Serveur d'exploration Hippolyte Bernheim

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories: A Psychosocial Genomic Approach to Reconstituting Social Memory during Religious Rituals

Identifieur interne : 000889 ( Istex/Corpus ); précédent : 000888; suivant : 000890

Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories: A Psychosocial Genomic Approach to Reconstituting Social Memory during Religious Rituals

Auteurs : Sidney M. Greenfield

Source :

RBID : ISTEX:ABD10C82C70746199D2FCB093F471336E1921078

English descriptors

Abstract

This article explores the relationship between efforts by members of diverse social groups to regain, reconstruct or remember disappearing cultural pasts - in which life was believed to have been better and more meaningful - and the distinctive forms taken in the ritual and other practices of so many of today’s religions. The author proposes that religious rituals that enable participants to enter into trance, or other altered states of consciousness (ASC), contribute positively to the reconstruction of social memories and make it possible for seemingly forgotten beliefs and practices to be retrieved. A combination of recent developments in research, theory and conjecture in neuroscience and older theory, data and speculation in anthropology are employed to explicate this idea. The analysis focuses on the (individual human) body within which social practice (or culture) and biophysiological processes converge. For so many in the contemporary world who see their lives as shattered by modernity’s secularizing and dehumanizing forces, what is taking place as they strive to reconstruct their social pasts is a cultural-biological response manifesting itself in a range of social practices in which religion is pre-eminent.

Url:
DOI: 10.1177/0011392105049540

Links to Exploration step

ISTEX:ABD10C82C70746199D2FCB093F471336E1921078

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI wicri:istexFullTextTei="biblStruct">
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories: A Psychosocial Genomic Approach to Reconstituting Social Memory during Religious Rituals</title>
<author wicri:is="90%">
<name sortKey="Greenfield, Sidney M" sort="Greenfield, Sidney M" uniqKey="Greenfield S" first="Sidney M." last="Greenfield">Sidney M. Greenfield</name>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">ISTEX</idno>
<idno type="RBID">ISTEX:ABD10C82C70746199D2FCB093F471336E1921078</idno>
<date when="2005" year="2005">2005</date>
<idno type="doi">10.1177/0011392105049540</idno>
<idno type="url">https://api.istex.fr/document/ABD10C82C70746199D2FCB093F471336E1921078/fulltext/pdf</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Istex/Corpus">000889</idno>
<idno type="wicri:explorRef" wicri:stream="Istex" wicri:step="Corpus" wicri:corpus="ISTEX">000889</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories: A Psychosocial Genomic Approach to Reconstituting Social Memory during Religious Rituals</title>
<author wicri:is="90%">
<name sortKey="Greenfield, Sidney M" sort="Greenfield, Sidney M" uniqKey="Greenfield S" first="Sidney M." last="Greenfield">Sidney M. Greenfield</name>
</author>
</analytic>
<monogr></monogr>
<series>
<title level="j">Current sociology</title>
<idno type="ISSN">0011-3921</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1461-7064</idno>
<imprint>
<publisher>Sage Publications</publisher>
<pubPlace>Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</pubPlace>
<date type="published" when="2005-03">2005-03</date>
<biblScope unit="volume">53</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="issue">2</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" from="275">275</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" to="291">291</biblScope>
</imprint>
<idno type="ISSN">0011-3921</idno>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
<seriesStmt>
<idno type="ISSN">0011-3921</idno>
</seriesStmt>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass>
<keywords scheme="Teeft" xml:lang="en">
<term>Accessing</term>
<term>American journal</term>
<term>Basic books</term>
<term>Basic cycle</term>
<term>Baywood publishing</term>
<term>Cambridge university press</term>
<term>Chicago press</term>
<term>Clinical hypnosis</term>
<term>Clock genes</term>
<term>Conscious memory</term>
<term>Conscious memory aspects</term>
<term>Cultural beliefs</term>
<term>Cultural practices</term>
<term>Current sociology</term>
<term>Gene expression</term>
<term>Greenfield</term>
<term>Hypnosis</term>
<term>Language acquisition</term>
<term>Large numbers</term>
<term>Local level</term>
<term>Long periods</term>
<term>Modernization theory</term>
<term>Monograph</term>
<term>Monotheistic faiths</term>
<term>Neuron</term>
<term>Next christendom</term>
<term>Original acquisition</term>
<term>Other capabilities</term>
<term>Other parts</term>
<term>Practical problems</term>
<term>Primitive culture</term>
<term>Psychobiological condition</term>
<term>Religious practices</term>
<term>Religious rituals</term>
<term>Rossi</term>
<term>Second half</term>
<term>Second world</term>
<term>Similar situation</term>
<term>Social groups</term>
<term>Social memory</term>
<term>Social pasts</term>
<term>State dependently</term>
<term>Synapse</term>
<term>Synaptic connections</term>
<term>Trance</term>
<term>Trance states</term>
<term>Wave nature</term>
</keywords>
</textClass>
<langUsage>
<language ident="en">en</language>
</langUsage>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">This article explores the relationship between efforts by members of diverse social groups to regain, reconstruct or remember disappearing cultural pasts - in which life was believed to have been better and more meaningful - and the distinctive forms taken in the ritual and other practices of so many of today’s religions. The author proposes that religious rituals that enable participants to enter into trance, or other altered states of consciousness (ASC), contribute positively to the reconstruction of social memories and make it possible for seemingly forgotten beliefs and practices to be retrieved. A combination of recent developments in research, theory and conjecture in neuroscience and older theory, data and speculation in anthropology are employed to explicate this idea. The analysis focuses on the (individual human) body within which social practice (or culture) and biophysiological processes converge. For so many in the contemporary world who see their lives as shattered by modernity’s secularizing and dehumanizing forces, what is taking place as they strive to reconstruct their social pasts is a cultural-biological response manifesting itself in a range of social practices in which religion is pre-eminent.</div>
</front>
