Serveur d'exploration sur le patient édenté

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

Male pipefish prefer dominant over attractive females

Identifieur interne : 004182 ( Istex/Corpus ); précédent : 004181; suivant : 004183

Male pipefish prefer dominant over attractive females

Auteurs : Anders Berglund ; Gunilla Rosenqvist

Source :

RBID : ISTEX:848CE6AC64DDA7ADE118030ED403544E3F7F7F6C

English descriptors

Abstract

Animals may obtain information guiding their choice between potential partners from observing competitive interactions and displays between them, or from displays directed at the choosing individual. In the sex-role reversed pipefish Syngnathus typhle females display a temporary ornament (a color pattern) to other females as well as to males. We have previously shown that display of female ornaments per se is attractive to males. Here we show that information from competitive displays can override such direct attraction displays as signals in the partner choice process. In a mate choice experiment, an enclosed male could choose between two females. On the first experimental day, females could interact freely, while on the second day they were isolated from each other. When female-female competition was allowed, the ornament display was directed more to the other female than to the male: Time competing, rather than time courting the male, correlated with ornament display duration. However, ornament display under competition and ornament display in the absence of competition did not correlate significantly. In fact, females competing more intensively on day one displayed the ornament less on day two. Furthermore, the ornament display during the first, but not the second, day predicted male mate choice on the second day. Thus, males remembered previous information from competitive displays and used it rather than immediate information from displays in the absence of female-female competition. We suggest that competitive displays more reliably signal female quality as compared to noncompetitive ones, and that males benefit from mating with dominant females.

