Serveur d'exploration MERS

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

Middle East respiratory syndrome risk perception among students at a university in South Korea, 2015

Identifieur interne : 000A37 ( Pmc/Corpus ); précédent : 000A36; suivant : 000A38

Middle East respiratory syndrome risk perception among students at a university in South Korea, 2015

Auteurs : Seongwoo Yang ; Sung-Il Cho

Source :

RBID : PMC:7115287

Abstract

Background

The 2015 Middle East respiratory syndrome (MERS) outbreak in South Korea was a serious threat to public health, and was exacerbated by the inappropriate responses of major institutions and the public. This study examined the sources of confusion during the MERS outbreak and identified the factors that can affect people's behavior.

Methods

An online survey of the risk perception of university students in South Korea was performed after the epidemic had peaked. The questionnaire addressed the major social determinants in South Korea during the MERS epidemic. The analysis included data from 1,470 subjects who provided complete answers.

Results

The students had 53.5% of the essential knowledge about MERS. Women showed higher risk perception than men, and trust in the media was positively associated with risk perception (P < .001). Additionally, risk perception was positively associated with overreaction by the public (odds ratio, 2.80; 95% confidence interval, 2.17-3.60; P < .001). These findings suggest that media content affected the public's perception of MERS risk and that perception of a high level of risk led to overreaction.

Conclusions

Risk perception was associated with most of the social factors examined and overreaction by the public. Therefore, providing accurate information and data to the public, establishing trust, and facilitating the development of an attitude will all be important in future crises.