</TEI>
<istex>
<corpusName>sage</corpusName>
<keywords>
<teeft>
<json:string>trance states</json:string>
<json:string>synapse</json:string>
<json:string>accessing</json:string>
<json:string>hypnosis</json:string>
<json:string>current sociology</json:string>
<json:string>rossi</json:string>
<json:string>greenfield</json:string>
<json:string>practical problems</json:string>
<json:string>monograph</json:string>
<json:string>neuron</json:string>
<json:string>state dependently</json:string>
<json:string>modernization theory</json:string>
<json:string>cultural beliefs</json:string>
<json:string>social pasts</json:string>
<json:string>monotheistic faiths</json:string>
<json:string>basic cycle</json:string>
<json:string>american journal</json:string>
<json:string>clinical hypnosis</json:string>
<json:string>trance</json:string>
<json:string>other capabilities</json:string>
<json:string>similar situation</json:string>
<json:string>second half</json:string>
<json:string>next christendom</json:string>
<json:string>local level</json:string>
<json:string>other parts</json:string>
<json:string>synaptic connections</json:string>
<json:string>religious practices</json:string>
<json:string>conscious memory</json:string>
<json:string>clock genes</json:string>
<json:string>social groups</json:string>
<json:string>psychobiological condition</json:string>
<json:string>language acquisition</json:string>
<json:string>large numbers</json:string>
<json:string>second world</json:string>
<json:string>gene expression</json:string>
<json:string>original acquisition</json:string>
<json:string>cultural practices</json:string>
<json:string>long periods</json:string>
<json:string>wave nature</json:string>
<json:string>conscious memory aspects</json:string>
<json:string>baywood publishing</json:string>
<json:string>religious rituals</json:string>
<json:string>cambridge university press</json:string>
<json:string>basic books</json:string>
<json:string>social memory</json:string>
<json:string>chicago press</json:string>
<json:string>primitive culture</json:string>
</teeft>
</keywords>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>Sidney M. Greenfield</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<subject>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>altered states of consciousness (ASC)</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>BRAC (one basic rest-activity cycle)</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>ritual</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>social memory</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>trance</value>
</json:item>
</subject>
<articleId>
<json:string>10.1177_0011392105049540</json:string>
</articleId>
<arkIstex>ark:/67375/M70-C4VG936P-F</arkIstex>
<language>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</language>
<originalGenre>
<json:string>research-article</json:string>
</originalGenre>
<abstract>This article explores the relationship between efforts by members of diverse social groups to regain, reconstruct or remember disappearing cultural pasts - in which life was believed to have been better and more meaningful - and the distinctive forms taken in the ritual and other practices of so many of today’s religions. The author proposes that religious rituals that enable participants to enter into trance, or other altered states of consciousness (ASC), contribute positively to the reconstruction of social memories and make it possible for seemingly forgotten beliefs and practices to be retrieved. A combination of recent developments in research, theory and conjecture in neuroscience and older theory, data and speculation in anthropology are employed to explicate this idea. The analysis focuses on the (individual human) body within which social practice (or culture) and biophysiological processes converge. For so many in the contemporary world who see their lives as shattered by modernity’s secularizing and dehumanizing forces, what is taking place as they strive to reconstruct their social pasts is a cultural-biological response manifesting itself in a range of social practices in which religion is pre-eminent.</abstract>
<qualityIndicators>
<score>9.22</score>
<pdfWordCount>7054</pdfWordCount>
<pdfCharCount>42590</pdfCharCount>
<pdfVersion>1.5</pdfVersion>
<pdfPageCount>17</pdfPageCount>
<pdfPageSize>441.929 x 663.417 pts</pdfPageSize>
<refBibsNative>true</refBibsNative>
<abstractWordCount>185</abstractWordCount>
<abstractCharCount>1235</abstractCharCount>
<keywordCount>5</keywordCount>
</qualityIndicators>
<title>Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories: A Psychosocial Genomic Approach to Reconstituting Social Memory during Religious Rituals</title>
<genre>
<json:string>research-article</json:string>
</genre>
<host>
<title>Current sociology</title>
<language>
<json:string>unknown</json:string>
</language>
<issn>
<json:string>0011-3921</json:string>
</issn>
<eissn>
<json:string>1461-7064</json:string>
</eissn>
<publisherId>
<json:string>CSI</json:string>
</publisherId>
<volume>53</volume>
<issue>2</issue>
<pages>
<first>275</first>
<last>291</last>
</pages>
<genre>
<json:string>journal</json:string>
</genre>
</host>
<namedEntities>
<unitex>
<date>
<json:string>2005</json:string>
<json:string>3/2/05</json:string>
<json:string>the 20th century</json:string>
</date>
<geogName></geogName>
<orgName>
<json:string>Mexico, Guatemala, El Salvador, Peru, Columbia and Chile</json:string>
</orgName>
<orgName_funder></orgName_funder>
<orgName_provider></orgName_provider>
<persName>
<json:string>Louis Bunuel</json:string>
<json:string>Marquis de Puységur</json:string>
<json:string>Franz Anton</json:string>
<json:string>Al Qaeda</json:string>
<json:string>SAGE Publications</json:string>
<json:string>Milton H. Erickson</json:string>
<json:string>Having</json:string>
</persName>
<placeName>
<json:string>New Delhi</json:string>
<json:string>Brazil</json:string>
<json:string>US</json:string>
<json:string>Europe</json:string>
<json:string>America</json:string>
</placeName>
<ref_url></ref_url>
<ref_bibl>
<json:string>Rossi and Cheek (1988: 107)</json:string>
<json:string>Moore and Kaplan, 1983</json:string>
<json:string>LeDoux, 2002: 98</json:string>
<json:string>Ley and Freeman, 1984</json:string>
<json:string>Rossi, 2002: 67</json:string>
<json:string>Kandel, 1999: 508</json:string>
<json:string>LeDoux, 2002: 97</json:string>
<json:string>Edelman and Tononi, 2000: 187</json:string>
<json:string>Ruzyla-Smith et al., 1995</json:string>
<json:string>Milner et al., 1998</json:string>
<json:string>Wolf (1974: 33)</json:string>
<json:string>Gauld, 1992</json:string>
<json:string>Rossi, 1996: 120</json:string>
<json:string>1993, 1996, 2002</json:string>
<json:string>Crick, 1957</json:string>