Url:
DOI: 10.1093/beheco/12.4.402

Links to Exploration step

ISTEX:848CE6AC64DDA7ADE118030ED403544E3F7F7F6C

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI wicri:istexFullTextTei="biblStruct">
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">Male pipefish prefer dominant over attractive females</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Berglund, Anders" sort="Berglund, Anders" uniqKey="Berglund A" first="Anders" last="Berglund">Anders Berglund</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>Department of Animal Ecology, Uppsala University, Norbyvägen 18d, S-752 36 Uppsala, Sweden</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rosenqvist, Gunilla" sort="Rosenqvist, Gunilla" uniqKey="Rosenqvist G" first="Gunilla" last="Rosenqvist">Gunilla Rosenqvist</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>Department of Zoology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, N-7491 Trondheim, Norway</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">ISTEX</idno>
<idno type="RBID">ISTEX:848CE6AC64DDA7ADE118030ED403544E3F7F7F6C</idno>
<date when="2001" year="2001">2001</date>
<idno type="doi">10.1093/beheco/12.4.402</idno>
<idno type="url">https://api.istex.fr/document/848CE6AC64DDA7ADE118030ED403544E3F7F7F6C/fulltext/pdf</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Istex/Corpus">004182</idno>
<idno type="wicri:explorRef" wicri:stream="Istex" wicri:step="Corpus" wicri:corpus="ISTEX">004182</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">Male pipefish prefer dominant over attractive females</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Berglund, Anders" sort="Berglund, Anders" uniqKey="Berglund A" first="Anders" last="Berglund">Anders Berglund</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>Department of Animal Ecology, Uppsala University, Norbyvägen 18d, S-752 36 Uppsala, Sweden</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rosenqvist, Gunilla" sort="Rosenqvist, Gunilla" uniqKey="Rosenqvist G" first="Gunilla" last="Rosenqvist">Gunilla Rosenqvist</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>Department of Zoology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, N-7491 Trondheim, Norway</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<monogr></monogr>
<series>
<title level="j">Behavioral Ecology</title>
<title level="j" type="abbrev">Behavioral Ecology</title>
<idno type="ISSN">1045-2249</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1465-7279</idno>
<imprint>
<publisher>Oxford University Press</publisher>
<date type="published" when="2001-07">2001-07</date>
<biblScope unit="volume">12</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="issue">4</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" from="402">402</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" to="406">406</biblScope>
</imprint>
<idno type="ISSN">1045-2249</idno>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
<seriesStmt>
<idno type="ISSN">1045-2249</idno>
</seriesStmt>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass>
<keywords scheme="KwdEn" xml:lang="en">
<term>Anim behav berglund</term>
<term>Aquarium</term>
<term>Attraction display</term>
<term>Attraction displays</term>
<term>Attractive displays</term>
<term>Behav</term>
<term>Behav ecol sociobiol berglund</term>
<term>Behavioral ecology</term>
<term>Berglund</term>
<term>Bernet</term>
<term>Brood pouch</term>
<term>Brown females</term>
<term>Brown males</term>
<term>Choice index</term>
<term>Competition duration</term>
<term>Competitive ability</term>
<term>Competitive displays</term>
<term>Competitive interactions</term>
<term>Cowbird song</term>
<term>Dancing duration</term>
<term>Direct observations</term>
<term>Dominant females</term>
<term>Dominant males</term>
<term>Ecol</term>
<term>Female choice</term>
<term>Female ornaments</term>
<term>Female pair</term>
<term>Green females</term>
<term>High quality females</term>
<term>Indirect mate choice</term>
<term>Intense competition</term>
<term>Linear regression line</term>
<term>Male choice</term>
<term>Male competition</term>
<term>Male mate choice</term>
<term>Males mating</term>
<term>Maria sandvik</term>
<term>Mate choice</term>
<term>Mate choice experiment</term>
<term>Mating competition</term>
<term>Opaque divider</term>
<term>Ornament</term>
<term>Ornament display</term>
<term>Ornament display duration</term>
<term>Other females</term>
<term>Risky behaviors</term>
<term>Rosenqvist</term>
<term>Same result</term>
<term>Same signal</term>
<term>Sexual selection</term>
<term>Shorter time</term>
<term>Sociobiol</term>
<term>Syngnathus typhle</term>
<term>Temporary ornament</term>
<term>Total time</term>
<term>Typhle</term>
<term>Wilcoxon test</term>
<term>mate choice</term>
<term>mate competition</term>
<term>ornament</term>
<term>pipefish</term>
<term>sexual selection</term>
<term>signal honesty</term>
</keywords>
<keywords scheme="Teeft" xml:lang="en">
<term>Anim behav berglund</term>
<term>Aquarium</term>
<term>Attraction display</term>
<term>Attraction displays</term>
<term>Attractive displays</term>
<term>Behav</term>
<term>Behav ecol sociobiol berglund</term>
<term>Behavioral ecology</term>
<term>Berglund</term>
<term>Bernet</term>
<term>Brood pouch</term>
<term>Brown females</term>
<term>Brown males</term>
<term>Choice index</term>
<term>Competition duration</term>
<term>Competitive ability</term>
<term>Competitive displays</term>
<term>Competitive interactions</term>
<term>Cowbird song</term>
<term>Dancing duration</term>
<term>Direct observations</term>
<term>Dominant females</term>
<term>Dominant males</term>
<term>Ecol</term>
<term>Female choice</term>
<term>Female ornaments</term>
<term>Female pair</term>
<term>Green females</term>
<term>High quality females</term>
<term>Indirect mate choice</term>
<term>Intense competition</term>
<term>Linear regression line</term>
<term>Male choice</term>
<term>Male competition</term>
<term>Male mate choice</term>
<term>Males mating</term>
<term>Maria sandvik</term>
<term>Mate choice</term>
<term>Mate choice experiment</term>
<term>Mating competition</term>
<term>Opaque divider</term>
<term>Ornament</term>
<term>Ornament display</term>
<term>Ornament display duration</term>
<term>Other females</term>
<term>Risky behaviors</term>
<term>Rosenqvist</term>
<term>Same result</term>
<term>Same signal</term>
<term>Sexual selection</term>
<term>Shorter time</term>
<term>Sociobiol</term>
<term>Syngnathus typhle</term>
<term>Temporary ornament</term>
<term>Total time</term>
<term>Typhle</term>
<term>Wilcoxon test</term>
</keywords>
</textClass>
<langUsage>
<language ident="en">en</language>
</langUsage>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">Animals may obtain information guiding their choice between potential partners from observing competitive interactions and displays between them, or from displays directed at the choosing individual. In the sex-role reversed pipefish Syngnathus typhle females display a temporary ornament (a color pattern) to other females as well as to males. We have previously shown that display of female ornaments per se is attractive to males. Here we show that information from competitive displays can override such direct attraction displays as signals in the partner choice process. In a mate choice experiment, an enclosed male could choose between two females. On the first experimental day, females could interact freely, while on the second day they were isolated from each other. When female-female competition was allowed, the ornament display was directed more to the other female than to the male: Time competing, rather than time courting the male, correlated with ornament display duration. However, ornament display under competition and ornament display in the absence of competition did not correlate significantly. In fact, females competing more intensively on day one displayed the ornament less on day two. Furthermore, the ornament display during the first, but not the second, day predicted male mate choice on the second day. Thus, males remembered previous information from competitive displays and used it rather than immediate information from displays in the absence of female-female competition. We suggest that competitive displays more reliably signal female quality as compared to noncompetitive ones, and that males benefit from mating with dominant females.</div>
</front>
</TEI>
<istex>
<corpusName>oup</corpusName>
<keywords>
<teeft>
<json:string>berglund</json:string>
<json:string>ornament display</json:string>
<json:string>behav</json:string>
<json:string>rosenqvist</json:string>
<json:string>ecol</json:string>
<json:string>aquarium</json:string>
<json:string>typhle</json:string>
<json:string>mate choice</json:string>
<json:string>sociobiol</json:string>
<json:string>ornament</json:string>
<json:string>dancing duration</json:string>
<json:string>competitive interactions</json:string>
<json:string>dominant females</json:string>
<json:string>competitive displays</json:string>
<json:string>competition duration</json:string>
<json:string>syngnathus typhle</json:string>
<json:string>other females</json:string>
<json:string>total time</json:string>
<json:string>male mate choice</json:string>
<json:string>competitive ability</json:string>
<json:string>bernet</json:string>
<json:string>brown males</json:string>
<json:string>female ornaments</json:string>
<json:string>behavioral ecology</json:string>
<json:string>intense competition</json:string>
<json:string>brown females</json:string>
<json:string>behav ecol sociobiol berglund</json:string>
<json:string>male competition</json:string>
<json:string>female choice</json:string>
<json:string>cowbird song</json:string>
<json:string>risky behaviors</json:string>
<json:string>males mating</json:string>
<json:string>high quality females</json:string>
<json:string>opaque divider</json:string>
<json:string>male choice</json:string>
<json:string>wilcoxon test</json:string>
<json:string>attraction displays</json:string>
<json:string>shorter time</json:string>
<json:string>green females</json:string>
<json:string>same signal</json:string>
<json:string>mate choice experiment</json:string>
<json:string>direct observations</json:string>
<json:string>choice index</json:string>
<json:string>same result</json:string>
<json:string>linear regression line</json:string>