Url:
DOI: 10.1016/j.ajic.2017.02.013
PubMed: 28385465
PubMed Central: 7115287

Links to Exploration step

PMC:7115287

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI>
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">Middle East respiratory syndrome risk perception among students at a university in South Korea, 2015</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yang, Seongwoo" sort="Yang, Seongwoo" uniqKey="Yang S" first="Seongwoo" last="Yang">Seongwoo Yang</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="af9000">Department of Public Health Science, Graduate School of Public Health, Seoul National University, Seoul, Republic of Korea</nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cho, Sung Il" sort="Cho, Sung Il" uniqKey="Cho S" first="Sung-Il" last="Cho">Sung-Il Cho</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="af9005">Department of Public Health Science, Graduate School of Public Health and Institute of Health and Environment, Seoul National University, Seoul, Republic of Korea</nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">PMC</idno>
<idno type="pmid">28385465</idno>
<idno type="pmc">7115287</idno>
<idno type="url">http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7115287</idno>
<idno type="RBID">PMC:7115287</idno>
<idno type="doi">10.1016/j.ajic.2017.02.013</idno>
<date when="2017">2017</date>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Corpus">000A37</idno>
<idno type="wicri:explorRef" wicri:stream="Pmc" wicri:step="Corpus" wicri:corpus="PMC">000A37</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title xml:lang="en" level="a" type="main">Middle East respiratory syndrome risk perception among students at a university in South Korea, 2015</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yang, Seongwoo" sort="Yang, Seongwoo" uniqKey="Yang S" first="Seongwoo" last="Yang">Seongwoo Yang</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="af9000">Department of Public Health Science, Graduate School of Public Health, Seoul National University, Seoul, Republic of Korea</nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cho, Sung Il" sort="Cho, Sung Il" uniqKey="Cho S" first="Sung-Il" last="Cho">Sung-Il Cho</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="af9005">Department of Public Health Science, Graduate School of Public Health and Institute of Health and Environment, Seoul National University, Seoul, Republic of Korea</nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<series>
<title level="j">American Journal of Infection Control</title>
<idno type="ISSN">0196-6553</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1527-3296</idno>
<imprint>
<date when="2017">2017</date>
</imprint>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">
<sec>
<title>Background</title>
<p>The 2015 Middle East respiratory syndrome (MERS) outbreak in South Korea was a serious threat to public health, and was exacerbated by the inappropriate responses of major institutions and the public. This study examined the sources of confusion during the MERS outbreak and identified the factors that can affect people's behavior.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Methods</title>
<p>An online survey of the risk perception of university students in South Korea was performed after the epidemic had peaked. The questionnaire addressed the major social determinants in South Korea during the MERS epidemic. The analysis included data from 1,470 subjects who provided complete answers.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Results</title>
<p>The students had 53.5% of the essential knowledge about MERS. Women showed higher risk perception than men, and trust in the media was positively associated with risk perception (
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001). Additionally, risk perception was positively associated with overreaction by the public (odds ratio, 2.80; 95% confidence interval, 2.17-3.60;
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001). These findings suggest that media content affected the public's perception of MERS risk and that perception of a high level of risk led to overreaction.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Conclusions</title>
<p>Risk perception was associated with most of the social factors examined and overreaction by the public. Therefore, providing accurate information and data to the public, establishing trust, and facilitating the development of an attitude will all be important in future crises.</p>
</sec>
</div>
</front>
<back>
<div1 type="bibliography">
<listBibl>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Korea Centers For Disease Control And Prevention" uniqKey="Korea Centers For Disease Control And Prevention">Korea Centers for Disease Control and Prevention</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Penttinen, P M" uniqKey="Penttinen P">P.M. Penttinen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kaasik Aaslav, K" uniqKey="Kaasik Aaslav K">K. Kaasik-Aaslav</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Friaux, A" uniqKey="Friaux A">A. Friaux</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Donachie, A" uniqKey="Donachie A">A. Donachie</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sudre, B" uniqKey="Sudre B">B. Sudre</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Amato Gauci, A J" uniqKey="Amato Gauci A">A.J. Amato-Gauci</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fung, I C" uniqKey="Fung I">I.C. Fung</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tse, Z T" uniqKey="Tse Z">Z.T. Tse</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chan, B S" uniqKey="Chan B">B.S. Chan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fu, K W" uniqKey="Fu K">K.W. Fu</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ma, R" uniqKey="Ma R">R. Ma</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="King, G" uniqKey="King G">G. King</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pan, J" uniqKey="Pan J">J. Pan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Roberts, M E" uniqKey="Roberts M">M.E. Roberts</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Keane, M" uniqKey="Keane M">M. Keane</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Donald, S H" uniqKey="Donald S">S.H. Donald</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tausczik, Y" uniqKey="Tausczik Y">Y. Tausczik</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Faasse, K" uniqKey="Faasse K">K. Faasse</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pennebaker, J W" uniqKey="Pennebaker J">J.W. Pennebaker</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Petrie, K J" uniqKey="Petrie K">K.J. Petrie</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chew, C" uniqKey="Chew C">C. Chew</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Eysenbach, G" uniqKey="Eysenbach G">G. Eysenbach</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gesser Edelsburg, A" uniqKey="Gesser Edelsburg A">A. Gesser-Edelsburg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shir Raz, Y" uniqKey="Shir Raz Y">Y. Shir-Raz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cvetkovich, G" uniqKey="Cvetkovich G">G. Cvetkovich</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Winter, P L" uniqKey="Winter P">P.L. Winter</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="World Health Organization" uniqKey="World Health Organization">World Health Organization</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fischhoff, B" uniqKey="Fischhoff B">B. Fischhoff</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bostrom, A" uniqKey="Bostrom A">A. Bostrom</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Quadrel, M J" uniqKey="Quadrel M">M.J. Quadrel</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Renn, O" uniqKey="Renn O">O. Renn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rohrmann, B" uniqKey="Rohrmann B">B. Rohrmann</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Der Pligt, J" uniqKey="Van Der Pligt J">J. Van der Pligt</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rohloff, A" uniqKey="Rohloff A">A. Rohloff</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wright, S" uniqKey="Wright S">S. Wright</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sjoberg, L" uniqKey="Sjoberg L">L. Sjöberg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Maswanya, E S" uniqKey="Maswanya E">E.S. Maswanya</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Moji, K" uniqKey="Moji K">K. Moji</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Horiguchi, I" uniqKey="Horiguchi I">I. Horiguchi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nagata, K" uniqKey="Nagata K">K. Nagata</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aoyagi, K" uniqKey="Aoyagi K">K. Aoyagi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Honda, S" uniqKey="Honda S">S. Honda</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Damme, W" uniqKey="Van Damme W">W. Van Damme</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Lerberghe, W" uniqKey="Van Lerberghe W">W. Van Lerberghe</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rubsamen, N" uniqKey="Rubsamen N">N. Rubsamen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Castell, S" uniqKey="Castell S">S. Castell</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Horn, J" uniqKey="Horn J">J. Horn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Karch, A" uniqKey="Karch A">A. Karch</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ott, J J" uniqKey="Ott J">J.J. Ott</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Raupach Rosin, H" uniqKey="Raupach Rosin H">H. Raupach-Rosin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gesser Edelsburg, A" uniqKey="Gesser Edelsburg A">A. Gesser-Edelsburg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shir Raz, Y" uniqKey="Shir Raz Y">Y. Shir-Raz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hayek, S" uniqKey="Hayek S">S. Hayek</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lev, O S B" uniqKey="Lev O">O.S.B. Lev</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Zwart, O" uniqKey="De Zwart O">O. de Zwart</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Veldhuijzen, I K" uniqKey="Veldhuijzen I">I.K. Veldhuijzen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Elam, G" uniqKey="Elam G">G. Elam</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aro, A R" uniqKey="Aro A">A.R. Aro</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Abraham, T" uniqKey="Abraham T">T. Abraham</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bishop, G D" uniqKey="Bishop G">G.D. Bishop</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Korea Centers For Disease Control And Prevention" uniqKey="Korea Centers For Disease Control And Prevention">Korea Centers for Disease Control and Prevention</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shortell, T" uniqKey="Shortell T">T. Shortell</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rhemtulla, M" uniqKey="Rhemtulla M">M. Rhemtulla</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brosseau Liard, P E" uniqKey="Brosseau Liard P">P.É. Brosseau-Liard</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Savalei, V" uniqKey="Savalei V">V. Savalei</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thomas, R K" uniqKey="Thomas R">R.K. Thomas</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jackob, N G E" uniqKey="Jackob N">N.G.E. Jackob</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kim, S" uniqKey="Kim S">S. Kim</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Irwin, A" uniqKey="Irwin A">A. Irwin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dickson, D" uniqKey="Dickson D">D. Dickson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Berry, T R" uniqKey="Berry T">T.R. Berry</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wharf Higgins, J" uniqKey="Wharf Higgins J">J. Wharf-Higgins</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Naylor, P J" uniqKey="Naylor P">P.J. Naylor</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sjoberg, L" uniqKey="Sjoberg L">L. Sjoberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Drottzsjoberg, B M" uniqKey="Drottzsjoberg B">B.M. Drottzsjoberg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stern, P C" uniqKey="Stern P">P.C. Stern</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fung, I C" uniqKey="Fung I">I.C. Fung</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tse, Z T" uniqKey="Tse Z">Z.T. Tse</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cheung, C N" uniqKey="Cheung C">C.N. Cheung</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Miu, A S" uniqKey="Miu A">A.S. Miu</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fu, K W" uniqKey="Fu K">K.W. Fu</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Altheide, D L" uniqKey="Altheide D">D.L. Altheide</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Salathe, M" uniqKey="Salathe M">M. Salathe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Khandelwal, S" uniqKey="Khandelwal S">S. Khandelwal</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yoda, L U" uniqKey="Yoda L">L.U. Yoda</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nahl, D" uniqKey="Nahl D">D. Nahl</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Crosby, M E" uniqKey="Crosby M">M.E. Crosby</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kass Hout, T A" uniqKey="Kass Hout T">T.A. Kass-Hout</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Alhinnawi, H" uniqKey="Alhinnawi H">H. Alhinnawi</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, R D" uniqKey="Smith R">R.D. Smith</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sandman, P M" uniqKey="Sandman P">P.M. Sandman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pangestu, T" uniqKey="Pangestu T">T. Pangestu</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cvetkovich, G" uniqKey="Cvetkovich G">G. Cvetkovich</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lofstedt, R E" uniqKey="Lofstedt R">R.E. Lofstedt</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lofstedt, R" uniqKey="Lofstedt R">R. Löfstedt</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nulty, D D" uniqKey="Nulty D">D.D. Nulty</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
</listBibl>
</div1>
</back>
</TEI>
<pmc article-type="research-article">
<pmc-dir>properties open_access</pmc-dir>
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="nlm-ta">Am J Infect Control</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="iso-abbrev">Am J Infect Control</journal-id>
<journal-title-group>
<journal-title>American Journal of Infection Control</journal-title>
</journal-title-group>
<issn pub-type="ppub">0196-6553</issn>
<issn pub-type="epub">1527-3296</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Association for Professionals in Infection Control and Epidemiology, Inc. Published by Elsevier Inc.</publisher-name>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmid">28385465</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmc">7115287</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="publisher-id">S0196-6553(17)30134-7</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.ajic.2017.02.013</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Article</subject>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>Middle East respiratory syndrome risk perception among students at a university in South Korea, 2015</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author" id="au9000">
<name>
<surname>Yang</surname>
<given-names>Seongwoo</given-names>
</name>
<degrees>RN, MPH</degrees>
<xref rid="af9000" ref-type="aff">a</xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author" id="au9005">
<name>
<surname>Cho</surname>
<given-names>Sung-Il</given-names>
</name>
<degrees>MD, ScD</degrees>
<email>scho@snu.ac.kr</email>
<xref rid="af9005" ref-type="aff">b</xref>
<xref rid="co9000" ref-type="corresp">*</xref>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<aff id="af9000">
<label>a</label>
Department of Public Health Science, Graduate School of Public Health, Seoul National University, Seoul, Republic of Korea</aff>
<aff id="af9005">
<label>b</label>
Department of Public Health Science, Graduate School of Public Health and Institute of Health and Environment, Seoul National University, Seoul, Republic of Korea</aff>
<author-notes>
<corresp id="co9000">
<label>*</label>
Address correspondence to Sung-Il Cho, MD, ScD, Graduate School of Public Health, Public Health Science, Seoul National University, 88026 Gwanakro 1, Gwanak-gu, Seoul, Republic of Korea. (S. Cho).
<email>scho@snu.ac.kr</email>
