcare workers, Japan</article-title>
<source>Emerg Infect Dis</source>
<year>2005</year>
<volume>11</volume>
<fpage>404</fpage>
<lpage>140</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15757555</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR36">
<label>36.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tam</surname>
<given-names>DKP</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lee</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lee</surname>
<given-names>SS</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Impact of SARS on avian influenza preparedness in healthcare workers</article-title>
<source>Infection</source>
<year>2007</year>
<volume>35</volume>
<fpage>320</fpage>
<lpage>325</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s15010-007-6353-z</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17882357</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR37">
<label>37.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Shiao</surname>
<given-names>JS-C</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Koh</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lo</surname>
<given-names>L-H</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lim</surname>
<given-names>M-K</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Guo</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Factors predicting nurses’ consideration of leaving their job during the SARS outbreak</article-title>
<source>Nurs Ethics</source>
<year>2007</year>
<volume>14</volume>
<fpage>5</fpage>
<lpage>17</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0969733007071350</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17334166</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR38">
<label>38.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tang</surname>
<given-names>CSK</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wong</surname>
<given-names>C-Y</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Factors influencing the wearing of facemasks to prevent the severe acute respiratory syndrome among adult Chinese in Hong Kong</article-title>
<source>Prev Med</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>39</volume>
<fpage>1187</fpage>
<lpage>1193</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.04.032</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15539054</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR39">
<label>39.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tang</surname>
<given-names>CSK</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wong</surname>
<given-names>C-Y</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Psychosocial factors influencing the practice of preventive behaviors against the severe acute respiratory syndrome among older Chinese in Hong Kong</article-title>
<source>J Aging Health</source>
<year>2005</year>
<volume>17</volume>
<fpage>490</fpage>
<lpage>506</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0898264305277966</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16020576</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR40">
<label>40.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>deZwart</surname>
<given-names>O</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Veldhuijzen</surname>
<given-names>IK</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Elam</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Aro</surname>
<given-names>AR</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Abraham</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bishop</surname>
<given-names>GD</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
<article-title>Avian influenza risk perception, Europe and Asia</article-title>
<source>Emerg Infect Dis</source>
<year>2007</year>
<volume>13</volume>
<fpage>290</fpage>
<lpage>293</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3201/eid1302.060303</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17479894</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR41">
<label>41.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lau</surname>
<given-names>JTF</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Yang</surname>
<given-names>X</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wong</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tsui</surname>
<given-names>H</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Prevalence and factors associated with social avoidance of recovered SARS patients in the Hong Kong general population</article-title>
<source>Health Educ Res</source>
<year>2006</year>
<volume>21</volume>
<fpage>662</fpage>
<lpage>673</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1093/her/cyl064</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16880214</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR42">
<label>42.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ji</surname>
<given-names>L-J</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zhang</surname>
<given-names>Z</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Usborne</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Guan</surname>
<given-names>Y</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Optimism across cultures: in response to the SARS outbreak</article-title>
<source>Asian J Soc Psychol</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>7</volume>
<fpage>25</fpage>
<lpage>34</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1467-839X.2004.00132.x</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR43">
<label>43.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fielding</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lam</surname>
<given-names>WW</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ho</surname>
<given-names>EY</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lam</surname>
<given-names>H</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hedley</surname>
<given-names>AJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Leung</surname>
<given-names>GM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Avian influenza risk perception, Hong Kong</article-title>
<source>Emerg Infect Dis</source>
<year>2005</year>
<volume>11</volume>
<fpage>677</fpage>
<lpage>682</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15890118</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR44">
<label>44.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Brug</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Aro</surname>
<given-names>AR</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Oenema</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>de Zwart</surname>
<given-names>O</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Richardus</surname>
<given-names>JH</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bishop</surname>
<given-names>GD</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>SARS risk perception, knowledge, precautions, and information sources, the Netherlands</article-title>
<source>Emerg Infect Dis</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>10</volume>
<fpage>1486</fpage>
<lpage>1489</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15496256</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR45">
<label>45.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Leung</surname>
<given-names>GM</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ho</surname>
<given-names>L-M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Chan</surname>
<given-names>SKK</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ho</surname>
<given-names>S-Y</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bacon-Shone</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Choy</surname>
<given-names>RYL</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
<article-title>Longitudinal assessment of community psychobehavioral responses during and after the 2003 outbreak of severe acute respiratory syndrome in Hong Kong</article-title>
<source>Clin Infect Dis</source>
<year>2005</year>
<volume>40</volume>
<fpage>1713</fpage>
<lpage>1720</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1086/429923</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15909256</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR46">