<json:string>Rossi, 1996: 130</json:string>
<json:string>1963, 1969</json:string>
<json:string>1987; cited in LeDoux, 2002: 73</json:string>
<json:string>Rossi, 1996: 129</json:string>
<json:string>Wolf, 1974: 33</json:string>
<json:string>see Kroeber, 1948: 2</json:string>
<json:string>LeDoux, 2002: 49</json:string>
<json:string>Rossi, 2002: 69</json:string>
</ref_bibl>
<bibl></bibl>
</unitex>
</namedEntities>
<ark>
<json:string>ark:/67375/M70-C4VG936P-F</json:string>
</ark>
<categories>
<wos>
<json:string>1 - social science</json:string>
<json:string>2 - sociology</json:string>
</wos>
<scienceMetrix>
<json:string>1 - economic & social sciences</json:string>
<json:string>2 - social sciences</json:string>
<json:string>3 - sociology</json:string>
</scienceMetrix>
<scopus>
<json:string>1 - Social Sciences</json:string>
<json:string>2 - Social Sciences</json:string>
<json:string>3 - Sociology and Political Science</json:string>
</scopus>
<inist>
<json:string>1 - sciences humaines et sociales</json:string>
</inist>
</categories>
<publicationDate>2005</publicationDate>
<copyrightDate>2005</copyrightDate>
<doi>
<json:string>10.1177/0011392105049540</json:string>
</doi>
<id>ABD10C82C70746199D2FCB093F471336E1921078</id>
<score>1</score>
<fulltext>
<json:item>
<extension>pdf</extension>
<original>true</original>
<mimetype>application/pdf</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/ABD10C82C70746199D2FCB093F471336E1921078/fulltext/pdf</uri>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<extension>zip</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>application/zip</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/ABD10C82C70746199D2FCB093F471336E1921078/fulltext/zip</uri>
</json:item>
<istex:fulltextTEI uri="https://api.istex.fr/document/ABD10C82C70746199D2FCB093F471336E1921078/fulltext/tei">
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories: A Psychosocial Genomic Approach to Reconstituting Social Memory during Religious Rituals</title>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<authority>ISTEX</authority>
<publisher scheme="https://publisher-list.data.istex.fr">Sage Publications</publisher>
<pubPlace>Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</pubPlace>
<availability>
<licence>
<p>sage</p>
</licence>
</availability>
<p scheme="https://loaded-corpus.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/XBH-0J1N7DQT-B"></p>
<date>2005</date>
</publicationStmt>
<notesStmt>
<note type="research-article" scheme="https://content-type.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/XTP-1JC4F85T-7">research-article</note>
<note type="journal" scheme="https://publication-type.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/JMC-0GLKJH51-B">journal</note>
</notesStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct type="inbook">
<analytic>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories: A Psychosocial Genomic Approach to Reconstituting Social Memory during Religious Rituals</title>
<author xml:id="author-0000">
<persName>
<forename type="first">Sidney M.</forename>
<surname>Greenfield</surname>
</persName>
</author>
<idno type="istex">ABD10C82C70746199D2FCB093F471336E1921078</idno>
<idno type="ark">ark:/67375/M70-C4VG936P-F</idno>
<idno type="DOI">10.1177/0011392105049540</idno>
<idno type="article-id">10.1177_0011392105049540</idno>
</analytic>
<monogr>
<title level="j">Current sociology</title>
<idno type="pISSN">0011-3921</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1461-7064</idno>
<idno type="publisher-id">CSI</idno>
<idno type="PublisherID-hwp">spcsi</idno>
<imprint>
<publisher>Sage Publications</publisher>
<pubPlace>Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</pubPlace>
<date type="published" when="2005-03"></date>
<biblScope unit="volume">53</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="issue">2</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" from="275">275</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" to="291">291</biblScope>
</imprint>
</monogr>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<creation>
<date>2005</date>
</creation>
<langUsage>
<language ident="en">en</language>
</langUsage>
<abstract xml:lang="en">
<p>This article explores the relationship between efforts by members of diverse social groups to regain, reconstruct or remember disappearing cultural pasts - in which life was believed to have been better and more meaningful - and the distinctive forms taken in the ritual and other practices of so many of today’s religions. The author proposes that religious rituals that enable participants to enter into trance, or other altered states of consciousness (ASC), contribute positively to the reconstruction of social memories and make it possible for seemingly forgotten beliefs and practices to be retrieved. A combination of recent developments in research, theory and conjecture in neuroscience and older theory, data and speculation in anthropology are employed to explicate this idea. The analysis focuses on the (individual human) body within which social practice (or culture) and biophysiological processes converge. For so many in the contemporary world who see their lives as shattered by modernity’s secularizing and dehumanizing forces, what is taking place as they strive to reconstruct their social pasts is a cultural-biological response manifesting itself in a range of social practices in which religion is pre-eminent.</p>
</abstract>
<textClass>
<keywords scheme="keyword">
<list>
<head>keywords</head>
<item>
<term>altered states of consciousness (ASC)</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>BRAC (one basic rest-activity cycle)</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>ritual</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>social memory</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>trance</term>
</item>
</list>
</keywords>
</textClass>
</profileDesc>
<revisionDesc>
<change when="2005-03">Published</change>
</revisionDesc>
</teiHeader>
</istex:fulltextTEI>
<json:item>
<extension>txt</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>text/plain</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/ABD10C82C70746199D2FCB093F471336E1921078/fulltext/txt</uri>
</json:item>
</fulltext>
<metadata>
<istex:metadataXml wicri:clean="corpus sage not found" wicri:toSee="no header">
<istex:xmlDeclaration>version="1.0" encoding="UTF-8"</istex:xmlDeclaration>
<istex:docType PUBLIC="-//NLM//DTD Journal Publishing DTD v2.3 20070202//EN" URI="journalpublishing.dtd" name="istex:docType"></istex:docType>
<istex:document>
<article article-type="research-article" dtd-version="2.3" xml:lang="EN">
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="hwp">spcsi</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="publisher-id">CSI</journal-id>