<json:string>attractive displays</json:string>
<json:string>female pair</json:string>
<json:string>indirect mate choice</json:string>
<json:string>attraction display</json:string>
<json:string>maria sandvik</json:string>
<json:string>anim behav berglund</json:string>
<json:string>brood pouch</json:string>
<json:string>sexual selection</json:string>
<json:string>temporary ornament</json:string>
<json:string>ornament display duration</json:string>
<json:string>dominant males</json:string>
<json:string>mating competition</json:string>
</teeft>
</keywords>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>Anders Berglund</name>
<affiliations>
<json:string>Department of Animal Ecology, Uppsala University, Norbyvägen 18d, S-752 36 Uppsala, Sweden</json:string>
</affiliations>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>Gunilla Rosenqvist</name>
<affiliations>
<json:string>Department of Zoology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, N-7491 Trondheim, Norway</json:string>
</affiliations>
</json:item>
</author>
<subject>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>mate choice</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>mate competition</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>ornament</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>pipefish</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>sexual selection</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>signal honesty</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>Syngnathus typhle</value>
</json:item>
</subject>
<arkIstex>ark:/67375/HXZ-305XLPLB-V</arkIstex>
<language>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</language>
<originalGenre>
<json:string>research-article</json:string>
</originalGenre>
<abstract>Animals may obtain information guiding their choice between potential partners from observing competitive interactions and displays between them, or from displays directed at the choosing individual. In the sex-role reversed pipefish Syngnathus typhle females display a temporary ornament (a color pattern) to other females as well as to males. We have previously shown that display of female ornaments per se is attractive to males. Here we show that information from competitive displays can override such direct attraction displays as signals in the partner choice process. In a mate choice experiment, an enclosed male could choose between two females. On the first experimental day, females could interact freely, while on the second day they were isolated from each other. When female-female competition was allowed, the ornament display was directed more to the other female than to the male: Time competing, rather than time courting the male, correlated with ornament display duration. However, ornament display under competition and ornament display in the absence of competition did not correlate significantly. In fact, females competing more intensively on day one displayed the ornament less on day two. Furthermore, the ornament display during the first, but not the second, day predicted male mate choice on the second day. Thus, males remembered previous information from competitive displays and used it rather than immediate information from displays in the absence of female-female competition. We suggest that competitive displays more reliably signal female quality as compared to noncompetitive ones, and that males benefit from mating with dominant females.</abstract>
<qualityIndicators>
<score>9.372</score>
<pdfWordCount>4372</pdfWordCount>
<pdfCharCount>25767</pdfCharCount>
<pdfVersion>1.3</pdfVersion>
<pdfPageCount>5</pdfPageCount>
<pdfPageSize>612 x 792 pts (letter)</pdfPageSize>
<refBibsNative>true</refBibsNative>
<abstractWordCount>250</abstractWordCount>
<abstractCharCount>1681</abstractCharCount>
<keywordCount>7</keywordCount>
</qualityIndicators>
<title>Male pipefish prefer dominant over attractive females</title>
<pii>
<json:string>1465-7279</json:string>
</pii>
<genre>
<json:string>research-article</json:string>
</genre>
<host>
<title>Behavioral Ecology</title>
<language>
<json:string>unknown</json:string>
</language>
<issn>
<json:string>1045-2249</json:string>
</issn>
<eissn>
<json:string>1465-7279</json:string>
</eissn>
<publisherId>
<json:string>beheco</json:string>
</publisherId>
<volume>12</volume>
<issue>4</issue>
<pages>
<first>402</first>
<last>406</last>
</pages>
<genre>
<json:string>journal</json:string>
</genre>
</host>
<namedEntities>
<unitex>
<date>
<json:string>1997</json:string>
<json:string>2001</json:string>
<json:string>1996</json:string>
</date>
<geogName></geogName>
<orgName>
<json:string>Norwegian University</json:string>
<json:string>Norway Animals</json:string>
<json:string>Kristineberg Marine Research Station</json:string>
<json:string>Society for Behavioral Ecology</json:string>
<json:string>Norwegian Research Council</json:string>
<json:string>Swedish Natural Research Council</json:string>
</orgName>
<orgName_funder>
<json:string>Swedish Natural Research Council</json:string>
</orgName_funder>
<orgName_provider></orgName_provider>
<persName>
<json:string>Klubban Biological</json:string>
<json:string>Gunilla</json:string>
<json:string>Hanna Schioler-Wasslavik</json:string>
<json:string>Elisabet Forsgren</json:string>
<json:string>Inserts</json:string>
<json:string>Maria Sandvik</json:string>
<json:string>Ingrid Ahnesjo</json:string>
<json:string>A. Berglund</json:string>
<json:string>Ian Fleming</json:string>
</persName>
<placeName>
<json:string>Trondheim</json:string>
<json:string>Sweden</json:string>
</placeName>
<ref_url></ref_url>
<ref_bibl>
<json:string>Berglund and Rosenqvist, 2001</json:string>
<json:string>a longer time with the female has earlier been shown to accurately predict mate choice; Berglund, 1994</json:string>
<json:string>Wiley and Poston, 1996</json:string>
<json:string>Berglund and Rosenqvist, 1990, 1993</json:string>
<json:string>Berglund, 1993</json:string>
<json:string>Thornhill, 1988</json:string>
<json:string>Berglund, 1999, 2000</json:string>
<json:string>Fiedler, 1954</json:string>
<json:string>Berglund et al., 1986a</json:string>
<json:string>Jones et al., 1999</json:string>
<json:string>Vincent et al., 1995</json:string>
<json:string>Fuller and Berglund, 1996</json:string>
<json:string>Berglund et al., 1986a,b</json:string>
<json:string>Farr and Travis, 1986</json:string>
<json:string>West et al., 1981</json:string>
<json:string>Qvarnstrom and Fors¨ gren, 1998</json:string>
<json:string>Berglund, 1991</json:string>
<json:string>Vincent et al., 1994</json:string>
<json:string>Bisazza et al., 1989a,b</json:string>
<json:string>Viljugrein, 1997</json:string>
<json:string>Vincent et al., 1994, 1995</json:string>
<json:string>Berglund et al., 1997</json:string>
<json:string>Mateos and Carranza, 1997, 1999</json:string>
<json:string>Forsgren, 1997</json:string>
<json:string>Borgia, 1981</json:string>
<json:string>Cox and LeBoeuf, 1977</json:string>
<json:string>Bernet et al., 1998</json:string>
<json:string>Moore and Moore, 1999</json:string>
<json:string>Berglund et al., 1996</json:string>
<json:string>Kokko, 1997</json:string>
</ref_bibl>
<bibl></bibl>
</unitex>
</namedEntities>
<ark>
<json:string>ark:/67375/HXZ-305XLPLB-V</json:string>
</ark>
<categories>
<wos>
<json:string>science</json:string>
<json:string>zoology</json:string>
<json:string>ecology</json:string>
<json:string>behavioral sciences</json:string>
</wos>
<scienceMetrix>
<json:string>health sciences</json:string>
<json:string>psychology & cognitive sciences</json:string>
<json:string>behavioral science & comparative psychology</json:string>
</scienceMetrix>
<scopus>
<json:string>1 - Life Sciences</json:string>
<json:string>2 - Agricultural and Biological Sciences</json:string>
<json:string>3 - Animal Science and Zoology</json:string>
<json:string>1 - Life Sciences</json:string>
<json:string>2 - Agricultural and Biological Sciences</json:string>
<json:string>3 - Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics</json:string>
</scopus>
<inist>
<json:string>sciences appliquees, technologies et medecines</json:string>
<json:string>sciences biologiques et medicales</json:string>
<json:string>sciences biologiques fondamentales et appliquees. psychologie</json:string>
</inist>
</categories>
<publicationDate>2001</publicationDate>
<copyrightDate>2001</copyrightDate>
<doi>
<json:string>10.1093/beheco/12.4.402</json:string>
</doi>
<id>848CE6AC64DDA7ADE118030ED403544E3F7F7F6C</id>
<score>1</score>
<fulltext>
<json:item>
<extension>pdf</extension>
<original>true</original>
<mimetype>application/pdf</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/848CE6AC64DDA7ADE118030ED403544E3F7F7F6C/fulltext/pdf</uri>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<extension>zip</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>application/zip</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/848CE6AC64DDA7ADE118030ED403544E3F7F7F6C/fulltext/zip</uri>
</json:item>
<istex:fulltextTEI uri="https://api.istex.fr/document/848CE6AC64DDA7ADE118030ED403544E3F7F7F6C/fulltext/tei">
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">Male pipefish prefer dominant over attractive females</title>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<authority>ISTEX</authority>
<publisher scheme="https://publisher-list.data.istex.fr">Oxford University Press</publisher>
<availability>
<licence>
<p>International Society of Behavioral Ecology</p>
</licence>
<p scheme="https://loaded-corpus.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/XBH-GTWS0RDP-M">oup</p>
</availability>
<date>2001</date>
</publicationStmt>
<notesStmt>
<note type="research-article" scheme="https://content-type.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/XTP-1JC4F85T-7">research-article</note>
<note type="journal" scheme="https://publication-type.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/JMC-0GLKJH51-B">journal</note>
<note>Address correspondence to A. Berglund. E-mail: anders.berglund@ebc.uu.se .</note>
</notesStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct type="inbook">
<analytic>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">Male pipefish prefer dominant over attractive females</title>
<author xml:id="author-0000">
<persName>
<forename type="first">Anders</forename>
<surname>Berglund</surname>
</persName>