<label>46.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Nickell</surname>
<given-names>LA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Crighton</surname>
<given-names>EJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tracy</surname>
<given-names>CS</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Al-Enazy</surname>
<given-names>H</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bolaji</surname>
<given-names>Y</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hanjrah</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
<article-title>Psychosocial effects of SARS on hospital staff: survey of a large tertiary care institution</article-title>
<source>Can Med Assoc J</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>170</volume>
<fpage>793</fpage>
<lpage>798</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1503/cmaj.1031077</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14993174</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR47">
<label>47.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kristiansen</surname>
<given-names>IS</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Halvorsen</surname>
<given-names>PA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gyrd-Hansen</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Influenza pandemic: perception of risk and individual precautions in a general population. Cross-sectional study</article-title>
<source>BMC Public Health</source>
<year>2007</year>
<volume>7</volume>
<fpage>48</fpage>
<lpage>54</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1186/1471-2458-7-48</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17407563</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR48">
<label>48.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lau</surname>
<given-names>JTF</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Yang</surname>
<given-names>X</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tsui</surname>
<given-names>HY</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pang</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>SARS related preventive and risk behaviours practised by Hong Kong-mainland China cross border travellers during the outbreak of the SARS epidemic in Hong Kong</article-title>
<source>J Epidemiol Community Health</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>58</volume>
<fpage>988</fpage>
<lpage>996</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1136/jech.2003.017483</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15547057</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR49">
<label>49.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Leung</surname>
<given-names>GM</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Quah</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ho</surname>
<given-names>L-M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ho</surname>
<given-names>S-Y</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hedley</surname>
<given-names>AJ</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lee</surname>
<given-names>H-P</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
<article-title>A tale of two cities: community psychobehavioral surveillance and related impact on outbreak control in Hong Kong and Singapore during the severe acute respiratory syndrome epidemic</article-title>
<source>Infect Control Hosp Epidemiol</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>25</volume>
<fpage>1033</fpage>
<lpage>1041</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1086/502340</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15636289</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR50">
<label>50.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Abbate</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Di Giuseppe</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Marinelli</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Angelillo</surname>
<given-names>IF</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Knowledge, attitudes, and practices of avian influenza, poultry workers, Italy</article-title>
<source>Emerg Infect Dis</source>
<year>2006</year>
<volume>12</volume>
<fpage>1762</fpage>
<lpage>1765</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17283632</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR51">
<label>51.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lau</surname>
<given-names>JTF</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Yang</surname>
<given-names>X</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tsui</surname>
<given-names>HY</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pang</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kim</surname>
<given-names>JH</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>SARS preventive and risk behaviours of Hong Kong air travellers</article-title>
<source>Epidemiol Infect</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>132</volume>
<fpage>727</fpage>
<lpage>736</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1017/S0950268804002225</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15310175</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR52">
<label>52.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Leung</surname>
<given-names>GM</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lam</surname>
<given-names>T-H</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ho</surname>
<given-names>L-M</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ho</surname>
<given-names>S-Y</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Chan</surname>
<given-names>BHY</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wong</surname>
<given-names>IOL</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
<article-title>The impact of community psychological responses on outbreak control for severe acute respiratory syndrome in Hong Kong</article-title>
<source>J Epidemiol Community Health</source>
<year>2003</year>
<volume>57</volume>
<fpage>857</fpage>
<lpage>863</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1136/jech.57.11.857</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14600110</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR53">
<label>53.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hong</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Collins</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Societal responses to familiar versus unfamiliar risk: comparisons of influenza and SARS in Korea</article-title>
<source>Risk Anal</source>
<year>2006</year>
<volume>26</volume>
<fpage>1247</fpage>
<lpage>1257</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1539-6924.2006.00812.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17054529</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR54">
<label>54.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Koh</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lim</surname>
<given-names>MK</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Chia</surname>
<given-names>SE</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ko</surname>
<given-names>SM</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Qian</surname>
<given-names>F</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ng</surname>
<given-names>V</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
<article-title>Risk perception and impact of Severe Acute Respiratory Syndrome (SARS) on work and personal lives of healthcare workers in Singapore: What can we learn?</article-title>
<source>Health Med Care Serv Rev</source>
<year>2005</year>
<volume>43</volume>
<fpage>676</fpage>
<lpage>682</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR55">
<label>55.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Koh</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Takahashi</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lim</surname>
<given-names>MK</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Imai</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Chia</surname>
<given-names>SE</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Qian</surname>
<given-names>F</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
<article-title>SARS risk perception and preventive measures, Singapore and Japan</article-title>