</corresp>
</author-notes>
<pub-date pub-type="pmc-release">
<day>3</day>
<month>4</month>
<year>2017</year>
</pub-date>
<pmc-comment> PMC Release delay is 0 months and 0 days and was based on .</pmc-comment>
<pub-date pub-type="ppub">
<day>1</day>
<month>6</month>
<year>2017</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="epub">
<day>3</day>
<month>4</month>
<year>2017</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>45</volume>
<issue>6</issue>
<fpage>e53</fpage>
<lpage>e60</lpage>
<permissions>
<copyright-statement>© 2017 Association for Professionals in Infection Control and Epidemiology, Inc. Published by Elsevier Inc.</copyright-statement>
<copyright-year>2017</copyright-year>
<copyright-holder>Association for Professionals in Infection Control and Epidemiology, Inc.</copyright-holder>
<license>
<license-p>Since January 2020 Elsevier has created a COVID-19 resource centre with free information in English and Mandarin on the novel coronavirus COVID-19. The COVID-19 resource centre is hosted on Elsevier Connect, the company's public news and information website. Elsevier hereby grants permission to make all its COVID-19-related research that is available on the COVID-19 resource centre - including this research content - immediately available in PubMed Central and other publicly funded repositories, such as the WHO COVID database with rights for unrestricted research re-use and analyses in any form or by any means with acknowledgement of the original source. These permissions are granted for free by Elsevier for as long as the COVID-19 resource centre remains active.</license-p>
</license>
</permissions>
<abstract id="ab0010">
<sec>
<title>Background</title>
<p>The 2015 Middle East respiratory syndrome (MERS) outbreak in South Korea was a serious threat to public health, and was exacerbated by the inappropriate responses of major institutions and the public. This study examined the sources of confusion during the MERS outbreak and identified the factors that can affect people's behavior.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Methods</title>
<p>An online survey of the risk perception of university students in South Korea was performed after the epidemic had peaked. The questionnaire addressed the major social determinants in South Korea during the MERS epidemic. The analysis included data from 1,470 subjects who provided complete answers.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Results</title>
<p>The students had 53.5% of the essential knowledge about MERS. Women showed higher risk perception than men, and trust in the media was positively associated with risk perception (
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001). Additionally, risk perception was positively associated with overreaction by the public (odds ratio, 2.80; 95% confidence interval, 2.17-3.60;
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001). These findings suggest that media content affected the public's perception of MERS risk and that perception of a high level of risk led to overreaction.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Conclusions</title>
<p>Risk perception was associated with most of the social factors examined and overreaction by the public. Therefore, providing accurate information and data to the public, establishing trust, and facilitating the development of an attitude will all be important in future crises.</p>
</sec>
</abstract>
<kwd-group id="kwd0010">
<title>Key Words</title>
<kwd>MERS</kwd>
<kwd>Risk perception</kwd>
<kwd>Risk communication</kwd>
<kwd>Infectious disease</kwd>
<kwd>Trust</kwd>
<kwd>Overreaction</kwd>
</kwd-group>
</article-meta>
</front>
<body>
<p id="p0010">The first Middle East respiratory syndrome (MERS) case was confirmed in South Korea on May 20, 2015. The last case was diagnosed on July 4 and summed up to a total of 186 confirmed cases, almost half of which were in Seoul. After that, South Korea officially declared the end of the MERS epidemic on December 23, 2015. During the MERS outbreak, 38 died and 16,752 had been quarantined (
<xref rid="f0010" ref-type="fig">Fig 1</xref>
).
<xref rid="bib0010" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>1</sup>
</xref>
The case fatality rate of MERS in South Korea was approximately 20.4%, lower than that on the Arabian Peninsula (approximately 45%).
<xref rid="bib0015" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>2</sup>
</xref>
<fig id="f0010">
<label>Fig 1</label>
<caption>
<p>Epidemic curve of Middle East respiratory syndrome in South Korea from May 11-July 10, 2015.</p>
</caption>
<alt-text id="atte0010">Fig 1</alt-text>
<graphic xlink:href="ymic4343-fig-0001_lrg"></graphic>
</fig>
</p>
<p id="p0015">Public apprehension was exacerbated because the government did not disclose timely information about the status of the epidemic or hospitals' names and procedures related to MERS infection. Therefore, the public were unaware of the appropriate actions to take, but were provided with relevant information by the media. Indeed, several citizens created Web sites that listed confirmed and suspected MERS patients.
<xref rid="bib0020" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>3</sup>
</xref>
However, the public also received inaccurate information from the Internet and social media; this increased the level of concern over MERS and resulted in rumors. The government of South Korea stated that any person who disseminated an untrue rumor would be prosecuted, but this failed to reduce the level of panic.</p>
<p id="p0020">Although this action was supposed to prevent secondary damage, it was similar to the censorship of the media in China, where the propaganda departments of the Chinese Communist Party directly supervised the media flow when the severe acute respiratory syndrome (SARS) outbreak occurred in 2003.
<xref rid="bib0025" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>4</sup>
</xref>
The Chinese government attempted to maintain political, social, and economic stability by minimizing the SARS crisis through the withholding of information; however, a reverse effect occurred. Nevertheless, the Chinese government assigned responsibility for censorship of the media, including the Internet, to local agencies.
<xref rid="bib0030" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>5</sup>
</xref>
In the new media age, in which social media (including the Internet, short message services, and mobile applications) are centralized, the government restricts freedom of expression in the same way as it has restricted such freedom among traditional media sources, such as newspapers, radio, and television.
<xref rid="bib0035" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>6</sup>
</xref>
The control of the acquisition of information is more difficult in this new age because social media are tools not only for the dissemination, sharing, and seeking of health information but also for the expression of feelings and the sharing of personal experiences and opinions.
<xref rid="bib0040" ref-type="bibr">7</xref>
,
<xref rid="bib0045" ref-type="bibr">8</xref>
Therefore, organizations need to build effective communication tools to respond to emerging infectious disease (EID) outbreaks because the public may express scientific skepticism about scientific topics and participation in decision-making.
<xref rid="bib0050" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>9</sup>
</xref>
</p>
<p id="p0025">During epidemics, people usually require guidance on how to behave from a trusted source. The government and public institutions are the ideal sources because people tend to rely on the national administration. For this reason, trust in these institutions plays a main role in the public's acceptance of policies and actions.
<xref rid="bib0055" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>10</sup>
</xref>
The World Health Organization suggested that outbreak communication should incorporate the following 5 key factors: (1) building, maintaining, or restoring trust; (2) announcing early; (3) maintaining transparency; (4) understanding the public; and (5) planning of all aspects of the response to an outbreak.
<xref rid="bib0060" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>11</sup>
</xref>
Therefore, risk communication enhances the decision-making ability of laypeople, and can be examined by assessing risk perception.
<xref rid="bib0065" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>12</sup>
</xref>
Trust in not only the government and public agencies but also in the media and other institutions may be associated with risk perception. Therefore, the instigation of fear among the public by the media may contribute to social panic, particularly in emergency situations.</p>
<p id="p0030">The term risk perception usually refers to individuals' judgments about and evaluations of hazards to which they might be exposed.
<xref rid="bib0070" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>13</sup>
</xref>
Therefore, risk perception might be among the social phenomena related to exposure to the risk of disease. In addition, perceived risk influences health behavior both positively and negatively.
<xref rid="bib0075" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>14</sup>
</xref>
During the MERS outbreak in South Korea, negative behaviors were observed, such as oversensitive or inappropriate reactions. Some children of health care workers at hospitals treating MERS patients were prevented from attending school. Meanwhile, self-quarantined subjects occasionally escaped out of their homes until the level of infection subsided. These reactions of citizens reflect distrust in the government and accelerated noncompliance with the directions provided by the Korea Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. This finding is important because overreaction is an indicator of the level of trust in the government among the public and may provoke another social problem related to moral panic.
<xref rid="bib0080" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>15</sup>
</xref>
</p>
<p id="p0035">Fear causing the behaviors described previously mentioned is likely related to risk perception and a low level of trust in the government and society. However, because risk perception may be related to a number of unknown determinants, it is important to identify factors that may affect risk perception. Sjöberg examined risk perception using several approaches,
<xref rid="bib0085" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>16</sup>
</xref>
and reported that 30%-40% of risk perception could be explained by risk sensitivity, attitude, and a specific fear model. In other models, <20% of risk perception was explained. There were also some studies regarding risk perception. Previous studies of risk perception related to nuclear explosions and infectious diseases addressed the relationship between perceived risk and various social predictors, such as knowledge, social trust, and attitude.
<xref rid="bib0090" ref-type="bibr">17</xref>
,
<xref rid="bib0095" ref-type="bibr">18</xref>
However, previous literature on the determinants of risk perception has been limited.</p>
<p id="p0040">The aim of this study was to determine whether risk perception was associated with personal and social variables, including trust in the media, the health care field, and government. Additionally, we sought to identify the associations of risk perception and social variables with compliance with self-quarantine guidelines and overreaction during the MERS epidemic. In this study, knowledge, trust, personal characteristics, and other social determinants were considered the main factors affecting risk perception and overreaction.</p>
<sec id="s0010">
<title>Methods</title>
<sec id="s0015">
<title>Questionnaire</title>
<p id="p0045">We developed a questionnaire based on previous studies of perception of the risk of SARS and Ebola conducted outside of South Korea.
<xref rid="bib0100" ref-type="bibr">19</xref>
,
<xref rid="bib0105" ref-type="bibr">20</xref>
,
<xref rid="bib0110" ref-type="bibr">21</xref>
The questionnaire comprised the following 5 components: knowledge of MERS (9 items), risk perceptions about MERS (7 items), trust in society and governmental health policy(5 items), preventive behavior (11 items), and particular situations relating to MERS (7 items). Sociodemographic information was also obtained. Korean- and English-language versions of the questionnaire were developed to enable inclusion of foreign students in the survey (
<xref rid="ec0010" ref-type="supplementary-material">Supplementary Appendix S1</xref>
). The English-language version of the questionnaire was backtranslated into Korean for quality control purposes.</p>
<p id="p0050">Questions regarding knowledge of MERS were used to estimate the level of accuracy of the following information: the self-recognized level of knowledge, the concept of MERS, route of transmission, the concept of close contact, symptoms, the concept of the incubation period, characteristics of MERS, self-quarantine, and treatment of MERS. Evidence for these questions was from the MERS response guidelines of the Korea Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.
<xref rid="bib0115" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>22</sup>
</xref>
The cumulative knowledge score of the participants was calculated (range, 0-8; 1 point for each correct response [ie, in agreement with the evidence]). Because all questions referred to information provided in governmental guidelines, the survey was regarded as providing an estimate of respondents' essential knowledge.</p>
<p id="p0055">Risk perceptions were assessed using revised versions of the Ebola risk perception surveys conducted in Germany
<xref rid="bib0105" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>20</sup>
</xref>
and Israel.
<xref rid="bib0110" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>21</sup>
</xref>
Risk perception was assessed using levels of agreement with the following statements: “I think that I will contract MERS if I come into contact with a MERS patient (risk perception 1),” “I think that I might contract MERS even if I do not come into contact with a MERS patient (risk perception 2),” “My health will be severely damaged if I contract MERS (risk perception 3),” “I think MERS is more severe than other respiratory diseases (risk perception 4),” “Even if I fall ill with another disease, I will not go to hospital because of MERS (risk perception 5),” “MERS will inflict serious damage on my community (risk perception 6),” and “MERS may spread in Korea again someday (risk perception 7).” The questions were responded to on a 5-point Likert scale. In addition, because risk perception is a continuous variable, it was classified into 4 groups according to quantile (very high, high, low, and very low).</p>
<p id="p0060">A revised questionnaire regarding trust in society, media, and health policy was developed from the Korean General Social Survey (KGSS). The KGSS has been validated in South Korea and is conducted periodically nationwide. Several questions in the KGSS estimate the levels of trust among respondents in society, the media, central and local government, the national assembly, the medical profession, and nongovernmental organizations (NGOs). Such trust was expected to be associated with trust in health policy because governmental actions tend to reflect governmental policies.</p>
<p id="p0065">Finally, questions regarding particular situations related to MERS were classified into the following 2 categories: overreaction and compliance with self-quarantine. A large proportion of the population of South Korea was unaware of the appropriate actions during the MERS epidemic. These categories measure the characteristics of individuals not inclined to abide by the self-quarantine guidelines. Therefore, this section assessed the following personal characteristics: degree of optimism about the health policies of South Korea, willingness to sacrifice for society, responsiveness to an emergency situation, and attitude toward self-quarantine and overreaction. Attitude toward self-quarantine was measured with a question asking whether respondents were willing to prioritize quarantine adherence above all personal needs (responses were categorized as yes and no). The overreaction section comprised the following 2 questions: (1) How would you react if you realized your children were in the same class as those of someone suspected (or confirmed) to have MERS (including school and extracurricular activities)?; and (2) How would you react if you realized your children were in the same class as those of medical workers at hospitals with MERS patients (including school and extracurricular activities)? The responses to the formerly involved overreactions, for example, “(Do not) Allow my relatives or children of my family to attend school,” and those to the latter were the opposite, such as “I think children of medical workers at hospitals with MERS patients should (not) be allowed to attend school.” Each question was assumed to indicate overreaction to the behavior of oneself and others.</p>