<journal-title>Current Sociology</journal-title>
<issn pub-type="ppub">0011-3921</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Sage Publications</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</publisher-loc>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0011392105049540</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="publisher-id">10.1177_0011392105049540</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Articles</subject>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories: A Psychosocial Genomic Approach to Reconstituting Social Memory during Religious Rituals</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Greenfield</surname>
<given-names>Sidney M.</given-names>
</name>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<pub-date pub-type="ppub">
<month>03</month>
<year>2005</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>53</volume>
<issue>2</issue>
<fpage>275</fpage>
<lpage>291</lpage>
<abstract>
<p>This article explores the relationship between efforts by members of diverse social groups to regain, reconstruct or remember disappearing cultural pasts - in which life was believed to have been better and more meaningful - and the distinctive forms taken in the ritual and other practices of so many of today’s religions. The author proposes that religious rituals that enable participants to enter into trance, or other altered states of consciousness (ASC), contribute positively to the reconstruction of social memories and make it possible for seemingly forgotten beliefs and practices to be retrieved. A combination of recent developments in research, theory and conjecture in neuroscience and older theory, data and speculation in anthropology are employed to explicate this idea. The analysis focuses on the (individual human) body within which social practice (or culture) and biophysiological processes converge. For so many in the contemporary world who see their lives as shattered by modernity’s secularizing and dehumanizing forces, what is taking place as they strive to reconstruct their social pasts is a cultural-biological response manifesting itself in a range of social practices in which religion is pre-eminent.</p>
</abstract>
<kwd-group>
<kwd>altered states of consciousness (ASC)</kwd>
<kwd>BRAC (one basic rest-activity cycle)</kwd>
<kwd>ritual</kwd>
<kwd>social memory</kwd>
<kwd>trance</kwd>
</kwd-group>
<custom-meta-wrap>
<custom-meta xlink:type="simple">
<meta-name>sagemeta-type</meta-name>
<meta-value>Journal Article</meta-value>
</custom-meta>
<custom-meta xlink:type="simple">
<meta-name>search-text</meta-name>
<meta-value> Sidney M. Greenfield Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories: A Psychosocial Genomic Approach to Reconstituting Social Memory during Religious Rituals You have to begin to lose your memory, if only in bits and pieces, to realize that memory is what makes our lives. Life without memory is no life at all. . . . Our memory is our coherence, our reason, our feeling, even our action. Without it, we are nothing. (Louis Bunuel, quoted in LeDoux, 2002: 97) Introduction While the tragic events of 11 September 2001 marked a new phase in the relationship between the US and the rest of the world, within the social sciences a death knell was signaled for its principal paradigm. For more than half a century, modernization theory, including its Marxist variants, had been the only framework on which both policy analysts and scholars agreed.1 A central premise was that with the passage of time, the diverse peoples and societies of the world, no matter how large, small, or isolated they might be, would be absorbed into the world market and become undifferentiated parts of one or another contemporary national state. On being incorporated, they were expected to behave more like the peoples of the West than their own parents and ancestors, and like those westerners, they would become more secular and rational, while their religions,2 along with other symbolic aspects of their 'traditional' lives, would wither in importance, if not disappear alto- gether. The attacks by Al Qaeda on New York and Washington, in the name of a fundamentalist Islam, brought home to the general public and the social science community the reality that religion, in its many diverse forms, Current Sociology, March 2005, Vol. 53(2): 275­291 SAGE Publications (London, Thousand Oaks, CA and New Delhi) www.sagepublications.com DOI: 10.1177/0011392105049540 certainly had not disappeared. In spite of long periods of urbanization, participation in the world economy, becoming members of independent nations and 'modernizing' in other ways, it still provided meaningful, albeit differing, views of the world and motivation for countless numbers of indi- viduals.3 Clearly, the practitioners of purportedly disappearing cultural practices such as followers of traditional religions are changing, but their lives, at least what generally is understood about them, are more a caricature of the West than the Xeroxed copy predicted in writings on modernization. Words such as 'impoverished', 'uneducated', or 'ignorant' convey a negative impression and the opposite of what a western reader might know and understand about this large and continuously growing segment of humanity. What in the name of a quest for identity seems to drive so much of the conscious activities of this highly diverse population might be better understood as a desire by members of each group to reconstruct the memory of a disappearing social past in which life was believed to have been better, where there were greater opportunities for individual fulfillment and more meaning. This article explores the relationship between these efforts by social groups to regain (or remember) their cultural pasts and the distinctive forms taken in the ritual and other practices of so many of today's religions. Specifically, I propose that religious rituals that enable participants to enter into trance, or other altered states of consciousness (ASC), contribute posi- tively to the reconstruction of social memories and make it possible for seem- ingly forgotten cultural beliefs and practices to be retrieved. A combination of recent developments in research, theory and conjecture in neuroscience and older theory, data and speculation in anthropology is employed to examine this idea. The analysis focuses on the (individual human) body within which social practice (or culture) and biophysiological processes converge. I suggest that for so many in the contemporary world who see their lives as shattered by modernity's secularizing and dehumanizing forces, what is taking place as they strive to reconstruct