<affiliation>Department of Animal Ecology, Uppsala University, Norbyvägen 18d, S-752 36 Uppsala, Sweden</affiliation>
</author>
<author xml:id="author-0001">
<persName>
<forename type="first">Gunilla</forename>
<surname>Rosenqvist</surname>
</persName>
<affiliation>Department of Zoology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, N-7491 Trondheim, Norway</affiliation>
</author>
<idno type="istex">848CE6AC64DDA7ADE118030ED403544E3F7F7F6C</idno>
<idno type="ark">ark:/67375/HXZ-305XLPLB-V</idno>
<idno type="DOI">10.1093/beheco/12.4.402</idno>
<idno type="PII">1465-7279</idno>
<idno type="local">0120402</idno>
</analytic>
<monogr>
<title level="j">Behavioral Ecology</title>
<title level="j" type="abbrev">Behavioral Ecology</title>
<idno type="pISSN">1045-2249</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1465-7279</idno>
<idno type="publisher-id">beheco</idno>
<idno type="PublisherID-hwp">beheco</idno>
<idno type="PublisherID-nlm-ta">Behav Ecol</idno>
<imprint>
<publisher>Oxford University Press</publisher>
<date type="published" when="2001-07"></date>
<biblScope unit="volume">12</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="issue">4</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" from="402">402</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" to="406">406</biblScope>
</imprint>
</monogr>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<creation>
<date>2001</date>
</creation>
<langUsage>
<language ident="en">en</language>
</langUsage>
<abstract xml:lang="en">
<p>Animals may obtain information guiding their choice between potential partners from observing competitive interactions and displays between them, or from displays directed at the choosing individual. In the sex-role reversed pipefish Syngnathus typhle females display a temporary ornament (a color pattern) to other females as well as to males. We have previously shown that display of female ornaments per se is attractive to males. Here we show that information from competitive displays can override such direct attraction displays as signals in the partner choice process. In a mate choice experiment, an enclosed male could choose between two females. On the first experimental day, females could interact freely, while on the second day they were isolated from each other. When female-female competition was allowed, the ornament display was directed more to the other female than to the male: Time competing, rather than time courting the male, correlated with ornament display duration. However, ornament display under competition and ornament display in the absence of competition did not correlate significantly. In fact, females competing more intensively on day one displayed the ornament less on day two. Furthermore, the ornament display during the first, but not the second, day predicted male mate choice on the second day. Thus, males remembered previous information from competitive displays and used it rather than immediate information from displays in the absence of female-female competition. We suggest that competitive displays more reliably signal female quality as compared to noncompetitive ones, and that males benefit from mating with dominant females.</p>
</abstract>
<textClass xml:lang="en">
<keywords scheme="keyword">
<list>
<head>KWD</head>
<item>
<term>mate choice</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>mate competition</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>ornament</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>pipefish</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>sexual selection</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>signal honesty</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>Syngnathus typhle</term>
</item>
</list>
</keywords>
</textClass>
</profileDesc>
<revisionDesc>
<change when="2001-07">Published</change>
</revisionDesc>
</teiHeader>
</istex:fulltextTEI>
<json:item>
<extension>txt</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>text/plain</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/848CE6AC64DDA7ADE118030ED403544E3F7F7F6C/fulltext/txt</uri>
</json:item>
</fulltext>
<metadata>
<istex:metadataXml wicri:clean="corpus oup, element #text not found" wicri:toSee="no header">
<istex:xmlDeclaration>version="1.0" encoding="US-ASCII"</istex:xmlDeclaration>
<istex:docType PUBLIC="-//NLM//DTD Journal Publishing DTD v2.3 20070202//EN" URI="journalpublishing.dtd" name="istex:docType"></istex:docType>
<istex:document>
<article xml:lang="en" article-type="research-article">
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="hwp">beheco</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="nlm-ta">Behav Ecol</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="publisher-id">beheco</journal-id>
<journal-title>Behavioral Ecology</journal-title>
<abbrev-journal-title abbrev-type="publisher">Behavioral Ecology</abbrev-journal-title>
<issn pub-type="ppub">1045-2249</issn>
<issn pub-type="epub">1465-7279</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1093/beheco/12.4.402</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="other">0120402</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="pii">1465-7279</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Article</subject>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>Male pipefish prefer dominant over attractive females</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Berglund</surname>
<given-names>Anders</given-names>
</name>
<xref rid="AFF1">
<sup>a</sup>
</xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rosenqvist</surname>
<given-names>Gunilla</given-names>
</name>
<xref rid="AFF2">
<sup>b</sup>
</xref>
</contrib>
<aff id="AFF1">
<label>
<sup>a</sup>
</label>
Department of Animal Ecology, Uppsala University, Norbyvägen 18d, S-752 36 Uppsala, Sweden</aff>
<aff id="AFF2">
<label>
<sup>b</sup>
</label>
Department of Zoology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, N-7491 Trondheim, Norway</aff>
</contrib-group>
<author-notes>
<corresp> Address correspondence to A. Berglund. E-mail:
<ext-link xlink:href="anders.berglund@ebc.uu.se" ext-link-type="email">anders.berglund@ebc.uu.se</ext-link>
.</corresp>
</author-notes>
<pub-date pub-type="ppub">
<month>7</month>
<year>2001</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>12</volume>
<issue>4</issue>
<fpage>402</fpage>
<lpage>406</lpage>
<history>
<date date-type="accepted">
<day>7</day>
<month>9</month>
<year>2000</year>
</date>
<date date-type="received">
<day>14</day>
<month>4</month>
<year>2000</year>
</date>
<date date-type="rev-recd">
<day>1</day>
<month>9</month>
<year>2000</year>
</date>
</history>
<permissions>
<copyright-statement>International Society of Behavioral Ecology</copyright-statement>
<copyright-year>2001</copyright-year>
</permissions>
<abstract xml:lang="en">
<p>Animals may obtain information guiding their choice between potential partners from observing competitive interactions and displays between them, or from displays directed at the choosing individual. In the sex-role reversed pipefish
<italic>Syngnathus typhle</italic>
females display a temporary ornament (a color pattern) to other females as well as to males. We have previously shown that display of female ornaments per se is attractive to males. Here we show that information from competitive displays can override such direct attraction displays as signals in the partner choice process. In a mate choice experiment, an enclosed male could choose between two females. On the first experimental day, females could interact freely, while on the second day they were isolated from each other. When female-female competition was allowed, the ornament display was directed more to the other female than to the male: Time competing, rather than time courting the male, correlated with ornament display duration. However, ornament display under competition and ornament display in the absence of competition did not correlate significantly. In fact, females competing more intensively on day one displayed the ornament less on day two. Furthermore, the ornament display during the first, but not the second, day predicted male mate choice on the second day. Thus, males remembered previous information from competitive displays and used it rather than immediate information from displays in the absence of female-female competition. We suggest that competitive displays more reliably signal female quality as compared to noncompetitive ones, and that males benefit from mating with dominant females.</p>
</abstract>
<kwd-group kwd-group-type="KWD" xml:lang="en">
<kwd>mate choice</kwd>
<kwd>mate competition</kwd>
<kwd>ornament</kwd>
<kwd>pipefish</kwd>
<kwd>sexual selection</kwd>
<kwd>signal honesty</kwd>
<kwd>
<italic>Syngnathus typhle</italic>
</kwd>
</kwd-group>
<custom-meta-wrap>
<custom-meta>
<meta-name>hwp-legacy-fpage</meta-name>
<meta-value>402</meta-value>
</custom-meta>
<custom-meta>
<meta-name>hwp-legacy-dochead</meta-name>
<meta-value>Article</meta-value>
</custom-meta>
</custom-meta-wrap>
</article-meta>
</front>
<body>
<sec>
<title></title>
<p>A sexually selected signal usually serves both as an armament (a weapon or a status badge) and as an ornament (a mate attractant;
<xref rid="REF6">Berglund et al., 1996</xref>
). However, this may not be invariably true for all sexually selected signals (
<xref rid="REF20">Forsgren, 1997</xref>
;
<xref rid="REF26">Moore and Moore, 1999</xref>
;
<xref rid="REF27">Qvarnström and Forsgren, 1998</xref>
), because messages like “high fighting ability” and “tender parental care” may be difficult to communicate convincingly through the same signal (Zahavi A, personal communication). Nevertheless, in a majority of cases the same signal has been found to function in both contexts (
<xref rid="REF6">Berglund et al., 1996</xref>
; Mateos and Carranza,
<xref rid="REF24">1997</xref>
,
<xref rid="REF25">1999</xref>
). Furthermore, signals of competitive ability can be expected to be attractive in themselves. Given that such information is used in mate choice, will a direct observation of competitive interactions/displays be more important to a choosing individual than observations of some ornamental display meant to attract the chooser? Since not necessarily all individuals signal their quality honestly, we expect direct observations of competitive displays to yield more reliable information about the condition of the potential partner than do purely attractive displays. This is because condition is immediately put to trial in a contest situation (through potentially costly fights), whereas the punishment for discovery of a faked signal of attraction is much less severe (a lost mating opportunity; see e.g.,