<p id="p0070">A presurvey including 152 participants was performed to assess the reliability and validity of the questionnaire and the direction of the responses. The presurvey was conducted both in the field using a tablet personal computer or laptop and online using an online survey application (SurveyMonkey, San Mateo, CA). Data for the main survey were collected by e-mailing students at Seoul National University. The Cronbach α values for the 2 new measures developed for this study, trust and risk perception, were 0.8307 and 0.6755, respectively, in the main survey.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="s0020">
<title>Participants</title>
<p id="p0075">Students at Seoul National University were selected for the study. We considered this population appropriate for several reasons. First, they are not likely to include people associated with hospitals. Second, younger individuals tend to have access to up-to-date information through smartphones or the Internet. Third, the subjects had similar levels of academic attainment and demographic characteristics. Finally, because of the need for a timely investigation, this setting assured the benefit of accessibility and cost-efficiency.</p>
<p id="p0080">For the main survey, e-mails containing the link connected to the online survey setting were sent to all of the 30,727 students at Seoul National University, including undergraduates, postgraduates, and foreign students. Survey e-mails were sent on 3 occasions at weekly intervals from October 6-23, 2015. Of the 30,727 students, 1,487 (4.8%) provided a complete response. After exclusion of those with missing responses, a total of 1,470 subjects were included in the study. This survey and research were approved by the Institutional Review Board of Seoul National University (No. 1508/002-003).</p>
</sec>
<sec id="s0025">
<title>Statistical analysis</title>
<p id="p0085">Relationships between levels of risk perception and demographic factors were evaluated by the χ
<sup>2</sup>
test, and the mean and SE values were estimated. To evaluate correlations among independent variables, Spearman correlation coefficients were calculated. Correlation coefficients among explanatory variables were all <0.5, which was regarded as not having multicollinearity.
<xref rid="bib0120" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>23</sup>
</xref>
Variables were selected for inclusion by stepwise calculation of the Akaike information criterion. Moreover, to prevent multicollinearity, a test of variation inflation factors was performed for each analysis. To assess the associations of demographic factors, knowledge, trust in social organizations, intention to sacrifice, and responsiveness to emergency situations with risk perception, multiple linear regression analyses were used. Each question regarding risk perception was aggregated to produce a cumulative score indicative of the overall effect of risk perception. Prior to analysis, the variables were tested using a Q-Q plot to verify the normality of their distribution and—with the exception of knowledge—were confirmed to be continuous because the ordinal values differed only slightly from a normal distribution.
<xref rid="bib0125" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>24</sup>
</xref>
Risk perception was estimated on a scale of 1-7, and its relationship with predictors was assessed. In addition, to identify the association of risk perception with overreaction to the epidemic and compliance with self-quarantine, multiple logistic regression analyses were performed. Odds ratios (ORs) were calculated for self-quarantine and overreaction. Analyses were conducted using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="s0030">
<title>Results</title>
<sec id="s0035">
<title>Risk perception</title>
<p id="p0090">Sociodemographic variables and levels of risk perception are shown in
<xref rid="t0010" ref-type="table">Table 1</xref>
. More than half of the participants were men (57.01%), 24-26 years of age (30.54%), citizens of South Korea (95.51%), and postgraduate students (52.04%). A high frequency of risk perception was detected in women (n = 152; 52.6%), those 24-26 years of age (n = 97; 33.56%), students from South Korea (n = 281; 97.23%), and postgraduate students (n = 166; 57.44%). The mean knowledge score was 5.35 ± 1.41 out of 10. Therefore, respondents had approximately 53.5% of the essential knowledge about MERS. The mean score for trust in the medical profession was higher than those for other social components (3.63 ± 0.95); the mean score for trust in the central government was lowest (2.37 ± 1.03) (
<xref rid="t0015" ref-type="table">Table 2</xref>
). In terms of the media section, the item regarding trust in newspapers was excluded from the analysis because of its lack of validity according to the variable selection process.
<table-wrap position="float" id="t0010">
<label>Table 1</label>
<caption>
<p>Risk perception by sociodemographic characteristics (N = 1,487)</p>
</caption>
<alt-text id="atte0020">Table 1</alt-text>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th rowspan="2" align="left">Characteristic</th>
<th colspan="5" align="left">Risk perception</th>
<th rowspan="2" align="left">
<italic>P</italic>
value</th>
</tr>
<tr>
<th align="left">Total</th>
<th align="left">Very low (Q1)</th>
<th align="left">Low (Q2)</th>
<th align="left">High (Q3)</th>
<th align="left">Very high (Q4)</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="left">Sex</td>
<td align="left"></td>
<td align="left"></td>
<td align="left"></td>
<td align="left"></td>
<td align="left"></td>
<td align="left"></td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left"> Male</td>
<td align="char">838(57.01)</td>
<td align="char">180(67.16)</td>
<td align="char">248(65.09)</td>
<td align="char">273(51.32)</td>
<td align="char">137(47.40)</td>
<td rowspan="2" align="char"><.0001</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left"> Female</td>
<td align="char">632(42.99)</td>
<td align="char">88(32.84)</td>
<td align="char">133(34.91)</td>
<td align="char">259(48.68)</td>
<td align="char">152(52.60)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Age (y)</td>
<td align="char"></td>
<td align="char"></td>
<td align="char"></td>
<td align="char"></td>
<td align="char"></td>
<td align="char"></td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left"> 18-20</td>
<td align="char">228(15.51)</td>
<td align="char">41(15.30)</td>
<td align="char">64(16.80)</td>
<td align="char">83(15.60)</td>
<td align="char">40(13.84)</td>
<td rowspan="5" align="char">.1092</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left"> 21-23</td>
<td align="char">362(24.63)</td>
<td align="char">64(23.88)</td>
<td align="char">113(29.66)</td>
<td align="char">128(24.06)</td>
<td align="char">57(19.72)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left"> 24-26</td>
<td align="char">449(30.54)</td>
<td align="char">86(32.09)</td>
<td align="char">114(29.92)</td>
<td align="char">152(28.57)</td>
<td align="char">97(33.56)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left"> 27-30</td>
<td align="char">253(17.21)</td>
<td align="char">46(17.16)</td>
<td align="char">55(14.44)</td>
<td align="char">102(19.17)</td>
<td align="char">50(17.30)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left"> ≥30</td>
<td align="char">178(12.11)</td>
<td align="char">31(11.57)</td>
<td align="char">35(9.19)</td>
<td align="char">67(12.59)</td>
<td align="char">45(15.57)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Country</td>
<td align="char"></td>
<td align="char"></td>
<td align="char"></td>
<td align="char"></td>
<td align="char"></td>
<td align="char"></td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left"> South Korea</td>
<td align="char">1,404(95.51)</td>
<td align="char">254(94.78)</td>
<td align="char">361(94.75)</td>
<td align="char">508(95.49)</td>
<td align="char">281(97.23)</td>
<td rowspan="2" align="char">.4157</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left"> Foreign</td>
<td align="char">66(4.49)</td>
<td align="char">14(5.22)</td>
<td align="char">20(5.25)</td>
<td align="char">24(4.51)</td>
<td align="char">8(2.77)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Education</td>
<td align="char"></td>
<td align="char"></td>
<td align="char"></td>
<td align="char"></td>
<td align="char"></td>
<td align="char"></td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left"> Undergraduate</td>
<td align="char">705(47.96)</td>
<td align="char">127(47.39)</td>
<td align="char">206(54.07)</td>
<td align="char">249(46.80)</td>
<td align="char">123(42.56)</td>
<td rowspan="2" align="char">.0245</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left"> Graduate</td>
<td align="char">765(52.04)</td>
<td align="char">141(52.61)</td>
<td align="char">175(45.93)</td>
<td align="char">283(53.20)</td>
<td align="char">166(57.44)</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn>
<p>NOTE. Values are n (%) or as otherwise indicated.
<italic>Q</italic>
, quantile.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
<table-wrap position="float" id="t0015">
<label>Table 2</label>
<caption>
<p>Risk perception, knowledge, trust, and personal characteristics</p>
</caption>
<alt-text id="atte0025">Table 2</alt-text>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th align="left">Characteristic</th>
<th align="left">Mean</th>
<th align="left">Median</th>
<th align="left">SE</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="left">Risk perception</td>
<td align="char">3.14</td>
<td align="char">3.14</td>
<td align="char">0.58</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Knowledge
<xref rid="tn0010" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">5.35</td>
<td align="char">5.00</td>
<td align="char">1.41</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (media, television)</td>
<td align="char">2.69</td>
<td align="char">3.00</td>
<td align="char">0.97</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (medical)</td>
<td align="char">3.63</td>
<td align="char">4.00</td>
<td align="char">0.95</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (central government)</td>
<td align="char">2.37</td>
<td align="char">2.00</td>
<td align="char">1.03</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (local government)</td>
<td align="char">2.63</td>
<td align="char">3.00</td>
<td align="char">0.96</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (NGO)</td>
<td align="char">2.79</td>
<td align="char">3.00</td>
<td align="char">0.95</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (society)</td>
<td align="char">2.45</td>
<td align="char">2.00</td>
<td align="char">0.86</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (health policy)</td>
<td align="char">2.72</td>
<td align="char">3.00</td>
<td align="char">0.90</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Optimism (health policy)</td>
<td align="char">3.20</td>
<td align="char">3.00</td>
<td align="char">0.86</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Willingness to sacrifice
<xref rid="tn0015" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">2.54</td>
<td align="char">3.00</td>
<td align="char">0.67</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Responsiveness to emergency
<xref rid="tn0020" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">2.06</td>
<td align="char">2.00</td>
<td align="char">0.62</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn>
<p>
<italic>NGO</italic>
, nongovernmental organization.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="tn0010">
<label>*</label>
<p id="np0020">Ten-point scale.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="tn0015">
<label></label>
<p id="np0025">Four-point scale.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="tn0020">
<label></label>
<p id="np0030">Three-point scale.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
</p>
<p id="p0095">Women had a higher risk perception score than men, not only overall (by 0.17/5 points; β = 0.17;
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001), but also in all other risk perceptions from 1-7 (
<xref rid="t0020" ref-type="table">Table 3</xref>
). Older age was negatively associated with risk perception 2 (concern over contracting MERS through indirect contact) (β = −0.07;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .004) and positively associated with risk perceptions 4 (considering MERS to be more severe than other respiratory diseases) (β = 0.09;
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001), 6 (concern over damage to the community because of MERS) (β = 0.05;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .026), and 7 (future reemergence of MERS) (β = 0.06;
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001). In addition, only risk perception 6 (damage to the community) was significantly positively associated with Korean ethnicity (β = 0.44;
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001).
<table-wrap position="float" id="t0020">
<label>Table 3</label>
<caption>
<p>Factors that affect risk perception according to multiple regression</p>
</caption>
<alt-text id="atte0030">Table 3</alt-text>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th rowspan="2" align="left">Characteristic</th>
<th colspan="2" align="left">Cumulative (overall) risk perception
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</th>
<th colspan="2" align="left">Riskperception 1</th>
<th colspan="2" align="left">Riskperception 2</th>
<th colspan="2" align="left">Riskperception 3</th>
<th colspan="2" align="left">Riskperception 4</th>
<th colspan="2" align="left">Riskperception 5</th>
<th colspan="2" align="left">Riskperception 6</th>
<th colspan="2" align="left">Riskperception 7</th>
</tr>
<tr>
<th align="left">β</th>
<th align="left">
<italic>P</italic>
value</th>
<th align="left">β</th>
<th align="left">
<italic>P</italic>
value</th>
<th align="left">β</th>
<th align="left">
<italic>P</italic>
value</th>
<th align="left">β</th>
<th align="left">
<italic>P</italic>
value</th>
<th align="left">β</th>
<th align="left">
<italic>P</italic>
value</th>
<th align="left">β</th>
<th align="left">
<italic>P</italic>
value</th>
<th align="left">β</th>
<th align="left">
<italic>P</italic>
value</th>
<th align="left">β</th>
<th align="left">
<italic>P</italic>
value</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="left">(Intercept)</td>
<td align="char">2.64</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">2.83</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">2.74</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">2.86</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">1.82</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">2.62</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">2.32</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">3.27</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Female</td>
<td align="char">0.17</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.10</td>
<td align="char">.037
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.18</td>
<td align="char">.002
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.15</td>
<td align="char">.005
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.25</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.16</td>
<td align="char">.005
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.18</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.15</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Age (per 3-y increase)
<xref rid="tn0040" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.02</td>
<td align="char">.086</td>
<td align="char">0.00</td>
<td align="char">.860</td>
<td align="char">−0.07</td>
<td align="char">.004
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.02</td>
<td align="char">.271</td>
<td align="char">0.09</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.01</td>
<td align="char">.810</td>
<td align="char">0.05</td>
<td align="char">.026
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.06</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Korean</td>
<td align="char">0.11</td>
<td align="char">.111</td>
<td align="char">−0.04</td>
<td align="char">.727</td>