their social pasts is a cultural- biological response manifesting itself in a range of social practices in which religion is pre-eminent. Religious participation is increasing worldwide and its fastest develop- ing forms embrace both leaders and followers entering into trance states. While in these altered states individuals more easily access (remember or reconstruct) memories of seemingly lost cultural beliefs and actions, which may be retained in memory and later accessed even when other beliefs and behaviors seem to have replaced them. The practices and understandings concerned are those anthropologists call 'culture'. For the most part, they are acquired informally and not necessarily available to conscious memory, especially after other, often contradictory, ideas and activities are learned. Participation in religious 276 Current Sociology Vol. 53 No. 2 Monograph 1 rituals that are open to and embrace trance states that last at least one-and- a-half to two hours (one basic rest­activity cycle [BRAC]) facilitates access- ing these cultural memories. When groups of people with shared social backgrounds enter religiously induced ASC, they are able to reinforce the fragments that each is able to access individually, and collectively reconsti- tute a larger picture that may be thought of as an unintended goal of the religious involvement. Religious Practices and Social Memory Since scholars and intellectuals in the West contribute significantly to shaping debate on public policy and influence decision-makers, it is unfortunate, though understandable, that they have not reported or examined the continu- ing role played by religion in the lives of the peoples in what in the era of the Cold War came to be known as the 'third world'. As the United States and the Soviet Union, both vast territorial states located in the northern hemi- sphere, vied for world dominance in the second half of the 20th century, their opposing economic and organizational agendas and materialistic belief systems were imposed on, and in many respects, mostly superficially at the level of government officialdom, adopted by the postcolonial and other new nations located predominantly in the South. Focusing primarily on the pros and cons of affiliation with the West or its eastern adversary and the impact this would have (mostly) on the (elites of the) new nations, only an occasional handful of scholars said much about persisting, typically at the local level, cultural practices such as religion. What is less excusable is that these same scholars and intellectuals let their belief in modernity's inevitable secularization prevent them from seeing what was happening within their own societies. Whereas the mainstream Catholic and Protestant churches, which have been dominant in the West since the Reformation and the beginnings of modernity, were in fact becoming more secular and bureaucratic, their third world offshoots were not. Instead, the southern churches were engaged in a process not unlike what Christianity experienced when it was first brought from the Mediter- ranean to Europe. Beliefs and forms of worship coming from one part of the world were syncretized or mixed with those previously held by the new converts. The results were revised and modified forms of Christianity that were more politically conservative than those of their northern counterparts and incorporated elements, such as trance states, spirit possession and revel- ation, that although no longer practiced, had been significant aspects of earlier Christian practices. In Brazil, the most populous Roman Catholic country in the world, vast numbers participate more in processions, pilgrimages and the making of Greenfield: Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories 277 vows to saints for help with practical matters than in the activities of the official church and its priesthood. This concern of the people with illness, poverty, hunger and other forms of suffering gave birth to Liberation Theology and the establishment of Christian Base Communities (within the official Church), the goal of which was social change through engagement in the political process. Brazil is home to the largest number of inhabitants active in African- derived religions. In the second half of the 20th century, large numbers from all social classes joined what formerly had been small, marginal cults of African-derived Spiritist practice in which they learned to enter into trance and receive spirits who instructed and helped with the affairs of daily life. Worshippers, including many leaders, simultaneously attended Roman Catholic services enabling them to claim affiliation with this largest of Chris- tian groups. More recently, Pentecostal and other evangelical Protestants, having declared (spiritual) war on both the African-derived groups and the Catholic Church, have made a large and still growing number of converts. To gain followers, they, like their antagonists before them, have encompassed many of the indigenous practices previously absorbed by their adversaries. Syncretic mixtures of African, Amerindian and Spiritist elements character- ize many of the services of evangelical Brazilian churches. The result is a Christianity different from and often unfamiliar to not just the rank-and-file of worshippers in North America and Europe, who often subsidize their co-religionists in the South, but even to religious scholars. A similar situation is found throughout the rest of Latin America and the Caribbean, where Christian churches in nations that had sizeable indigenous populations, such as Mexico, Guatemala, El Salvador, Peru, Columbia and Chile, today exhibit greater inputs of Amerindian elements in their Christianity than the African-derived practices of Brazil and other places where large numbers of African slaves had been imported. In all cases, trance states, requests for supernatural assistance with practical problems and healing pervade the ever growing Christianity. In Africa, European churches accompanied the colonization process as they did in the Americas. In the western hemisphere, aspects of indigenous worship were mixed with and then incorporated as converts were absorbed. Here too, individuals at the local level, often claiming to receive revelations, established their own churches that after national independence either remained unaffiliated, or continued their association with denominations in Europe and the US. In local-level groups who maintain ties with affiliates in the North, trance states, incorporation of or communication with the super- natural and an emphasis on solving practical problems, including healing, dominate religious practices and beliefs in ways that although prevalent throughout the