<xref rid="REF6">Berglund et al., 1996</xref>
;
<xref rid="REF32">West and King, 1980</xref>
;
<xref rid="REF33">West et al., 1981</xref>
). Because a low frequency of dishonest signals of attraction may be evolutionarily stable (
<xref rid="REF23">Kokko, 1997</xref>
;
<xref rid="REF29">Viljugrein, 1997</xref>
), a preference for sources of information that have passed a test may be common in animals. This is further corroborated by the frequent observation that competitive encounters in many species seem to be staged or incited by the choosing sex (Bisazza et al.,
<xref rid="REF14">1989a</xref>
,
<xref rid="REF15">b</xref>
;
<xref rid="REF16">Borgia, 1981</xref>
;
<xref rid="REF17">Cox and LeBoeuf, 1977</xref>
;
<xref rid="REF18">Farr and Travis, 1986</xref>
;
<xref rid="REF28">Thornhill, 1988</xref>
).</p>
<p>In this article we show that in a sex-role reversed species, the pipefish
<italic>Syngnathus typhle</italic>
L., a male's mate choice is influenced more by a female's competitive display than by her attraction display. We also show that males are able to remember and utilize earlier information about competitive ability in preference to more recent information from attraction display in their choice of mates, indicating the overriding value of information from competitive interactions for mate choice in this species.</p>
<sec>
<title>The pipefish</title>
<p>A lengthy and ritualized mutual dance precedes copulation in all pipefish. In
<italic>S. typhle</italic>
, females transfer eggs to a brood pouch under the male's tail, where he then fertilizes them. Subsequently, embryos are nourished and oxygenated in the pouch until birth several weeks later (Berglund et al.,
<xref rid="REF11">1986a</xref>
,
<xref rid="REF12">b</xref>
). Males consequently enjoy a paternity confidence of exactly 100%, as confirmed by a recent microsatellite analysis of maternity and paternity in this species (
<xref rid="REF22">Jones et al., 1999</xref>
). From many laboratory experiments and field observations we already know that
<italic>S. typhle</italic>
is sex-role reversed: males are typically more choosy and females compete more intensely than males for access to mates (Berglund,
<xref rid="REF4">1999</xref>
,
<xref rid="REF5">2000</xref>
; Berglund and Rosenqvist,
<xref rid="REF7">1990</xref>
,
<xref rid="REF8">1993</xref>
; Berglund et al.,
<xref rid="REF11">1986a</xref>
,
<xref rid="REF12">b</xref>
; Vincent et al.,
<xref rid="REF30">1994</xref>
,
<xref rid="REF31">1995</xref>
). Fecundity increases with body size in both males and females, and given a choice both prefer to mate with larger partners (
<xref rid="REF11">Berglund et al., 1986a</xref>
).</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>The ornament</title>
<p>Female
<italic>S. typhle</italic>
display a temporary ornament when interacting either with other females or with males. The ornament is a striped contrasting dark color pattern resembling a row of capital B's along the female's side (
<xref rid="REF19">Fiedler, 1954</xref>
). Ornament appearance is rapid, occurring within a minute, and the display may also vanish just as fast. Both males and females are normally extremely cryptic because their color, shape, and movements look like eelgrass (
<italic>Zostera marina</italic>
L.; Vincent et al.,
<xref rid="REF30">1994</xref>
,
<xref rid="REF31">1995</xref>
). Therefore, the ornament display decreases crypsis in females, which consequently are reluctant to display under perceived predation threat (
<xref rid="REF13">Bernet et al., 1998</xref>
).</p>
<p>We have previously shown that females who spontaneously display the ornament for a longer time enjoy a higher mating success than females giving briefer display (
<xref rid="REF13">Bernet et al., 1998</xref>
). Furthermore, when kept with a male, females are more likely to display the ornament under female-female competition than in the absence of competition (
<xref rid="REF13">Bernet et al., 1998</xref>
). Moreover, females displaying more are also more fecund than similar-sized females displaying less (
<xref rid="REF10">Berglund et al., 1997</xref>
). In addition, males have been found to prefer ornamented females over nonornamented ones: an experiment where females were manipulated (painted) to differ in ornamentation but not in behavior (females were sedated and mechanically moved up and down by a motor) confirmed that males pay attention to the ornament even when female behavior was kept constant (
<xref rid="REF9">Berglund and Rosenqvist, 2001</xref>
).</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Pipefish mating competition</title>
<p>Overt aggression is less apparent in these slow-moving, joint-jawed, and toothless fishes. Female
<italic>S. typhle</italic>
compete for males indirectly through dominance hierarchies, and large females may interfere with and substantially decrease reproduction in small ones (
<xref rid="REF1">Berglund, 1991</xref>
). Competition may, however, also be overt. In nature male
<italic>S. typhle</italic>
actively search for, choose among, and reject some females, while females vigorously display, often in temporary groups in a lek-like fashion. Males typically swim cryptically within the eelgrass in search of females. Females display by swimming up and down and in and out of the eelgrass, with their ornaments in full bloom. Once such a displaying group of females is encountered by a male, he may start dancing and mating with one of them, usually a large individual (Vincent et al.,
<xref rid="REF30">1994</xref>
,
<xref rid="REF31">1995</xref>
). Females actively compete among themselves for matings during such group displays, and may try to herd other females away from the male (
<xref rid="REF31">Vincent et al., 1995</xref>
). When this happens, mating is interrupted, and indeed attempts to copulate can be thwarted for a long time by interference from other females (Berglund A and Rosenqvist G, own field and aquaria observations, unpublished data).</p>
<p>Displaying, dancing, and mating are risky behaviors in the otherwise reticent life of pipefish (
<xref rid="REF2">Berglund, 1993</xref>
;
<xref rid="REF13">Bernet et al., 1998</xref>
;
<xref rid="REF21">Fuller and Berglund, 1996</xref>
). Therefore, it may well pay males to select dominant females, which are able to discourage other females from courtship interference, as this will reduce the time spent on potentially risky behaviors. Moreover, males do not have to worry about the potential conflict between female signals of dominance and signals of caring ability, as females contribute no care, only eggs. Moreover, we have never observed female aggression towards males. Thus, our prediction is that males should treasure dominance and competitive ability highly in females, and that direct observations by males of such abilities in females may be more important than attraction displays in the male's mate choice process. Indeed, any female can display her ornament, but females having done so under actual female-female competition have demonstrated their power and competitive ability in a potentially costly situation. Therefore, males mating with such evidently dominant females can expect a smooth courtship and copulation process, gaining direct benefits for themselves in terms of reduced risk, as well as possible direct and/or genetic benefits for their offspring from mating with high quality females.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>METHODS</title>
<p>We caught the pipefish in shallow eelgrass meadows in the Gullmar Fjord on the Swedish west coast during the latter half of May 1997, before the breeding season commenced. We kept sexually mature males and females in separate aquaria to assure that individuals would be reproductively active. Stock aquaria contained plastic plants and continuously renewed sea water (natural temperature, salinity, and light regime). Fishes were fed brine shrimps (
<italic>Artemia</italic>
), small, wild-caught crustaceans, and frozen mysids
<italic>ad lib</italic>
.</p>
<p>To explore the importance of competitive versus purely attractive displays we ran a mate choice experiment 28 May-20 June. Each of 24 trials was run for 2 days, with one male choosing between two females. The females came from different stock aquaria. The male was separated from the females by a transparent plastic divider. During the first day females could interact freely with one another, but during the second day they were separated from one another by an opaque divider (
<xref rid="FIG1">Figure 1</xref>
). Separated females could not see or smell each other, but the male could see and smell both females both days as water flowed from the female compartment(s) into the male's and then out of the aquarium. Thus, males could observe and dance with females on the first day, and females could compete, dance and display their ornament on that day. On day one, we placed the females in the rear compartment and the male in the front compartment (
<xref rid="FIG1">Figure 1</xref>
), and videotaped them for 10 h. On this day, we measured the time each fish spent dancing, how long females displayed their ornaments and whether females competed or not. A male and a female were defined as dancing when they simultaneously bobbed up and down in close proximity while facing one another. We considered females to be competing when they pursued each other round in the aquarium for more than a minute.</p>
<p>
<fig id="FIG1" position="float">
<label>
<bold>Figure 1</bold>
</label>
<caption>
<p>The experimental design on day two. A male with his brood pouch is seen in the foreground, and two enclosed females in the back. On day one the opaque partitioning between the females was absent.</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="bhec-12-04-06-f01"></graphic>
</fig>
</p>