<td align="char">0.27</td>
<td align="char">.050</td>
<td align="char">−0.02</td>
<td align="char">.866</td>
<td align="char">−0.05</td>
<td align="char">.706</td>
<td align="char">0.07</td>
<td align="char">.591</td>
<td align="char">0.44</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.12</td>
<td align="char">.264</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Knowledge</td>
<td align="char">0.01</td>
<td align="char">.308</td>
<td align="char">0.04</td>
<td align="char">.015
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.04</td>
<td align="char">.055</td>
<td align="char">0.01</td>
<td align="char">.650</td>
<td align="char">0.02</td>
<td align="char">.366</td>
<td align="char">−0.01</td>
<td align="char">.596</td>
<td align="char">0.01</td>
<td align="char">.563</td>
<td align="char">0.05</td>
<td align="char">.004
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (media)</td>
<td align="char">0.06</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.10</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.03</td>
<td align="char">.360</td>
<td align="char">0.08</td>
<td align="char">.018
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.07</td>
<td align="char">.019
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.06</td>
<td align="char">.083</td>
<td align="char">0.11</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.01</td>
<td align="char">.790</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (medical)</td>
<td align="char">−0.04</td>
<td align="char">.027
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.06</td>
<td align="char">.044
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.10</td>
<td align="char">.003
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.05</td>
<td align="char">.086</td>
<td align="char">−0.13</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.04</td>
<td align="char">.172</td>
<td align="char">−0.04</td>
<td align="char">.146</td>
<td align="char">0.05</td>
<td align="char">.085</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (central government)</td>
<td align="char">−0.05</td>
<td align="char">.014
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.06</td>
<td align="char">.076</td>
<td align="char">0.01</td>
<td align="char">.711</td>
<td align="char">0.01</td>
<td align="char">.714</td>
<td align="char">−0.02</td>
<td align="char">.566</td>
<td align="char">−0.09</td>
<td align="char">.022
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.11</td>
<td align="char">.002
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.10</td>
<td align="char">.002
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (local government)</td>
<td align="char">0.04</td>
<td align="char">.046
<xref rid="tn0040" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.03</td>
<td align="char">.376</td>
<td align="char">0.07</td>
<td align="char">.075</td>
<td align="char">0.00</td>
<td align="char">.991</td>
<td align="char">0.03</td>
<td align="char">.433</td>
<td align="char">0.10</td>
<td align="char">.006
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.08</td>
<td align="char">.012
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.02</td>
<td align="char">.566</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (NGO)</td>
<td align="char">0.06</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.09</td>
<td align="char">.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.03</td>
<td align="char">.330</td>
<td align="char">0.08</td>
<td align="char">.008
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.13</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.03</td>
<td align="char">.402</td>
<td align="char">0.07</td>
<td align="char">.013
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.02</td>
<td align="char">.330</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (society)</td>
<td align="char">−0.03</td>
<td align="char">.211</td>
<td align="char">−0.03</td>
<td align="char">.372</td>
<td align="char">−0.02</td>
<td align="char">.593</td>
<td align="char">0.01</td>
<td align="char">.876</td>
<td align="char">0.04</td>
<td align="char">.270</td>
<td align="char">−0.03</td>
<td align="char">.408</td>
<td align="char">−0.11</td>
<td align="char">.002
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.04</td>
<td align="char">.265</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (health policy)</td>
<td align="char">−0.05</td>
<td align="char">.014
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.00</td>
<td align="char">.992</td>
<td align="char">−0.12</td>
<td align="char">.002
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.01</td>
<td align="char">.815</td>
<td align="char">−0.03</td>
<td align="char">.443</td>
<td align="char">−0.05</td>
<td align="char">.159</td>
<td align="char">−0.04</td>
<td align="char">.280</td>
<td align="char">−0.09</td>
<td align="char">.002
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Optimism (health policy)</td>
<td align="char">−0.04</td>
<td align="char">.024
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.04</td>
<td align="char">.210</td>
<td align="char">−0.03</td>
<td align="char">.487</td>
<td align="char">−0.09</td>
<td align="char">.009
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.06</td>
<td align="char">.066</td>
<td align="char">−0.02</td>
<td align="char">.505</td>
<td align="char">0.00</td>
<td align="char">.904</td>
<td align="char">−0.05</td>
<td align="char">.079</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Willingness to sacrifice</td>
<td align="char">−0.06</td>
<td align="char">.004
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.11</td>
<td align="char">.003
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.07</td>
<td align="char">.112</td>
<td align="char">−0.09</td>
<td align="char">.022
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.01</td>
<td align="char">.818</td>
<td align="char">−0.10</td>
<td align="char">.018
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">−0.06</td>
<td align="char">.125</td>
<td align="char">−0.03</td>
<td align="char">.363</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Responsiveness to emergency</td>
<td align="char">0.18</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.16</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.18</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.22</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0035" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.24</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.06</td>
<td align="char">.191</td>
<td align="char">0.27</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.11</td>
<td align="char">.003
<xref rid="tn0030" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn>
<p>
<italic>NGO</italic>
, nongovernmental organization;
<italic>Risk perception 1</italic>
, “I think that I will contract MERS if I come into contact with a MERS patient”;
<italic>Risk perception 2</italic>
, “I think that I might contract MERS even if I do not come into contact with a MERS patient”;
<italic>Risk perception 3</italic>
, “My health will be severely damaged if I contract MERS”;
<italic>Risk perception 4</italic>
, “I think MERS is more severe than other respiratory diseases”;
<italic>Risk perception 5</italic>
, “Even if I fall ill with another disease, I will not go to the hospital because of MERS”;
<italic>Risk perception 6</italic>
, “MERS will inflict serious damage on my community”;
<italic>Risk perception 7</italic>
, “MERS may spread in Korea again someday.”</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="tn0030">
<label>*</label>
<p id="np0040">
<italic>P </italic>
<
<italic></italic>
.01.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="tn0035">
<label></label>
<p id="np0045">
<italic>P </italic>
<
<italic></italic>
.05.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="tn0040">
<label></label>
<p id="np0050">Minimum age, 18 years; maximum age, >30 years.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
</p>
</sec>
<sec id="s0040">
<title>Knowledge</title>
<p id="p0100">Knowledge influenced only risk perceptions 1 and 7. That is, knowledge was related to concern over contracting MERS through direct contact (β = 0.04;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .015) and reemergence of MERS (β = 0.05;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .004). Knowledge was associated with risk perception, which does not support the hypothesis of this work.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="s0045">
<title>Trust in social organizations</title>
<p id="p0105">Trust in the media (broadcasting) was positively associated with cumulative risk perception (β = 0.06;
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001) and risk perceptions 1 (concern over contracting MERS through direct contact), 3 (severity of MERS), 4 (considering MERS to be more severe than other respiratory diseases), and 6 (concern over damage to the community because of MERS). However, trust in the medical profession (β = −0.04;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .027) and the central government of South Korea (β = −0.05;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .014) was negatively associated with overall risk perception. Risk perceptions 2 and 4 (relevant to disease traits) were related to trust in the medical profession. In addition, risk perceptions 5, 6, and 7 (relevant to community traits) were related to trust in the central government. In contrast, trust in local government (β = 0.04;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .046) and NGOs (β = 0.06;
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001) was positively associated with risk perception. Trust in local government and NGOs was associated with risk perceptions 5 and 6 and risk perceptions 1, 3, 4, and 6, respectively. Compared with the results for trust in the central government, those for trust in local government seemed to have similar effect sizes, but with a different direction of estimates. However, trust in NGOs was associated with the perception of the risk of transmission and that to personal health. This suggests that NGOs are believed to contribute to communities in various ways, including by providing medical services. Trust in society was not associated with the overall risk perception but was negatively associated with concern over damage to the community (β = −0.11;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .002). Finally, trust in health policy was negatively associated with risk perception (β = −0.05;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .014). Risk perceptions 2 and 7 were related to trust in the health policy of the government of South Korea.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="s0050">
<title>Personal characteristics</title>
<p id="p0110">Optimism about future health policy (β = −0.04;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .024), willingness to sacrifice (β = −0.06;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .004), and active responsiveness to an emergency (β = 0.18;
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001) were associated with risk perception. Only risk perception 3 was associated with optimism regarding health policy (β = −0.09;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .009). Willingness to sacrifice was negatively associated with risk perceptions 1, 3, and 5. In addition, responsiveness to an emergency was positively associated with risk perceptions 1, 2, 3, 4, 6, and 7. Therefore, respondents who were pessimistic about health policy and unlikely to sacrifice themselves in specific situations and respond actively to an emergency exhibited a higher perception of risk.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="s0055">
<title>Self-quarantine and overreaction</title>
<p id="p0115">ORs were calculated to estimate the associations among risk perception, knowledge, personal characteristics, and compliance with self-quarantine (
<xref rid="t0025" ref-type="table">Table 4</xref>
). The proportions of men and women who indicated they would comply with the self-quarantine guideline were 82.7% and 87.2%, respectively. Women were more likely to comply with self-quarantine (OR, 1.50; 95% confidence interval [CI], 1.10-2.06;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .010), as were older subjects (OR, 1.22; 95% CI, 1.01-1.46;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .037) and those with a greater level of knowledge (OR, 1.22; 95% CI, 1.10-1.35;
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001). Additionally, subjects with more trust in the medical profession (OR, 1.35; 95% CI, 1.14-1.60;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .001) and those who were willing to sacrifice (OR, 1.50; 95% CI, 1.20-1.88;
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001) and actively respond to emergencies (OR, 1.31; 95% CI, 1.03-1.67;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .028) were also more likely to comply with a self-quarantine order.
<table-wrap position="float" id="t0025">
<label>Table 4</label>
<caption>
<p>Associations of risk perception, knowledge, and personal characteristics with self-quarantine</p>
</caption>
<alt-text id="atte0035">Table 4</alt-text>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th rowspan="2" align="left">Characteristic</th>
<th colspan="3" align="left">Compliance with self-quarantine</th>
</tr>
<tr>
<th align="left">Odds ratio</th>
<th align="left">95% CI</th>
<th align="left">
<italic>P</italic>
value</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="left">Female (vs male)</td>
<td align="char">1.50</td>
<td align="char">1.10-2.06</td>
<td align="char">.010
<xref rid="tn0050" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Age (per 3-y increase)
<xref rid="tn0055" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
<td align="char">1.22</td>
<td align="char">1.01-1.46</td>
<td align="char">.037
<xref rid="tn0050" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Korean (vs foreigner)</td>
<td align="char">0.60</td>
<td align="char">0.25-1.46</td>
<td align="char">.263</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Undergraduate (vs graduate)</td>
<td align="char">1.04</td>
<td align="char">0.67-1.62</td>
<td align="char">.867</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Knowledge</td>
<td align="char">1.22</td>
<td align="char">1.10-1.35</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0060" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (media)</td>
<td align="char">0.95</td>
<td align="char">0.80-1.14</td>
<td align="char">.605</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (medical)</td>
<td align="char">1.35</td>
<td align="char">1.14-1.60</td>
<td align="char">.001
<xref rid="tn0060" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (central government)</td>
<td align="char">0.88</td>
<td align="char">0.71-1.10</td>
<td align="char">.262</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (local government)</td>
<td align="char">1.15</td>
<td align="char">0.93-1.42</td>
<td align="char">.194</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (NGO)</td>
<td align="char">1.03</td>
<td align="char">0.87-1.22</td>
<td align="char">.730</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (society)</td>
<td align="char">1.03</td>
<td align="char">0.83-1.27</td>
<td align="char">.823</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (health policy)</td>
<td align="char">0.96</td>
<td align="char">0.78-1.17</td>
<td align="char">.654</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Optimism (health policy)</td>
<td align="char">1.13</td>
<td align="char">0.93-1.37</td>
<td align="char">.228</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Willingness to sacrifice</td>
<td align="char">1.50</td>
<td align="char">1.20-1.88</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0060" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Responsiveness to emergency</td>
<td align="char">1.31</td>
<td align="char">1.03-1.67</td>
<td align="char">.028
<xref rid="tn0050" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Risk perception</td>
<td align="char">1.18</td>
<td align="char">0.90-1.54</td>
<td align="char">.233</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn>
<p>
<italic>CI</italic>
, confidence interval;
<italic>NGO</italic>
, nongovernmental organization.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="tn0050">
<label>*</label>
<p id="np0060">
<italic>P </italic>
<
<italic></italic>
.05</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="tn0055">
<label></label>
<p id="np0065">Minimum age, 18 years; maximum age, >30 years.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="tn0060">