South, remain suspect to those in the North. An interesting parallel may be drawn between these new, revitalized, or 278 Current Sociology Vol. 53 No. 2 Monograph 1 syncretized forms of Christianity ­ the 'Next Christendom', as Jenkins (2002) titles his book on the subject ­ and Islam. Both monotheistic faiths, according to demographic data, are expanding rapidly, but primarily in the South, where population growth is expected to be greatest in the new century. And as each expands in its own way, it seems to be incorporating aspects of the traditional practices of its converts that emphasize the more mystical parts of their pasts such as entering into a trance and communicating directly with the supernatural to obtain otherworldly help in solving practical problems of this world. In parts of the world where Buddhism, Hinduism and other ancient religions are competing with the missionary efforts of the proselytizing monotheistic faiths to retain their share of the growing world population, a similar situation may be found. Religion has not disappeared across most of the earth as modernization theorists predicted, but instead is growing, with the expansion being greater in groups whose names are known to western- ers than in exotic faiths. Many of the beliefs and practices of the rapidly expanding monotheistic faiths have diverged from those of their counterparts in the West in ways that have made them foreign even to those whose groups bear the same name. What seem to be major characteristics of so many of the religions ­ Christian, Muslim or traditional, old and new ­ in the rest of the world are: (1) the pervasiveness of trance states or other ASC that facilitates (2) direct communication with, and in cases the incorporation of, the super- natural, which is (3) enlisted to provide help with practical problems. Having proposed a relationship between this expanding constellation of religious attributes and the pervasive effort by peoples across the globe to recreate seemingly lost social pasts ­ to enable them to better respond to the exigencies of colonization, imperialism, globalization and internal subjuga- tion following independence ­ we turn to recent thinking and research on learning and memory, beginning from the perspective of neurobiology.4 The Neuroscience of Learning and Memory According to LeDoux (2002: 134), 'Most neuroscientists today believe that alterations in synaptic connectivity underlie learning, and that memory is the stabilization and maintenance of these changes over time.' This statement rests on a view of the way life works that assumes that all organisms are composed of cells that in turn are made up of genes containing DNA (and RNA) that provide instructions for the production of the proteins and other enzymes that make the organs and other parts of the members of each species. The genetic materials also contribute to the ordinary working behaviors of the organism. While genes may determine what may be thought of as the basic grid for all living forms, they, and consequently the phenotypes they Greenfield: Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories 279 produce, may be modified by information coming from outside the cell and, ultimately, from outside the organism. A genetically programmed plan containing pre-existing synaptic connec- tions leading from cells in other parts of the body to the neurons of the brain is assumed by all contemporary students of learning. But, as Edelman (1987) has shown, the synapses leading to the brain, like organisms in the Darwin- ian model of evolution, compete to stay alive, with only the successful ones surviving. Putting behind them the often acrimonious nature/nurture debate that polarizes thinking and research, neuroscientists have adopted Hebb's (1949) hypothesis of plasticity ­ that neurons may be altered by experience. This makes it possible for them to see nature and nurture as always interact- ing. 'The pattern of neural circuitry', as Edelman (1987; cited in LeDoux, 2002: 73) continues, therefore, '. . . is neither established nor rearranged instructively in response to external influences.' 'External influences', adds LeDoux (2002: 73), 'instead, select synapses by initiating and reinforcing certain patterns of neural activity that involve them' on which selection ulti- mately operates, and from which experience does the selecting. The human brain contains 'billions of neurons, or brain cells that together make trillions of synaptic connections among one another' (LeDoux, 2002: 49). Chemicals, such as glutamate, help to bind together neurons in the continuing formation of new synapses. Several categories of genes have been isolated. Most basic are those called housekeeping genes. These create the cells and other structures and their DNA provides instructions for making the enzymes that maintain the basic functions of life such as metabolism, energy production, growth, respiration, waste removal, etc. A second category of genes are the so-called 'clock genes' that coordinate the chronobiology of life, setting the time cycles of many common everyday states like waking, sleeping and dreaming. The time parameters set by the clock genes coordinate the pace and rhythm of life processes of metabolism, homeostasis, growth, energy, information flow, behavior, memory and meaning. Still another class of genes of special interest here is the immediate-early, or third messenger genes. These carry signals that transfer extra-cellular information ­ often coming from outside the organism ­ which modifies 'target gene' transcription, thereby changing the phenotype of neurons. According to Rossi (2002: 29), these 'Genes literally come to life when they creatively replay and express . . . information in response to signals from the environment to transcribe . . . stored DNA information into messenger RNA in the molecule of the cell.' Everything from sexual stimuli, temperature, food, psychological stress, physical trauma and toxins in the environment can be signaled to these genes to turn them on, thereby modi- fying the neurons. For most forms of learning to occur, information from outside the organism, entering through the eyes, ears, skin, etc., must be transduced to 280 Current Sociology Vol. 53 No. 2 Monograph 1 the cells where it may turn on immediate-early genes that target, for example, other genes to make the proteins that transmit the information along a series of synapses eventually reaching the brain. Of special importance here are studies showing that certain messenger molecules stabilize learning in a very distinctive way. When learning is encoded under conditions of high emotional arousal, Rossi (2002: 30ff.) writes, it tends 'to become state-dependent (or state bound) to that psycho- biological condition'. Remembering what has been learned is dependent on the original psychobiological condition when it was first transmitted along the synapses and encoded in the brain. The content of what is learned and stored then may become dissociated or apparently 'lost' after the messenger molecules released have been metabolized and returned to normal levels. 