<p>On the second day, we measured male choice of a female in the absence of female-female competition by placing the opaque divider between the females and filming for another 10 h. Females were allocated to either chamber randomly. After that males and females were released back into the wild. No mortality occurred during trials. We measured the duration of female resting, swimming, dancing, and ornament display, and the duration of male resting, swimming, and dancing before either female. The female with which the male spent the longest time was considered as the chosen one (a longer time with the female has earlier been shown to accurately predict mate choice;
<xref rid="REF3">Berglund, 1994</xref>
).</p>
<p>We filmed two trials simultaneously in 125 1 aquaria (45 × 50 × 60 [height] cm). The aquaria were continuously provided with surface sea water. Temperature and light conditions followed natural conditions. We planted fresh eelgrass in beach sand in the aquaria for shelter. Fish were not fed during trials. We selected females of similar standard length (within 10 mm of each other) and of the same contrast score (the vaguely striped basic pattern from which the ornament is formed, as estimated by eye on a five-grade scale; see
<xref rid="REF13">Bernet et al., 1998</xref>
for details), but individually recognizable by color (estimated by eye). New males and females were used in each trial. Males were smaller than females (males 177 ± 21 mm,
<italic>n</italic>
= 24, females 226 ± 27 mm,
<italic>n</italic>
= 48,
<italic>t</italic>
<sub>70</sub>
= 8.00,
<italic>p</italic>
<.001), as in the natural situation (in our 1997 field samples males were on average 161 ± 28 mm, females 191 ± 38 mm).</p>
<p>Videotaping was done with Hitachi Hi-8 WM-H80E video cameras connected to Panasonic AG-6730 time-lapse super-VHS video recorders equipped with AG-IA670 time code generators/computer interfaces, using the 24 h time-lapse mode (which employs 1/8 normal speed). Intense light (a 100W spot and a 20W luminescent lamp placed 1/2 m above each aquarium) was used during filming, in addition to natural light from windows. Videotapes were analyzed using the Observer 3.0 VTA software, and persons scoring videos were unaware of the objective of the study. The Observer software kept track of duration and latency of behaviors with a precision of less than a second, which is an accuracy far more than enough for these slow-moving fishes.</p>
<p>Statistical probabilities reported from these experiments are two-tailed. Nonparametric tests were used whenever the assumptions of parametric tests were not met. Experiments were performed under a license from the Swedish ethical board.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>RESULTS</title>
<p>We classified the two females in a pair as “the one displaying the ornament for a longer time” and “the one displaying for a shorter time,” respectively, during day one. We found no significant differences between these two female categories in standard body length (long displaying females were on average 228 ± SD 29 mm, short displaying 221 ± 26 mm, paired
<italic>t</italic>
test,
<italic>t</italic>
<sub>19</sub>
= 1.42,
<italic>p</italic>
=.2) or contrast (mean 3.10 ± 2.00 for both groups of females, Wilcoxon matched-pairs test,
<italic>T</italic>
= 6,
<italic>n</italic>
= 20 pairs,
<italic>p</italic>
= 1). In four trials both females either constantly displayed the ornament or did not display it at all; these females were discounted. When females instead were categorized as being “chosen” or “rejected,” that is, which female the male spent most or least time with on day 2, again female lengths did not differ (chosen females 229 ± 27 mm, rejected 224 ± 26 mm,
<italic>t</italic>
<sub>23</sub>
= 1.25,
<italic>p</italic>
=.2), nor did contrast (3.08 ± 0.4 in both groups,
<italic>T</italic>
= 85,
<italic>n</italic>
= 24 pairs,
<italic>p</italic>
= 1). Thus, our experiment included closely matching pairs of females.</p>
<p>Female color did not significantly influence male choice (females broadly categorized as brown or green, 17 brown and seven green females were chosen, binomial
<italic>p</italic>
=.152) or which female displayed the ornament for the longest duration on day one (14 brown versus six green, binomial
<italic>p</italic>
=.116). Pairing was not color-assortative: green males chose two green and six brown females, and brown males chose four green and 10 brown females (Fisher's Exact test
<italic>p</italic>
= 1). Similarly, females did not display their ornament in a color-assortative fashion: green females displayed more to two green and five brown males, and brown females more to five green and six brown males (Fisher's Exact test
<italic>p</italic>
=.6).</p>
<p>Females displaying the ornament for a longer time on day one were preferred by males on the second day, compared to females displaying for a shorter time (
<xref rid="FIG2">Figure 2</xref>
). Preference is here estimated as the total time a male spent in front of a female, but if instead the dancing duration with either female on day two is used as a choice index the same result emerges (males danced with more ornamented females for 15.9 ± 15.4 min and with less ornamented for 6.2 ± 9.8 min;
<italic>T</italic>
= 26,
<italic>n</italic>
= 20 pairs,
<italic>p</italic>
<.01). No choice for any particular female was evident on day one (
<italic>p</italic>
>.3 for dancing duration), but on that day dances were both infrequent and of short durations. Females displaying their ornaments for longer on day two were, however, not preferred by males as compared to females displaying for shorter on day two (291.4 ± 144.0 versus 306.0 ± 143.4 min total time before the respective females;
<italic>T</italic>
= 104,
<italic>n</italic>
= 20 pairs,
<italic>p</italic>
= 1). If dancing duration day two is used instead of total time as a choice index, the same result emerges (
<italic>p</italic>
=.5).</p>
<p>
<fig id="FIG2" position="float">
<label>
<bold>Figure 2</bold>
</label>
<caption>
<p>The female displaying the ornament for longer on day one (with female-female competition) was chosen by the male on day two (with females separated), as compared to the female displaying for shorter (Wilcoxon matched-pairs test,
<italic>T</italic>
= 32,
<italic>p</italic>
<.01,
<italic>n</italic>
= 24). Mate choice is estimated in terms of the total time spent by a male in front of either female on day two. Black squares are medians, boxes quartiles and bars represent minimum and maximum values. Inserts show mate choice aquaria from above on day one and two.</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="bhec-12-04-06-f02"></graphic>
</fig>
</p>
<p>Similarly, females chosen on day two had displayed their ornaments for longer on day one (chosen females for 233.2 ± 176.7 min, rejected for 130.3 ± 151.5 min;
<italic>T</italic>
= 40,
<italic>n</italic>
= 24 pairs,
<italic>p</italic>
=.015) but not on day two (123.7 ± 171.8 and 99.7 ± 128.4 min, respectively;
<italic>T</italic>
= 85,
<italic>n</italic>
= 24 pairs,
<italic>p</italic>
=.5).</p>
<p>On day one, females could either display the ornament to each other, in which case the duration of ornament display would correlate with the duration of competitive interactions, or females could display the ornament to males, in which case ornament display duration would correlate with the duration of intersexual interactions (i.e., dancing duration). It turned out that ornament display correlated significantly with competition duration, but not with dancing duration (
<xref rid="FIG3">Figure 3</xref>
), evidently because most females were busy competing and did not dance at all. As the competition duration by necessity was the same for both females in a pair, competition duration was regressed on the average ornamentation duration for each female pair. Similarly, the average dancing duration of each female pair was regressed on competition duration to avoid pseudoreplication. This result also emerges from a multiple correlation (regression summary
<italic>F</italic>
<sub>2,21</sub>
= 9.61,
<italic>p</italic>
=.001) for the effects of competition and dancing duration on ornament display duration: β
<sub>competition</sub>
= 0.544 (
<italic>p</italic>
=.01) and β
<sub>dance</sub>
= 0.215 (
<italic>p</italic>
=.28). Clearly competition more accurately predicted ornamentation than did dancing, and, moreover, this predominantly competition-induced ornamentation significantly predicted male mate choice the next day.</p>
<p>
<fig id="FIG3" position="float">
<label>
<bold>Figure 3</bold>
</label>
<caption>
<p>The duration of female ornament display on day one (with female-female competition) correlated with (a) the duration of female-female competition that day (Spearman
<italic>R
<sub>s</sub>
</italic>
=.60,
<italic>n</italic>
= 24,
<italic>p</italic>
<.002, linear regression line showed in graph), but not with (b) the dancing duration with the male (Spearman
<italic>R
<sub>s</sub>
</italic>
= -.05,
<italic>n</italic>
= 24,
<italic>p</italic>
=.8). The regression in (a) is significant even if the extreme data point in the upper right corner is removed (Spearman
<italic>R
<sub>s</sub>
</italic>
=.55,
<italic>n</italic>
= 23,
<italic>p</italic>
<.01).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="bhec-12-04-06-f03"></graphic>
</fig>
</p>
<p>The more intensely females engaged in competition on day one, the less likely they were to display the ornament on day two (
<xref rid="FIG4">Figure 4</xref>
). Pseudoreplication is compensated for by using average ornament display duration within each pair on day two. Interestingly, this correlation is driven by the chosen females: when analyzed separately, competition duration on day one for chosen females correlated significantly with ornament display day two (
<italic>R
<sub>s</sub>
</italic>
= -.432,
<italic>n</italic>
= 24,
<italic>p</italic>
=.035), but not so for rejected females (
<italic>R
<sub>s</sub>
</italic>
= -.302,
<italic>n</italic>
= 24,
<italic>p</italic>
=.15).</p>
<p>
<fig id="FIG4" position="float">
<label>
<bold>Figure 4</bold>
</label>
<caption>
<p>The more time females spent competing on day one, the less they displayed their ornament on the subsequent day when female-female interactions were prevented (Spearman
<italic>R
<sub>s</sub>
</italic>
= -.42,
<italic>n</italic>
= 24,
<italic>p</italic>
<.05, linear regression line shown in graph).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="bhec-12-04-06-f04"></graphic>