<label></label>
<p id="np0070">
<italic>P </italic>
<
<italic></italic>
.01.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
</p>
<p id="p0120">The proportions of positive responses to items about overreactions in one's own behavior (self-behavior) and overreactions in response to the behavior of others were 79.9% and 42.9%, respectively. Knowledge (OR, 0.90; 95% CI, 0.82-0.99;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .037) and trust in society (OR, 0.81; 95% CI, 0.66-0.98;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .029) were negatively associated with overreaction in self-behavior (
<xref rid="t0030" ref-type="table">Table 5</xref>
). However, responsiveness to an emergency (OR, 1.32; 95% CI, 1.05-1.66;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .016) and risk perception (OR, 2.80; 95% CI, 2.17-3.60,
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001) were positively associated with overreaction in self-behavior.
<table-wrap position="float" id="t0030">
<label>Table 5</label>
<caption>
<p>Associations of risk perception, knowledge, and personal characteristics with overreaction in one's own behavior</p>
</caption>
<alt-text id="atte0040">Table 5</alt-text>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th rowspan="2" align="left">Characteristic</th>
<th colspan="3" align="left">Overreaction (self)</th>
</tr>
<tr>
<th align="left">Odds ratio</th>
<th align="left">95% CI</th>
<th align="left">
<italic>P</italic>
value</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="left">Female (vs male)</td>
<td align="char">1.20</td>
<td align="char">0.90-1.60</td>
<td align="char">.212</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Age (per 3-y increase)
<xref rid="tn0070" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.92</td>
<td align="char">0.78-1.08</td>
<td align="char">.295</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Korean (vs foreigner)</td>
<td align="char">1.72</td>
<td align="char">0.94-3.15</td>
<td align="char">.079</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Undergraduate (vs graduate)</td>
<td align="char">1.16</td>
<td align="char">0.78-1.73</td>
<td align="char">.473</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Knowledge</td>
<td align="char">0.90</td>
<td align="char">0.82-0.99</td>
<td align="char">.037
<xref rid="tn0075" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (media)</td>
<td align="char">1.17</td>
<td align="char">0.99-1.37</td>
<td align="char">.060</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (medical)</td>
<td align="char">0.95</td>
<td align="char">0.81-1.11</td>
<td align="char">.529</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (central government)</td>
<td align="char">1.08</td>
<td align="char">0.89-1.31</td>
<td align="char">.428</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (local government)</td>
<td align="char">1.06</td>
<td align="char">0.88-1.28</td>
<td align="char">.516</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (NGO)</td>
<td align="char">1.04</td>
<td align="char">0.89-1.21</td>
<td align="char">.611</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (society)</td>
<td align="char">0.81</td>
<td align="char">0.66-0.98</td>
<td align="char">.029
<xref rid="tn0075" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (health policy)</td>
<td align="char">1.00</td>
<td align="char">0.84-1.20</td>
<td align="char">.992</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Optimism (health policy)</td>
<td align="char">0.98</td>
<td align="char">0.82-1.17</td>
<td align="char">.817</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Willingness to sacrifice</td>
<td align="char">0.95</td>
<td align="char">0.78-1.17</td>
<td align="char">.646</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Responsiveness to emergency</td>
<td align="char">1.32</td>
<td align="char">1.05-1.66</td>
<td align="char">.016
<xref rid="tn0075" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Risk perception</td>
<td align="char">2.80</td>
<td align="char">2.17-3.60</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0080" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn>
<p>
<italic>CI</italic>
, confidence interval;
<italic>NGO</italic>
, nongovernmental organization.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="tn0070">
<label>*</label>
<p id="np0080">Minimum age, 18 years; maximum age, >30 years.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="tn0075">
<label></label>
<p id="np0085">
<italic>P </italic>
<
<italic></italic>
.05.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="tn0080">
<label></label>
<p id="np0090">
<italic>P </italic>
<
<italic></italic>
.01.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
</p>
<p id="p0125">In terms of overreaction to the behavior of others (
<xref rid="t0035" ref-type="table">Table 6</xref>
), Korean respondents (OR, 0.486; 95% CI, 0.28-0.84;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .010), those with a greater level of knowledge (OR, 0.79; 95% CI, 0.73-0.86,
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001), and those with a higher level of trust in the medical profession (OR, 0.70; 95% CI, 0.61-0.80,
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001) were unlikely to overreact to the behavior of others. However, respondents with a higher level of trust in local government (OR, 1.19; 95% CI, 1.02-1.38;
<italic>P</italic>
 = .024) and higher risk perception (OR, 1.86; 95% CI, 1.52-2.28,
<italic>P</italic>
 < .001) were more likely to overreact to the behavior of others. Therefore, risk perception was associated with a change in behavior during an epidemic.
<table-wrap position="float" id="t0035">
<label>Table 6</label>
<caption>
<p>Associations of risk perception, knowledge, and personal characteristics with overreaction to the behavior of others</p>
</caption>
<alt-text id="atte0045">Table 6</alt-text>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th rowspan="2" align="left">Characteristic</th>
<th colspan="3" align="left">Overreaction to behavior of others</th>
</tr>
<tr>
<th align="left">Odds ratio</th>
<th align="left">95% CI</th>
<th align="left">
<italic>P</italic>
value</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="left">Female (vs male)</td>
<td align="char">1.03</td>
<td align="char">0.83-1.29</td>
<td align="char">.777</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Age (per 3-y increase)
<xref rid="tn0090" ref-type="table-fn">*</xref>
</td>
<td align="char">0.97</td>
<td align="char">0.85-1.10</td>
<td align="char">.602</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Korean (vs foreigner)</td>
<td align="char">0.49</td>
<td align="char">0.28-0.84</td>
<td align="char">.010
<xref rid="tn0095" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Undergraduate (vs graduate)</td>
<td align="char">1.20</td>
<td align="char">0.86-1.66</td>
<td align="char">.283</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Knowledge</td>
<td align="char">0.79</td>
<td align="char">0.73-0.86</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0100" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (media)</td>
<td align="char">1.19</td>
<td align="char">1.04-1.36</td>
<td align="char">.012
<xref rid="tn0095" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (medical)</td>
<td align="char">0.70</td>
<td align="char">0.61-0.80</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0100" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (central government)</td>
<td align="char">1.01</td>
<td align="char">0.86-1.18</td>
<td align="char">.949</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (local government)</td>
<td align="char">1.19</td>
<td align="char">1.02-1.38</td>
<td align="char">.024
<xref rid="tn0095" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (NGO)</td>
<td align="char">0.99</td>
<td align="char">0.88-1.13</td>
<td align="char">.977</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (society)</td>
<td align="char">1.01</td>
<td align="char">0.86-1.19</td>
<td align="char">.894</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Trust (health policy)</td>
<td align="char">1.01</td>
<td align="char">0.87-1.17</td>
<td align="char">.926</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Optimism (health policy)</td>
<td align="char">0.93</td>
<td align="char">0.81-1.08</td>
<td align="char">.342</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Willingness to sacrifice</td>
<td align="char">0.92</td>
<td align="char">0.78-1.09</td>
<td align="char">.325</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Responsiveness to emergency</td>
<td align="char">1.19</td>
<td align="char">0.99-1.43</td>
<td align="char">.058</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left">Risk perception</td>
<td align="char">1.86</td>
<td align="char">1.52-2.28</td>
<td align="char"><.001
<xref rid="tn0100" ref-type="table-fn"></xref>
</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn>
<p>
<italic>CI</italic>
, confidence interval;
<italic>NGO</italic>
, nongovernmental organization.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="tn0090">
<label>*</label>
<p id="np0100">Minimum age, 18 years; maximum age, >30 years.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="tn0095">
<label></label>
<p id="np0105">
<italic>P </italic>
<
<italic></italic>
.05.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
<table-wrap-foot>
<fn id="tn0100">
<label></label>
<p id="np0110">
<italic>P </italic>
<
<italic></italic>
.01.</p>
</fn>
</table-wrap-foot>
</table-wrap>
</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec id="s0060">
<title>Discussion</title>
<p id="p0130">The findings of this study indicate that risk perception is associated with various social factors. Risk perception was correlated with sex and the level of trust in social organizations. These findings indicate that risk perception interacts with demographic and personal attitudinal factors at a collective level, as previously suggested.
<xref rid="bib0130" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>25</sup>
</xref>
Women had a higher risk perception than men. Trust in the media (television), local government, and NGOs exhibited positive associations with risk perception, whereas trust in central government, the medical profession, and health policy exhibited negative ones. These results suggest that people are aware of the different roles of central and local government.</p>
<p id="p0135">Additionally, these results identify the types of determinant that affect whether people overreact when an infectious outbreak occurs. Women and those with low levels of trust in central government, the medical field, and health policy were more likely to overreact. On the other hand, people with high levels of trust in the media, local government, and NGOs may be hypersensitive when an EID outbreak occurs. Furthermore, the higher risk perception was associated with only overreaction, not compliance to self-quarantine. Hypersensitivity that leads to overreaction can be explained by perceived media dependency
<xref rid="bib0135" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>26</sup>
</xref>
; however, this may have been related only to television in this research. Moreover, differences in the effects of trust in central and local governments on risk perception may be explained by South Koreans' perceptions of the roles of these government bodies. Central and local governments do not always appear to smoothly coordinate the emergency response, and this may be related to ongoing challenges about decentralization from the central governmental system.
<xref rid="bib0140" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>27</sup>
</xref>
</p>
<p id="p0140">Another important finding is related to the role of the media during a crisis. In this study, trust in the media positively affected risk perception and overreaction, supporting that the mass media influence perceptions of disasters and risks; however, they cannot change an epidemic event itself.
<xref rid="bib0025" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>4</sup>
</xref>
Restrictions on the availability of information by the government can exacerbate the impact of a disaster, especially in the new media age, in which person-centered media is emphasized; this was the case in the 2003 SARS outbreak in China and the 2015 MERS outbreak in South Korea. Regardless of the political system (ie, even in democratic states), the absence of risk communication may yield unexpected results. Therefore, the World Health Organization outbreak guidelines
<xref rid="bib0060" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>11</sup>
</xref>
regarding building public trust and establishing a transparent flow of information need to be followed.</p>
<p id="p0145">Respondents in this study were moderately knowledgeable about MERS, possessing 53.5% of the essential knowledge. This finding may reflect a higher mean level of education of the respondents compared with the general population, but indicates that the public is not ignorant about environmental and health-related matters.
<xref rid="bib0145" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>28</sup>
</xref>
From another point of view, this finding can be said to contradict the deficit model, which holds that the public lacks knowledge.
<xref rid="bib0150" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>29</sup>
</xref>
On the emergence of a novel infectious disease, people tend to be receptive to health-related information conveyed by new media, including the Internet and mobile phones. For these reasons, the role of the mass media is particularly important because they usually provide widespread coverage of EIDs and convey information to the public.
<xref rid="bib0155" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>30</sup>
</xref>
Furthermore, the level of knowledge about MERS was not associated with risk perception. This is not in agreement with a previous report of a correlation between knowledge and perception of the risk of nuclear explosions.
<xref rid="bib0160" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>31</sup>
</xref>
Notwithstanding these considerations, knowledge of MERS was directly associated with overreaction and compliance with self-quarantine. This suggests that greater knowledge of unfamiliar diseases reduces the likelihood of undesirable behavior, which can lead to social problems.</p>
<p id="p0150">Personal characteristics, including willingness to sacrifice and responsiveness to an emergency, were negatively and positively, respectively, associated with risk perception. In addition, while responsiveness to an emergency was associated directly with overreaction and compliance with self-quarantine, willingness to sacrifice was associated only with compliance with self-quarantine. Personal characteristics were associated with responsive behaviors; however, the reasons for these associations could not be determined. One possibility is nationalism. Individuals who regard nations as abstract communities with shared emotional bonds governed by rules and/or norms tend to be altruistic.
<xref rid="bib0165" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>32</sup>
</xref>
Therefore, people in societies governed by established, systematic rules are less likely to exhibit unusual behaviors that may lead to social panic.</p>
<p id="p0155">Moreover, inaccurate information is disseminated through social media platforms, such as Twitter, Facebook, and Google. The enormous influence of social media during the 2014-2015 Ebola outbreak in West Africa has led to exaggerated concerns even among the population in the United States with negligible risks.
<xref rid="bib0170" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>33</sup>
</xref>
Indeed, the mass media can also incite panic, which is related to fear and antisocial behavior
<xref rid="bib0175" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>34</sup>
</xref>
; however, when they do their job properly, they can inform the public accurately about current conditions. Two studies in which a mobile health communication tool was used demonstrated the effectiveness of a social media–based approach in terms of changing vaccination behavior.
<xref rid="bib0180" ref-type="bibr">35</xref>
,
<xref rid="bib0185" ref-type="bibr">36</xref>
Furthermore, the provision of appropriate and timely public health information by social media and the mobile health tool could accelerate the detection of disease outbreaks and enhance the public's response,
<xref rid="bib0190" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>37</sup>