'Reactivating them has a tendency to reestablish the original encoding condition, and with it, the emotions and varying degrees of memory' of the original experience (Rossi, 2002: 67). Neuroscientists make a distinction between learning and memory and see them as distinct processes. Although based on learning, memories, as Bartlett (1932) demonstrated more than seven decades ago, are (re)construc- tions assembled after the fact and at the time of retrieval. They are in most instances based only in part on what was originally learned. At the time of learning, the experience, after being transduced to the cells where it may turn on some genes to produce enzymes that transmit it along the synapses, is stored in the brain. The original event, however, is only one of the items contributing to what is reconstructed and hence remembered. Conscious (Explicit) vs Non-Declarative (Implicit) Learning and Memory Students of learning and memory differentiate between what they classify as the conscious, or declarative, and the procedural, or implicit (LeDoux, 2002: Chs 5 and 6; Milner et al., 1998). The declarative is easy for an individual to recall and present verbally since it refers primarily to people, objects and places. The non-declarative type is used for perceptual and motor skills and, more generally, for behavior. This makes it difficult, if not impossible, to express verbally. Implicit learning is evident primarily in performance (Kandel, 1999: 508; LeDoux, 2002: 98). Most cultural behaviors studied by anthropologists are acquired implicitly. Information learned explicitly may be transformed into procedural memory through repetition. When first behind the wheel, for example, driving involves conscious recollection that, over time, becomes an automatic motor activity (Kandel, 1999: 508). Edelman and Tononi (2000: 186ff.) show how playing the piano provides another example. When first practicing a Greenfield: Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories 281 series of arpeggios, the pianist will begin with a single one, add another, and then still a third, eventually stringing them together. As each is mastered, they are 'nested' with links set up among the loops at the level of the basic ganglia or other cortical appendages.5 With practice, the at first conscious effort becomes implicit as the portions of the brain responsible for the execution of each routine are reduced to a specific and dedicated set of linked (neural) circuits. Eventually, during a performance, fingers move without thought and what has been learned is executed without conscious awareness as if uninfluenced by any intentional control the pianist exerts. Another example of declarative and implicit learning is seen in the acqui- sition and use of language. Even if we were to accept Pinker's (1994) idea that the potential for grammar and language exists in the genes of humans, it would still have to be activated for an individual to be able to speak or under- stand any specific language. By virtue of being exposed to one language, a child will make (and also hear) a certain range and variety of sounds. For example, someone growing up speaking Japanese would not differentiate between the 'l' and the 'r' sound since the two are used interchangeably. Should that same individual then study English, asking for a 'rorypop' would not be unexpected or unreasonable.6 The members of a language group, to return to Edelman's (1987) model of synaptic evolution, may use some of the synapses (making the necessary proteins, etc.) and not others, leading the activated ones to grow and those remaining to atrophy and perhaps disappear. Although it is possible to make new synapses, facilitating the enunciation of additional sounds, it becomes increasingly difficult to do so like a native speaker after childhood. This often results in what is commonly thought of as speaking with an accent. Acquir- ing another language, and the social problems at times associated with making sounds that a speaker's native tongue did not recognize, brings to the fore the emotion associated with language acquisition and use. Misspeaking or mispronouncing invites sanctions that may be as mild as a sideward glance or as strong as outward condemnation. When we grow up speaking a specific language, we master the appro- priate way of pronouncing sounds and placing them into larger units of phrases and sentences. Neuroscientists would state that, along with the synapses that encode the making, hearing and use of sounds in their proper places, other proteins form synapses that when activated produce socially recognized reactions to mistakes. These encoded, implicitly stored patterns lead individuals to monitor both the behavior of others and their own linguis- tic practices, with the latter resulting in something similar to feelings of guilt and shame. In brief, members of a community learn both the accepted emotional responses associated with linguistic misuse and appropriate ways to sanction errors; this combination results in a self-correcting system in 282 Current Sociology Vol. 53 No. 2 Monograph 1 which the specifics of sound production and grammatical arrangements are perpetuated over time. The degree of emotionality associated with language acquisition suggests that the social context at the time it is encoded may be state dependent and bound to the psychobiological condition during which the learning took place. Many other forms of culture are learned in a manner similar to acquir- ing a language or playing the piano. Although consciousness may seem to be involved, like the arpeggios and the phonemes, they too are implicit and transmitted chemically along the synapses to circuits in the brain and stored, to be retrieved later as memories when circumstances call for it. Since so much of human learning is acquired under conditions that encode emotional responses, I suggest that many aspects of cultural behavior are state depen- dent. Culture as Human Learning From its beginnings, anthropology has been a field straddling the sciences and the humanities. When Tylor first set out what was to become an academic discipline, he defined it in terms of its master concept. Culture for him was 'that complex whole which includes knowledge, belief, art, morals, law, custom, and any other capabilities and habits acquired by man as a member of society' (see