<source>Emerg Infect Dis</source>
<year>2005</year>
<volume>11</volume>
<fpage>641</fpage>
<lpage>642</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15834989</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR56">
<label>56.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lau</surname>
<given-names>JTF</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Yang</surname>
<given-names>X</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tsui</surname>
<given-names>H</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kim</surname>
<given-names>JH</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Monitoring community responses to the SARS epidemic in Hong Kong: from day 10 to day 62</article-title>
<source>J Epidemiol Community Health</source>
<year>2003</year>
<volume>57</volume>
<fpage>864</fpage>
<lpage>870</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1136/jech.57.11.864</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14600111</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR57">
<label>57.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Chang</surname>
<given-names>C-S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Du</surname>
<given-names>P-L</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Huang</surname>
<given-names>I-C</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Nurses’ perceptions of severe acute respiratory syndrome: relationship between commitment and intention to leave nursing</article-title>
<source>J Adv Nurs</source>
<year>2006</year>
<volume>54</volume>
<fpage>171</fpage>
<lpage>179</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1365-2648.2006.03796.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16553703</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR58">
<label>58.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Weinstein</surname>
<given-names>ND</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Optimistic biases about personal risks</article-title>
<source>Science</source>
<year>1989</year>
<volume>8</volume>
<fpage>1232</fpage>
<lpage>1233</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1126/science.2686031</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR59">
<label>59.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bandura</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>Self-efficacy: the exercise of control</source>
<year>1997</year>
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Freeman</publisher-name>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR60">
<label>60.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Weinstein</surname>
<given-names>ND</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sandman</surname>
<given-names>PM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>A model of the precaution adoption process: evidence form home radon testing</article-title>
<source>Health Psychol</source>
<year>1992</year>
<volume>11</volume>
<fpage>170</fpage>
<lpage>180</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0278-6133.11.3.170</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">1618171</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR61">
<label>61.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>VK</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Johnson</surname>
<given-names>FR</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>How do risk perceptions respond to information: the case of radon</article-title>
<source>Rev Econ Stat</source>
<year>1988</year>
<volume>70</volume>
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>8</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/1928144</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR62">
<label>62.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Viscusi</surname>
<given-names>WK</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Evans</surname>
<given-names>WN</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Estimation of revealed probabilities and utility functions for product safety decisions</article-title>
<source>Rev Econ Stat</source>
<year>1998</year>
<volume>80</volume>
<fpage>28</fpage>
<lpage>33</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1162/003465398557302</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR63">
<label>63.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tang</surname>
<given-names>CSK</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wong</surname>
<given-names>C-Y</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>An outbreak of the severe acute respiratory syndrome: predictors of health behaviours and effect of community prevention measures in Hong Kong, China</article-title>
<source>Am J Public Health</source>
<year>2003</year>
<volume>93</volume>
<fpage>1887</fpage>
<lpage>1888</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2105/AJPH.93.11.1887</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14600058</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR64">
<label>64.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kobbeltveld</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brun</surname>
<given-names>W</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Johnsen</surname>
<given-names>BH</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Eid</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Risk as feelings or risk and feelings? A cross-lagged panel analysis</article-title>
<source>J Risk Res</source>
<year>2005</year>
<volume>8</volume>
<fpage>417</fpage>
<lpage>437</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/1366987042000315519</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR65">
<label>65.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Borkovec</surname>
<given-names>TD</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Robinson</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pruzinsky</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>DePree</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Preliminary exploration of worry, some characteristics and processes</article-title>
<source>Behav Res Ther</source>
<year>1983</year>
<volume>21</volume>
<fpage>9</fpage>
<lpage>16</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0005-7967(83)90121-3</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">6830571</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR66">
<label>66.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Morris</surname>
<given-names>LW</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Davis</surname>
<given-names>MA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hutchings</surname>
<given-names>CH</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Cognitive and emotional components of anxiety: literature review and a revised worry-emotionality scale</article-title>
<source>J Educ Psychol</source>
<year>1981</year>
<volume>73</volume>
<fpage>541</fpage>
<lpage>555</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-0663.73.4.541</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">7024371</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR67">
<label>67.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Harris</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Middleton</surname>
<given-names>W</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The illusion of control and optimism about health: on being less at risk but no more in control than others</article-title>
<source>Br J Soc Psychol</source>
<year>1994</year>
<volume>33</volume>
<fpage>3269</fpage>
<lpage>3386</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR68">
<label>68.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Siu</surname>
<given-names>JY-M</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The SARS-associated stigma of SARS victims in the post SARS era of Hong Kong</article-title>
<source>Qual Health Res</source>
<year>2008</year>
<volume>18</volume>
<fpage>729</fpage>
<lpage>738</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/1049732308318372</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18503014</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR69">