</fig>
</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>DISCUSSION</title>
<p>When female-female competition was allowed, females displayed the ornament to one another more persistently than they did to the male. The female displaying most intensely is likely to be the dominant one within a pair. Clearly, a more intense ornament display (or other information from watching competitive interactions) made this female attractive to the male, as males used this information on the following day when females no longer were allowed to compete: The male spent most time before the female that had displayed for longer in the inter-female competitive situation. Indeed, this past information of ornament or competitive displays completely overrode more recently acquired information, and significantly influenced male mate choice. Furthermore, ornament display under competition did not correlate significantly with ornament display in the absence of competition. Consequently, on the day of the male's choice, ornament display contained little or no information regarding female dominance. Thus, male pipefish seem to remember and make use of information regarding partner dominance, presumably according to the reliability of that information. An alternative explanation is that males are conservative and stick to a decision once made. However, `conservatism' as a reason to why males should ignore recent display information and instead prefer older information seems unlikely. Moreover, in nature these fish do not form pair bonds or exhibit territoriality (
<xref rid="REF30">Vincent et al., 1994</xref>
), further making the “conservatism” explanation unlikely. Note that the effect of mating competition on the choosing sex is not merely one of constraining the options for choice (indirect mate choice, see
<xref rid="REF34">Wiley and Poston, 1996</xref>
) but active mate choice.</p>
<p>We ran a mirror-image experiment in 1996, with females separated the first day and competing the second (
<xref rid="REF9">Berglund and Rosenqvist, 2001</xref>
). Another difference from the experiment reported here was that males had full access to females on the second day, such that copulations were possible and occurred (this could not be allowed in the present experiment, as males would then get pregnant and lose interest in females the second day). In the mirror-image experiment, with females separated during the first day, ornament display day one significantly predicted ornament display on the second day, when female-female interactions were allowed. Ornament display on either day also significantly predicted male mate choice. This makes a possible alternative explanation to our results unlikely, namely the explanation that time itself, rather than the absence of female-female competition on day two, caused the results. Both males and females chose and behaved similarly on both days in the mirror-image experiment, so time effects, such as experience with the experimental set-up, are unlikely to have confounded the results presented here. Thus, when females were separated on the first day, their ornament display was the same on the second day with competition, but when competition preceded separation this was no longer true. Does intense competition make females less able, or less willing, to subsequently display their attractiveness? The finding that females competing more intensely on day one displayed their ornaments to a lesser extent on day two (
<xref rid="FIG4">Figure 4</xref>
) may suggest that intense competition is tiring or otherwise discourages females from performing the display on the following day. Indeed, long-term reproductive inhibition has been demonstrated in this species before: Larger, and presumably dominant, females may decrease reproductive activity in smaller females (
<xref rid="REF1">Berglund, 1991</xref>
). Alternatively, a female having proven her potential during female-female encounters may not need to display her attractiveness further, as males will rank such information more highly than pure displays of attraction. Presently, we cannot tell whether females become dispirited or arrogant after an episode of intense competition, but the finding that only chosen, not rejected, females refrained from displaying their attractiveness on day two suggests arrogance.</p>
<p>Thus, ornament display under female-female competition may be a more reliable and important signal than intersexual ornament display. Males mating with dominant females may gain direct benefits for themselves in terms of reduced risk during courtship, as well as possible direct and/or genetic benefits for their offspring from mating with high quality females. Therefore, male pipefish prefer beautifully ornamented females, but, above all, victorious ones.</p>
</sec>
</body>
<back>
<ack>
<p>The work was funded by the Swedish Natural Research Council (grant to A.B.) and by the Norwegian Research Council (grant to G.R.). We thank Klubban Biological Station and Kristineberg Marine Research Station for research facilities. Thanks to Arnt Narve Bordal, Maria Sandvik, and Hanna Schiöler-Wasslavik for field assistance and video analysis, and to Ingrid Ahnesjö, Ian Fleming, Elisabet Forsgren, Maria Sandvik, and Staffan Ulfstrand for valuable comments on an earlier draft.</p>
</ack>
<ref-list>
<ref id="REF1">
<citation>Berglund A,
<year>1991</year>
. Egg competition in a sex-role reversed pipefish: subdominant females trade reproduction for growth.
<source>Evolution</source>
<volume>45</volume>
:
<fpage>770</fpage>
-774.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF2">
<citation>Berglund A,
<year>1993</year>
. Risky sex: male pipefishes mate at random in the presence of a predator.
<source>Anim Behav</source>
<volume>46</volume>
:
<fpage>169</fpage>
-175.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF3">
<citation>Berglund A,
<year>1994</year>
. The operational sex ratio influences choosiness in a pipefish.
<source>Behav Ecol</source>
<volume>5</volume>
:
<fpage>254</fpage>
-258.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF4">
<citation>Berglund A,
<year>1999</year>
. Are female ornaments different from those of males? The pipefish evidence. In:
<source>Behaviour and conservation of littoral fishes</source>
(Almada VC, Oliveira RF, Gonçalves EJ, eds). Lisboa: ISPA;
<fpage>231</fpage>
-248.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF5">
<citation>Berglund A,
<year>2000</year>
. Sex role reversal in a pipefish: female ornaments as amplifying handicaps.
<source>Ann Zool Fenn</source>
<volume>37</volume>
:
<fpage>1</fpage>
-13.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF6">
<citation>Berglund A, Bisazza A, Pilastro A,
<year>1996</year>
. Armaments and ornaments: an evolutionary explanation of traits of dual utility.
<source>Biol J Linn Soc Lond</source>
<volume>58</volume>
:
<fpage>385</fpage>
-399.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF7">
<citation>Berglund A, Rosenqvist G,
<year>1990</year>
. Male limitation of female reproductive success in a pipefish: effects of body size differences.
<source>Behav Ecol Sociobiol</source>
<volume>27</volume>
:
<fpage>129</fpage>
-133.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF8">
<citation>Berglund A, Rosenqvist G,
<year>1993</year>
. Selective males and ardent females in pipefishes.
<source>Behav Ecol Sociobiol</source>
<volume>32</volume>
:
<fpage>331</fpage>
-336.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF9">
<citation>Berglund A, Rosenqvist G,
<year>2001</year>
. Male pipefish prefer ornamented females.
<source>Anim Behav</source>
<volume>61</volume>
:
<fpage>345</fpage>
-350.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF10">
<citation>Berglund A, Rosenqvist G, Bernet P,
<year>1997</year>
. Ornamentation predicts reproductive success in female pipefish.
<source>Behav Ecol Sociobiol</source>
<volume>40</volume>
:
<fpage>145</fpage>
-150.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF11">
<citation>Berglund A, Rosenqvist G, Svensson I,
<year>1986</year>
. Mate choice, fecundity and sexual dimorphism in two pipefish species (Syngnathidae).
<source>Behav Ecol Sociobiol</source>
<volume>19</volume>
:
<fpage>301</fpage>
-307.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF12">
<citation>Berglund A, Rosenqvist G, Svensson I,
<year>1986</year>
. Reversed sex roles and parental energy investment in zygotes of two pipefish (Syngnathidae) species.
<source>Marine Ecol Prog Ser</source>
<volume>29</volume>
:
<fpage>209</fpage>
-215.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF13">
<citation>Bernet P, Rosenqvist G, Berglund A,
<year>1998</year>
. Female-female competition affects female ornamentation in the sex-role reversed pipefish
<italic>Syngnathus typhle</italic>
.
<source>Behaviour</source>
<volume>135</volume>
:
<fpage>535</fpage>
-550.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF14">
<citation>Bisazza A, Marconato A, Marin G,
<year>1989</year>
. Male competition and female choice in
<italic>Padogobius martensi</italic>
(Pisces, Gobiidae).
<source>Anim Behav</source>
<volume>38</volume>
:
<fpage>406</fpage>
-413.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF15">
<citation>Bisazza A, Marconato A, Marin G,
<year>1989</year>
. Male mate preference in the mosquitofish
<italic>Gambusia holbrooki</italic>
.
<source>Ethology</source>
<volume>83</volume>
:
<fpage>335</fpage>
-343.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF16">
<citation>Borgia G,
<year>1981</year>
. Mate selection in the fly
<italic>Scatophaga stercoraria</italic>
: female choice in a male controlled system.
<source>Anim Behav</source>
<volume>29</volume>
:
<fpage>71</fpage>
-80.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF17">
<citation>Cox CR, LeBoeuf BJ,
<year>1977</year>
. Female incitation of male competition: a mechanism in sexual selection.
<source>Am Nat</source>
<volume>111</volume>
:
<fpage>317</fpage>
-335.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF18">
<citation>Farr JA, Travis J,
<year>1986</year>
. Fertility advertisement by female sailfin mollies,
<italic>Poecilia latipinna</italic>
(Pisces: Poeciliidae).
<source>Copeia</source>
<volume>1986</volume>
:
<fpage>467</fpage>
-472.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF19">