</xref>
thereby minimizing adverse health and economic effects.
<xref rid="bib0195" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>38</sup>
</xref>
</p>
<p id="p0160">The findings of our study are depicted in schematic form in
<xref rid="f0015" ref-type="fig">Figure 2</xref>
. In this model, risk perception was related to various factors such as sex, trust, and other personal characteristics. There was a notable aspect of risk perception: it was directly associated with overreaction, but not with compliance to self-quarantine. This suggests that risk perception motivates self-protective behavior, possibly resulting in overreaction. However, it does not automatically result in compliance to quarantine, which is generally perceived as protecting others, rather than oneself. In this sense, risk perception is a double-edged sword: at an optimal level, it helps the public protect themselves from infection and thereby deter the spread of epidemic; however, when excessive and characterized by fear, it can lead to other social problems, such as overreaction and discrimination. Sandman reported that risk perception is comprised of hazard and outrage.
<xref rid="bib0200" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>39</sup>
</xref>
The MERS outbreak caused fear and anxiety among the population, which might have resulted in an overreaction to both the response of the government and the behaviors of themselves and others.
<xref rid="bib0205" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>40</sup>
</xref>
Our model also points to the importance of trust in medical experts and proper knowledge. These 2 factors demonstrate the potential to maximize the benefit by promoting compliance and reducing overreaction. Therefore, risk communication should not just scare people, but should create a context in which there is trust and a flow of knowledge and clear information between the authorities and the public. These conditions should prevent adverse effects, particularly in democratic states. The existing literature emphasizes these essential components of risk management and risk communication.
<xref rid="bib0210" ref-type="bibr">41</xref>
,
<xref rid="bib0215" ref-type="bibr">42</xref>
<fig id="f0015">
<label>Fig 2</label>
<caption>
<p>Relationships among risk perception, compliance, and overreaction.</p>
</caption>
<alt-text id="atte0015">Fig 2</alt-text>
<graphic xlink:href="ymic4343-fig-0002_lrg"></graphic>
</fig>
</p>
<p id="p0165">To date, there are a limited number of studies on risk perception of infectious diseases (including MERS); therefore, this work makes an important contribution to the field. Several factors affecting perceived risk were evaluated (eg, trust in the media or health policy, personal characteristics), which were not assessed in previous studies. Although knowledge and trust have been previously reported to be associated with perceived risk,
<xref rid="bib0090" ref-type="bibr">17</xref>
,
<xref rid="bib0095" ref-type="bibr">18</xref>
this study further explored possible effects of risk perception and their implications in responding to an epidemic. EIDs are unfamiliar to the general public and are likely to be overestimated in the risk,
<xref rid="bib0160" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>31</sup>
</xref>
possibly leading to social panic. To prevent this, it is important to boost the risk communication with fostering trust and relevant knowledge, which requires transparency.</p>
<p id="p0170">This study had limitations. First, the survey involved students at a university; therefore, the participants might have different levels of concern about the epidemic compared with the general public. However, the participants represented the younger population with a homogeneous educational level with fewer confounding factors. Therefore, the findings were still likely meaningful for understanding the characteristics of risk perception in the essence. Second, the response rate was rather low (4.8%). Most online surveys have much lower response rates than person-to-person interviews.
<xref rid="bib0220" ref-type="bibr">
<sup>43</sup>
</xref>
The students who had a greater perception of the risk might have been better motivated to participate in the study. Even if the associations found in this study may be weaker in a larger population, it is informative to identify the direction and structure of the interrelationships among the factors.</p>
<p id="p0175">Future studies should investigate whether the findings from this study are applicable in more diverse populations. Furthermore, developing and testing more specific approaches to building trust and sharing knowledge during an epidemic would be needed. In responding to the public health emergency, collaboration is important among various social institutions, such as central and local governments, media, and medical community. More research is needed to explore the roles and relationships of these agents for effective risk communication.</p>
<p id="p0180">In conclusion, risk perception was found to be associated with social trust and personal attitudes toward emergency situations in this study. Risk perception was associated with overreaction, possibly by fear-induced changes in behavior. However, knowledge about the nature of disease mitigated this possibility and enhanced compliance to quarantine guidelines. Understanding the determinants of risk perception contributes to effective communication. Building trust and sharing knowledge are important to ensure a rapid response to disease outbreaks, and to prevent unnecessary behaviors among members of the public.</p>
</sec>
</body>
<back>
<ref-list id="bb0010">
<title>References</title>
<ref id="bib0010">
<label>1</label>
<element-citation publication-type="other" id="sr0010">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Korea Centers for Disease Control and Prevention</surname>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>MERS portal</article-title>
<comment>Available from</comment>
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.mers.go.kr/mers/html/jsp/main.jsp" id="iw0015">http://www.mers.go.kr/mers/html/jsp/main.jsp</ext-link>
<comment>Accessed October 2016</comment>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0015">
<label>2</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0015">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Penttinen</surname>
<given-names>P.M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kaasik-Aaslav</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Friaux</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Donachie</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sudre</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Amato-Gauci</surname>
<given-names>A.J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Taking stock of the first 133 MERS coronavirus cases globally–Is the epidemic changing?</article-title>
<source>Euro Surveill</source>
<volume>18</volume>
<year>2013</year>
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>5</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23449181</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0020">
<label>3</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0020">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fung</surname>
<given-names>I.C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tse</surname>
<given-names>Z.T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Chan</surname>
<given-names>B.S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fu</surname>
<given-names>K.W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Middle East respiratory syndrome in the Republic of Korea: transparency and communication are key</article-title>
<source>Western Pac Surveill Response J</source>
<volume>6</volume>
<year>2015</year>
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>2</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0025">
<label>4</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0025">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ma</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Media, crisis, and SARS: an introduction</article-title>
<source>Asian J Commun</source>
<volume>15</volume>
<year>2005</year>
<fpage>241</fpage>
<lpage>246</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0030">
<label>5</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0030">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>King</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pan</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Roberts</surname>
<given-names>M.E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>How censorship in China allows government criticism but silences collective expression</article-title>
<source>Am Polit Sci Rev</source>
<volume>107</volume>
<year>2013</year>
<fpage>326</fpage>
<lpage>343</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0035">
<label>6</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book" id="sr0035">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Keane</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Donald</surname>
<given-names>S.H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<chapter-title>Convergence, content industries and media governance. Media in China: consumption, content and crisis</chapter-title>
<year>2014</year>
<publisher-name>Routledge</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>London, UK</publisher-loc>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0040">
<label>7</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0040">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tausczik</surname>
<given-names>Y.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Faasse</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pennebaker</surname>
<given-names>J.W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Petrie</surname>
<given-names>K.J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Public anxiety and information seeking following the H1N1 outbreak: blogs, newspaper articles, and Wikipedia visits</article-title>
<source>Health Commun</source>
<volume>27</volume>
<year>2012</year>
<fpage>179</fpage>
<lpage>185</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21827326</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0045">
<label>8</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0045">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Chew</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Eysenbach</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Pandemics in the age of Twitter: content analysis of Tweets during the 2009 H1N1 outbreak</article-title>
<source>PLoS ONE</source>
<volume>5</volume>
<year>2010</year>
<fpage>e14118</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21124761</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0050">
<label>9</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book" id="sr0050">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gesser-Edelsburg</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Shir-Raz</surname>
<given-names>Y.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<chapter-title>Risk communication and infectious diseases in an age of digital media</chapter-title>
<year>2016</year>
<publisher-name>Routledge</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>New York (NY)</publisher-loc>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0055">
<label>10</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0055">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cvetkovich</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Winter</surname>
<given-names>P.L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Trust and social representations of the management of threatened and endangered species</article-title>
<source>Environ Behav</source>
<volume>35</volume>
<year>2003</year>
<fpage>286</fpage>
<lpage>307</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0060">
<label>11</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book" id="sr0060">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>World Health Organization</surname>
</name>
</person-group>
<chapter-title>Outbreak communication: best practices for communicating with the public during an outbreak</chapter-title>
<year>2005</year>
<publisher-name>WHO</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>Geneva, Switzerland</publisher-loc>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0065">
<label>12</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0065">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fischhoff</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bostrom</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Quadrel</surname>
<given-names>M.J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Risk perception and communication</article-title>
<source>Annu Rev Public Health</source>
<volume>14</volume>
<year>1993</year>
<fpage>183</fpage>
<lpage>203</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8323585</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0070">
<label>13</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book" id="sr0070">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Renn</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rohrmann</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<chapter-title>Cross-cultural risk perception: a survey of empirical studies</chapter-title>
<year>2000</year>
<publisher-name>Springer Science & Business Media</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>New York (NY)</publisher-loc>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0075">
<label>14</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0075">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Van der Pligt</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Risk perception and self-protective behavior</article-title>
<source>Eur Psychol</source>
<volume>1</volume>
<year>1996</year>
<fpage>34</fpage>
<lpage>43</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0080">
<label>15</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0080">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rohloff</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wright</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Moral panic and social theory beyond the heuristic</article-title>
<source>Curr Sociol</source>
<volume>58</volume>
<year>2010</year>
<fpage>403</fpage>
<lpage>419</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0085">
<label>16</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0085">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sjöberg</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Factors in risk perception</article-title>
<source>Risk Anal</source>
<volume>3</volume>
<year>2000</year>
<fpage>20</fpage>
<lpage>22</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0090">
<label>17</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0090">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Maswanya</surname>
<given-names>E.S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Moji</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Horiguchi</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nagata</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Aoyagi</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Honda</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Knowledge, risk perception of AIDS and reported sexual behaviour among students in secondary schools and colleges in Tanzania</article-title>
<source>Health Educ Res</source>
<volume>14</volume>
<year>1999</year>
<fpage>185</fpage>
<lpage>196</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10387499</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0095">
<label>18</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0095">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Van Damme</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Van Lerberghe</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Epidemics and fear</article-title>
<source>Trop Med Int Health</source>
<volume>5</volume>
<year>2000</year>
<fpage>511</fpage>
<lpage>514</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10995091</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0100">
<label>19</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0100">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rubsamen</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Castell</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Horn</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Karch</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ott</surname>