<label>69.</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="other">Yj S. From an epidemic to mass panic: the role of social and cultural elements in infection control of SARS in Hong Kong. Paper presented at the 38th APACPH (Asia-Pacific Academic Consortium for Public Health) Conference 2006 Bangkok, Thailand; 2006.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR70">
<label>70.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lai</surname>
<given-names>JC</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tao</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Perception of environmental hazards in Hong Kong Chinese</article-title>
<source>Risk Anal</source>
<year>2003</year>
<volume>23</volume>
<fpage>669</fpage>
<lpage>684</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/1539-6924.00346</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12926561</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR71">
<label>71.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Beck</surname>
<given-names>U</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>Risk society: towards a new modernity</source>
<year>1992</year>
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Sage</publisher-name>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR72">
<label>72.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Beck</surname>
<given-names>U</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<source>World risk society</source>
<year>1999</year>
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
<publisher-name>Polity Press</publisher-name>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR73">
<label>73.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wu</surname>
<given-names>JT</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Riley</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fraser</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Leung</surname>
<given-names>GM</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Reducing the impact of the next influenza pandemic using household-based public health interventions</article-title>
<source>PLoS Med</source>
<year>2006</year>
<volume>3</volume>
<fpage>e361</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pmed.0030361</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16881729</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR74">
<label>74.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Weber</surname>
<given-names>EU</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hsee</surname>
<given-names>CK</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Models and mosaics: investigating cross-cultural differences in risk perception and risk preference</article-title>
<source>Psychon Bull Rev</source>
<year>1999</year>
<volume>6</volume>
<fpage>611</fpage>
<lpage>617</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10682203</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR75">
<label>75.</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="other">FDA/US Food and Drug Administration. FDA approves first U.S. vaccine for humans against the avian influenza virus H5N1. FDA News P07-68
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.fda.gov/bbs/topics/NEWS/2007/NEW01611.html">http://www.fda.gov/bbs/topics/NEWS/2007/NEW01611.html</ext-link>
(2007). Accessed June 8, 2008.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR76">
<label>76.</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="other">Eurosurveillance. Eurosurveillance update.
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.eurosurveillance.org/edition/v13n06/080207_3.asp">http://www.eurosurveillance.org/edition/v13n06/080207_3.asp</ext-link>
(2008). Accessed June 8, 2008.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR77">
<label>77.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Moscona</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Oseltamivir resistance–disabling our influenza defenses</article-title>
<source>N Engl J Med</source>
<year>2005</year>
<volume>353</volume>
<fpage>2633</fpage>
<lpage>2636</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1056/NEJMp058291</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16371626</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR78">
<label>78.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Moscona</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Medical management of influenza infection</article-title>
<source>Annu Rev Med</source>
<year>2008</year>
<volume>59</volume>
<fpage>397</fpage>
<lpage>413</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1146/annurev.med.59.061506.213121</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17939760</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR79">
<label>79.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Singer</surname>
<given-names>AC</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nunn</surname>
<given-names>MA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gould</surname>
<given-names>EA</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Johnson</surname>
<given-names>AC</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Potential risks associated with the proposed widespread use of Tamiflu</article-title>
<source>Environ Health Perspect</source>
<year>2007</year>
<volume>115</volume>
<fpage>102</fpage>
<lpage>106</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1289/ehp.9574</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17366827</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR80">
<label>80.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Barennes</surname>
<given-names>H</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Martinez-Aussel</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Vongphrachanh</surname>
<given-names>P</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Strobel</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Avian influenza risk perceptions, Laos</article-title>
<source>Emerg Infect Dis</source>
<year>2007</year>
<volume>13</volume>
<fpage>1126</fpage>
<lpage>1128</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18214204</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR81">
<label>81.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Peltz</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Avisar-Shohat</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bar-Dayan</surname>
<given-names>Y</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Difference in public emotions, interest, sense of knowledge and compliance between the affected area and the nationwide general population during the first phase of a bird flu outbreak in Israel</article-title>
<source>J Infect</source>
<year>2007</year>
<volume>55</volume>
<fpage>545</fpage>
<lpage>550</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jinf.2007.07.014</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17826838</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="CR82">
<label>82.</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Quah</surname>
<given-names>SR</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hin-Peng</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Crisis prevention and management during SARS outbreak, Singapore</article-title>
<source>Emerg Infect Dis</source>
<year>2004</year>
<volume>10</volume>
<fpage>364</fpage>
<lpage>368</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15030714</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</pmc>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Sante/explor/SrasV1/Data/Pmc/Corpus
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 000543 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Corpus/biblio.hfd -nk 000543 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Sante
   |area=    SrasV1
   |flux=    Pmc
   |étape=   Corpus
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     PMC:7090865
   |texte=   Risk Perceptions Related to SARS and Avian Influenza: Theoretical Foundations of Current Empirical Research
}}

Pour générer des pages wiki

HfdIndexSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Corpus/RBID.i   -Sk "pubmed:19214752" \
       | HfdSelect -Kh $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Corpus/biblio.hfd   \
       | NlmPubMed2Wicri -a SrasV1 

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.33.
Data generation: Tue Apr 28 14:49:16 2020. Site generation: Sat Mar 27 22:06:49 2021