<citation>Fiedler K,
<year>1954</year>
. Vergleichende verhaltensstudien an seenadeln, schlangennadeln und seepferdchen (Syngnathidae).
<source>Z Tierpsychol</source>
<volume>11</volume>
:
<fpage>358</fpage>
-416.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF20">
<citation>Forsgren E,
<year>1997</year>
. Female sand gobies prefer good fathers over dominant males.
<source>Proc R Soc Lond B</source>
<volume>264</volume>
:
<fpage>1283</fpage>
-1286.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF21">
<citation>Fuller R, Berglund A,
<year>1996</year>
. Behavioral responses of a sex-role reversed pipefish to a gradient of perceived predation risk.
<source>Behav Ecol</source>
<volume>7</volume>
:
<fpage>69</fpage>
-75.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF22">
<citation>Jones AG, Rosenqvist G, Berglund A, Avise JC,
<year>1999</year>
. The genetic mating system of a sex-role-reversed pipefish (
<italic>Syngnathus typhle</italic>
): a molecular inquiry.
<source>Behav Ecol Sociobiol</source>
<volume>46</volume>
:
<fpage>357</fpage>
-365.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF23">
<citation>Kokko H,
<year>1997</year>
. Evolutionarily stable strategies of age-dependent sexual advertisement.
<source>Behav Ecol Sociobiol</source>
<volume>41</volume>
:
<fpage>99</fpage>
-107.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF24">
<citation>Mateos C, Carranza J,
<year>1997</year>
. Signals in intra-sexual competition between ring-necked pheasant males.
<source>Anim Behav</source>
<volume>53</volume>
:
<fpage>471</fpage>
-485.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF25">
<citation>Mateos C, Carranza J,
<year>1999</year>
. Effects of male dominance and courtship display on female choice in the ring-necked pheasant.
<source>Behav Ecol Sociobiol</source>
<volume>45</volume>
:
<fpage>235</fpage>
-244.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF26">
<citation>Moore AJ, Moore PJ,
<year>1999</year>
. Balancing sexual selection through opposing mate choice and male competition.
<source>Proc R Soc Edinb B</source>
<volume>266</volume>
:
<fpage>711</fpage>
-716.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF27">
<citation>Qvarnström A, Forsgren E,
<year>1998</year>
. Should females prefer dominant males?
<source>Trends Ecol Evol</source>
<volume>13</volume>
:
<fpage>498</fpage>
-501.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF28">
<citation>Thornhill R,
<year>1988</year>
. The jungle fowl hen's cackle incites male competition.
<source>Verh Deutschen Zool Ges</source>
<volume>81</volume>
:
<fpage>145</fpage>
-154.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF29">
<citation>Viljugrein H,
<year>1997</year>
. The cost of dishonesty.
<source>Proc R Soc Lond B</source>
<volume>264</volume>
:
<fpage>815</fpage>
-821.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF30">
<citation>Vincent A, Ahnesjö I, Berglund A,
<year>1994</year>
. Operational sex ratios and behavioural sex differences in a pipefish population.
<source>Behav Ecol Sociobiol</source>
<volume>34</volume>
:
<fpage>435</fpage>
-442.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF31">
<citation>Vincent A, Berglund A, Ahnesjö I,
<year>1995</year>
. Reproductive ecology of five pipefish species in one eelgrass meadow.
<source>Environ Biol Fish</source>
<volume>44</volume>
:
<fpage>347</fpage>
-361.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF32">
<citation>West MJ, King AP,
<year>1980</year>
. Enriching cowbird song by social deprivation.
<source>J Comp Physiol Psychol</source>
<volume>94</volume>
:
<fpage>263</fpage>
-270.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF33">
<citation>West MJ, King AP, Eastzer DH,
<year>1981</year>
. Validating the female bioassay of cowbird song: relating differences in song potency to mating success.
<source>Anim Behav</source>
<volume>29</volume>
:
<fpage>490</fpage>
-501.</citation>
</ref>
<ref id="REF34">
<citation>Wiley RH, Poston J,
<year>1996</year>
. Indirect mate choice, competition for mates, and coevolution of the sexes.
<source>Evolution</source>
<volume>50</volume>
:
<fpage>1371</fpage>
-1381.</citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</article>
</istex:document>
</istex:metadataXml>
<mods version="3.6">
<titleInfo lang="en">
<title>Male pipefish prefer dominant over attractive females</title>
</titleInfo>
<titleInfo type="alternative" lang="en" contentType="CDATA">
<title>Male pipefish prefer dominant over attractive females</title>
</titleInfo>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Anders</namePart>
<namePart type="family">Berglund</namePart>
<affiliation>Department of Animal Ecology, Uppsala University, Norbyvägen 18d, S-752 36 Uppsala, Sweden</affiliation>
<role>
<roleTerm type="text">author</roleTerm>
</role>
</name>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Gunilla</namePart>
<namePart type="family">Rosenqvist</namePart>
<affiliation>Department of Zoology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, N-7491 Trondheim, Norway</affiliation>
<role>
<roleTerm type="text">author</roleTerm>
</role>
</name>
<typeOfResource>text</typeOfResource>
<genre type="research-article" displayLabel="research-article" authority="ISTEX" authorityURI="https://content-type.data.istex.fr" valueURI="https://content-type.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/XTP-1JC4F85T-7">research-article</genre>
<originInfo>
<publisher>Oxford University Press</publisher>
<dateIssued encoding="w3cdtf">2001-07</dateIssued>
<copyrightDate encoding="w3cdtf">2001</copyrightDate>
</originInfo>
<language>
<languageTerm type="code" authority="iso639-2b">eng</languageTerm>
<languageTerm type="code" authority="rfc3066">en</languageTerm>
</language>
<abstract lang="en">Animals may obtain information guiding their choice between potential partners from observing competitive interactions and displays between them, or from displays directed at the choosing individual. In the sex-role reversed pipefish Syngnathus typhle females display a temporary ornament (a color pattern) to other females as well as to males. We have previously shown that display of female ornaments per se is attractive to males. Here we show that information from competitive displays can override such direct attraction displays as signals in the partner choice process. In a mate choice experiment, an enclosed male could choose between two females. On the first experimental day, females could interact freely, while on the second day they were isolated from each other. When female-female competition was allowed, the ornament display was directed more to the other female than to the male: Time competing, rather than time courting the male, correlated with ornament display duration. However, ornament display under competition and ornament display in the absence of competition did not correlate significantly. In fact, females competing more intensively on day one displayed the ornament less on day two. Furthermore, the ornament display during the first, but not the second, day predicted male mate choice on the second day. Thus, males remembered previous information from competitive displays and used it rather than immediate information from displays in the absence of female-female competition. We suggest that competitive displays more reliably signal female quality as compared to noncompetitive ones, and that males benefit from mating with dominant females.</abstract>
<note type="author-notes">Address correspondence to A. Berglund. E-mail: anders.berglund@ebc.uu.se .</note>
<subject lang="en">
<genre>KWD</genre>
<topic>mate choice</topic>
<topic>mate competition</topic>
<topic>ornament</topic>
<topic>pipefish</topic>
<topic>sexual selection</topic>
<topic>signal honesty</topic>
<topic>Syngnathus typhle</topic>
</subject>
<relatedItem type="host">
<titleInfo>
<title>Behavioral Ecology</title>
</titleInfo>
<titleInfo type="abbreviated">
<title>Behavioral Ecology</title>
</titleInfo>
<genre type="journal" authority="ISTEX" authorityURI="https://publication-type.data.istex.fr" valueURI="https://publication-type.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/JMC-0GLKJH51-B">journal</genre>
<identifier type="ISSN">1045-2249</identifier>
<identifier type="eISSN">1465-7279</identifier>
<identifier type="PublisherID">beheco</identifier>
<identifier type="PublisherID-hwp">beheco</identifier>
<identifier type="PublisherID-nlm-ta">Behav Ecol</identifier>
<part>
<date>2001</date>
<detail type="volume">
<caption>vol.</caption>
<number>12</number>
</detail>
<detail type="issue">
<caption>no.</caption>
<number>4</number>
</detail>
<extent unit="pages">
<start>402</start>
<end>406</end>
</extent>
</part>
</relatedItem>
<identifier type="istex">848CE6AC64DDA7ADE118030ED403544E3F7F7F6C</identifier>
<identifier type="DOI">10.1093/beheco/12.4.402</identifier>
<identifier type="PII">1465-7279</identifier>
<identifier type="local">0120402</identifier>
<accessCondition type="use and reproduction" contentType="copyright">International Society of Behavioral Ecology</accessCondition>
<recordInfo>
<recordContentSource authority="ISTEX" authorityURI="https://loaded-corpus.data.istex.fr" valueURI="https://loaded-corpus.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/XBH-GTWS0RDP-M">oup</recordContentSource>
<recordOrigin>International Society of Behavioral Ecology</recordOrigin>
</recordInfo>
</mods>
<json:item>
<extension>json</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>application/json</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/848CE6AC64DDA7ADE118030ED403544E3F7F7F6C/metadata/json</uri>
</json:item>
</metadata>
<serie></serie>
</istex>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Wicri/Santé/explor/EdenteV2/Data/Istex/Corpus
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 004182 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Istex/Corpus/biblio.hfd -nk 004182 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Wicri/Santé
   |area=    EdenteV2
   |flux=    Istex
   |étape=   Corpus
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     ISTEX:848CE6AC64DDA7ADE118030ED403544E3F7F7F6C
   |texte=   Male pipefish prefer dominant over attractive females
}}

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.32.
Data generation: Thu Nov 30 15:26:48 2017. Site generation: Tue Mar 8 16:36:20 2022