<given-names>J.J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Raupach-Rosin</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Ebola risk perception in Germany, 2014</article-title>
<source>Emerg Infect Dis</source>
<volume>21</volume>
<year>2015</year>
<fpage>1012</fpage>
<lpage>1018</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">25989020</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0105">
<label>20</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0105">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gesser-Edelsburg</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Shir-Raz</surname>
<given-names>Y.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hayek</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lev</surname>
<given-names>O.S.B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>What does the public know about Ebola?</article-title>
<source>Am J Infect Control</source>
<volume>43</volume>
<year>2015</year>
<fpage>669</fpage>
<lpage>675</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">25920703</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0110">
<label>21</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0110">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>de Zwart</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Veldhuijzen</surname>
<given-names>I.K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Elam</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Aro</surname>
<given-names>A.R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Abraham</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bishop</surname>
<given-names>G.D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Perceived threat, risk perception, and efficacy beliefs related to SARS and other (emerging) infectious diseases: results of an international survey</article-title>
<source>Int J Behav Med</source>
<volume>16</volume>
<year>2009</year>
<fpage>30</fpage>
<lpage>40</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19125335</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0115">
<label>22</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book" id="sr0115">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Korea Centers for Disease Control and Prevention</surname>
</name>
</person-group>
<chapter-title>MERS response guideline 2015</chapter-title>
<year>2015</year>
<publisher-name>Korea Centers for Disease Control and Prevention</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>Osong, South Korea</publisher-loc>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0120">
<label>23</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book" id="sr0120">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Shortell</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<chapter-title>An introduction to data analysis & presentation</chapter-title>
<year>2001</year>
<comment>Available from</comment>
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://academic.brooklyn.cuny.edu/soc/courses/712/chap18.html" id="iw9010">http://academic.brooklyn.cuny.edu/soc/courses/712/chap18.html</ext-link>
<comment>Accessed April 4, 2016</comment>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0125">
<label>24</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0125">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rhemtulla</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brosseau-Liard</surname>
<given-names>P.É.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Savalei</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>When can categorical variables be treated as continuous? A comparison of robust continuous and categorical SEM estimation methods under suboptimal conditions</article-title>
<source>Psychol Methods</source>
<volume>17</volume>
<year>2012</year>
<fpage>354</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22799625</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0130">
<label>25</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book" id="sr0130">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Thomas</surname>
<given-names>R.K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<chapter-title>Society and health: sociology for health professionals</chapter-title>
<year>2003</year>
<publisher-name>Springer Science & Business Media</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>New York (NY)</publisher-loc>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0135">
<label>26</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0135">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Jackob</surname>
<given-names>N.G.E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>No alternatives? The relationship between perceived media dependency, use of alternative information sources, and general trust in mass media</article-title>
<source>Int J Commun</source>
<volume>4</volume>
<year>2010</year>
<fpage>18</fpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0140">
<label>27</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0140">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kim</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Public trust in government in japan and south korea: does the rise of critical citizensmatter?</article-title>
<source>Public Admin Rev</source>
<volume>70</volume>
<year>2010</year>
<fpage>801</fpage>
<lpage>810</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0145">
<label>28</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book" id="sr0145">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Irwin</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<chapter-title>Citizen science: a study of people, expertise and sustainable development</chapter-title>
<year>1995</year>
<publisher-name>Psychology Press</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>New York (NY)</publisher-loc>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0150">
<label>29</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book" id="sr0150">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Dickson</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<chapter-title>The case for a ‘deficit model’ of science communication</chapter-title>
<source>SciDev.Net</source>
<year>2005</year>
<fpage>27</fpage>
<comment>Available from</comment>
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.scidev.net/global/communication/editorials/thecase-for-a-deficit-model-of-science-communic.html" id="iw9015">http://www.scidev.net/global/communication/editorials/thecase-for-a-deficit-model-of-science-communic.html</ext-link>
<comment>Accessed November 20, 2016</comment>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0155">
<label>30</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0155">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Berry</surname>
<given-names>T.R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wharf-Higgins</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Naylor</surname>
<given-names>P.J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>SARS wars: an examination of the quantity and construction of health information in the news media</article-title>
<source>Health Commun</source>
<volume>21</volume>
<year>2007</year>
<fpage>35</fpage>
<lpage>44</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17461750</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0160">
<label>31</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0160">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sjoberg</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Drottzsjoberg</surname>
<given-names>B.M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Knowledge and risk perception among nuclear-power-plant employees</article-title>
<source>Risk Anal</source>
<volume>11</volume>
<year>1991</year>
<fpage>607</fpage>
<lpage>618</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">1780501</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0165">
<label>32</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0165">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Stern</surname>
<given-names>P.C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Why do people sacrifice for their nations?</article-title>
<source>Polit Psychol</source>
<year>1995</year>
<fpage>217</fpage>
<lpage>235</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0170">
<label>33</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0170">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fung</surname>
<given-names>I.C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tse</surname>
<given-names>Z.T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cheung</surname>
<given-names>C.N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Miu</surname>
<given-names>A.S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fu</surname>
<given-names>K.W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Ebola and the social media</article-title>
<source>Lancet</source>
<volume>384</volume>
<year>2014</year>
<fpage>2207</fpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0175">
<label>34</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0175">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Altheide</surname>
<given-names>D.L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Moral panic: from sociological concept to public discourse</article-title>
<source>Crime Media Cult</source>
<volume>5</volume>
<year>2009</year>
<fpage>79</fpage>
<lpage>99</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0180">
<label>35</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0180">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Salathe</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Khandelwal</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Assessing vaccination sentiments with online social media: implications for infectious disease dynamics and control</article-title>
<source>PLoS Comput Biol</source>
<volume>7</volume>
<year>2011</year>
<fpage>e1002199</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22022249</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0185">
<label>36</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book" id="sr0185">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Yoda</surname>
<given-names>L.U.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nahl</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Crosby</surname>
<given-names>M.E.</given-names>
</name>
<collab>Stage-based mHealth communication interventions for HPV education</collab>
</person-group>
<chapter-title>Proceedings of the 2013 46th Hawaii International Conference on Science Systems</chapter-title>
<comment>Wailea, Hawaii; January 7-10</comment>
<year>2013</year>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0190">
<label>37</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0190">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kass-Hout</surname>
<given-names>T.A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Alhinnawi</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Social media in public health</article-title>
<source>Br Med Bull</source>
<volume>108</volume>
<year>2013</year>
<fpage>5</fpage>
<lpage>24</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">24103335</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0195">
<label>38</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0195">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>R.D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>Responding to global infectious disease outbreaks: lessons from SARS on the role of risk perception, communication and management</article-title>
<source>Soc Sci Med</source>
<volume>63</volume>
<year>2006</year>
<fpage>3113</fpage>
<lpage>3123</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16978751</pub-id>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0200">
<label>39</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book" id="sr0200">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sandman</surname>
<given-names>P.M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<chapter-title>Hazard versus outrage in the public perception of risk. Effective risk communication</chapter-title>
<year>1989</year>
<publisher-name>Springer</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>New York (NY)</publisher-loc>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0205">
<label>40</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0205">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Pangestu</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>MERS-CoV outbreak in South Korea–public policy trumped by fear and politics</article-title>
<source>Asian J Public Aff</source>
<volume>8</volume>
<year>2015</year>
<fpage>49</fpage>
<lpage>52</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0210">
<label>41</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book" id="sr0210">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cvetkovich</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lofstedt</surname>
<given-names>R.E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<chapter-title>Social trust and the management of risk</chapter-title>
<year>2013</year>
<publisher-name>Routledge</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>New York (NY)</publisher-loc>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0215">
<label>42</label>
<element-citation publication-type="book" id="sr0215">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Löfstedt</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<chapter-title>Risk management in post-trust societies</chapter-title>
<year>2005</year>
<publisher-name>Springer</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>New York (NY)</publisher-loc>
</element-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="bib0220">
<label>43</label>
<element-citation publication-type="journal" id="sr0220">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Nulty</surname>
<given-names>D.D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
<article-title>The adequacy of response rates to online and paper surveys: what can be done?</article-title>
<source>Assess Eval High Educ</source>
<volume>33</volume>
<year>2008</year>
<fpage>301</fpage>
<lpage>314</lpage>
</element-citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
<sec id="s0070" sec-type="supplementary-material">
<label>Appendix</label>
<title>Supplementary data</title>
<p id="p0195">The following is the supplementary data to this article:
<supplementary-material content-type="local-data" id="ec0010">
<caption>
<title>Appendix S1</title>
<p>Middle East respiratory syndrome questionnaire</p>
</caption>
<media xlink:href="mmc1.pdf">
<alt-text>Appendix S1</alt-text>
</media>
</supplementary-material>
</p>
</sec>
<ack id="ac0010">
<title>Acknowledgments</title>
<p>We thank all members, Hye-rin Park, Jee-eun Yi, Kang-jin Kim, Nam-kyu Cho, Nara Lee, and Yunisa Astiarani who participated in conducting the survey together in Community Health Field Training, one of the programs of a Master's course at the Graduate School of Public Health. We also thank the supporting from BK21 Plus.</p>
</ack>
<fn-group>
<fn id="d32e2419">
<p id="np9000">Funding/support: Supported by the Community Health Field Training Program, the Graduate School of Public Health,
<funding-source id="gsp9000">Seoul National University</funding-source>
.</p>
</fn>
<fn id="d32e2425">
<p id="np0015">Conflicts of interest: None to report.</p>
</fn>
<fn id="s0065" fn-type="supplementary-material">
<label>Appendix</label>
<p id="p0190">Supplementary data related to this article can be found at
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ajic.2017.02.013" id="iw0010">http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ajic.2017.02.013</ext-link>
.</p>
</fn>
</fn-group>
</back>
</pmc>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Sante/explor/MersV1/Data/Pmc/Corpus
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 000A37 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Corpus/biblio.hfd -nk 000A37 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Sante
   |area=    MersV1
   |flux=    Pmc
   |étape=   Corpus
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     PMC:7115287
   |texte=   Middle East respiratory syndrome risk perception among students at a university in South Korea, 2015
}}

Pour générer des pages wiki

HfdIndexSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Corpus/RBID.i   -Sk "pubmed:28385465" \
       | HfdSelect -Kh $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Corpus/biblio.hfd   \
       | NlmPubMed2Wicri -a MersV1 

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.33.
Data generation: Mon Apr 20 23:26:43 2020. Site generation: Sat Mar 27 09:06:09 2021