Tylor, 1970: 1; emphasis added). Later Kroeber would add: 'anthropology is the interrelation of what is biological in man and what is social and historical in him' raising the issue of the balance between the two and the influence of each on the other (see Kroeber, 1948: 2). Thus, anthro- pology was inevitably placed squarely in the middle of the nature/nurture debate. Field studies conducted after the turn of the 20th century revealed the broad range of diversity humans possessed in organizing their lives, leading anthropologists to emphasize the differences between people, raising ques- tions about the cultural and behavioral potential biology made possible. Prior to the Second World War, as Wolf (1974: 33) observes, 'the psychobiological design of man seemed irrelevant'. It was assumed to be open and 'could be made to subscribe to any culture. Cultural variability, unhampered by limi- tations of physique or psyche, seemed endless.' Emphasis was placed on the range of socially based cultural learning acquired by a human being as a member of society almost to the neglect of possible biological limitations. This orientation changed significantly after the Second World War. Humans came to be seen as forced by the inherited design 'over and over again to seek answers to the same questions, solutions to the same needs' (Wolf, 1974: 33). This postwar change as to how much the genetic underpinnings determined or constrained human potential to create culture coincided with the genetic Greenfield: Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories 283 revolution. Founders of new fields, such as sociobiology and evolutionary psychology, accepting the determinism postulated by Crick,7 berated anthro- pology, claiming that it paid no attention to the biological design of the human animal. Using older anthropological studies and discussions of learned behavior as straw men, they continue to criticize the field in their effort to reduce explanations of behavior to biology. This assault was unnecessary. Recent theory and research in neuroscience show emphatically that 'nature and nurture are always interacting in the processes of psychobiological communication via the psychosocial dynamics of gene expression' (Rossi, 2002: 201; emphasis in the original). A logical sugges- tion at this point in time is that neuroscience would profit by adding the comparative cultural perspective anthropology has brought to scientific inquiry, while anthropology would benefit from the addition of more biophysiology when analyzing its data. Non-Conscious Learning, Images and Emotion Many aspects of culture learned early in life, similarly to music and language, are learned implicitly. Consequently, rarely are they ever expressed verbally, or brought to the level of consciousness. Examples abound. It is well docu- mented that when people from sub-Saharan Africa walk or dance, their hips and lower limbs alone seem to be in motion, while Europeans, in contrast, employ their heads, shoulders and legs, moving their hips less. One pattern has no adaptive advantage over the other since both enable the individual to go from one place to another or to rhythmically accompany music. This does not stop members of either group from sanctioning those among them who do not act 'properly'. That is, members of social groups judge and then sanction each other when one does not conform to those patterns that are normative to the group. The behaviors, as in the case of language, are learned in emotion-laden contexts, making them, as suggested previously, state dependent. This is true of other seemingly mundane aspects of life such as eating, or covering the body. Certainly, at a basic biological level, humans are capable of consuming and deriving nutrition from a wide variety of items. In fact, however, specific societies consume only a limited range of things available to them and their members often respond negatively8 to even the thought of ingesting, let alone swallowing what they have been taught is not food. Conversely, many foods are associated with strong positive feelings. In instances where populations have been incorporated as minorities in modern nations, foods believed to have been eaten regularly by their ancestors have on occasions become markers of their identity. The emotionality associated with their consumption suggests that learning to ingest them was state dependent. 284 Current Sociology Vol. 53 No. 2 Monograph 1 We claim that biological processes make us 'feel hungry' at appropriate times. When a meal is missed because of pressing activities or when travel- ing in parts of the world where three meals a day are not standard, we become hungry according to our schedule and react when deprived of access to food. A similar response frequently occurs when fully nutritious exotic foods are offered. Even if very hungry, accepting dog or rat and eating it could result in (uncontrolled) biological reactions. Where and how one eats are learned behaviors that also carry an emotional load. While it is fully acceptable to be offered pasta or an omelet, to have to eat it with one's fingers or in the closet can be expected to elicit responses indicating that the original acquisition of the behavioral pattern was state dependent.9 I am suggesting that a great variety of distinctively human behaviors, including how people think about the world in which they live and the categories they use to classify what is in it ­ 'the belief(s), art, morals, law, custom, and . . . other capabilities and habits acquired by man as a member of society' used by Tylor (1970: 1) to define culture ­ are learned implicitly and encoded in a state dependent manner. Though not available to conscious- ness, and for discussion, they are present ordering the thinking and activities of those exposed to (or socialized in terms of) the learning patterns of their particular group. Held in the declarative memories of individual members of a group, they appear more in their shared symbols and images than in the words they use. 'People in interaction', as Aijmer (1995: 4­5) writes, . . . always draw on their repository of images when they construct social life. The force of imagery is something which [sic] is not easily retrievable in terms of language. . . . Images make themselves known through cultural institutions, not by way of reflexive thought. . . . imagery, especially in the form of stable icons, frames and supports the social discourse. The force it transmits being differently construed than the sentences of language, gives a particular dignity ­ the voice of human morality ­ to its messages, which are clearly intuited and yet not reflexively understood. The imagery seems to operate as visual pictures whose semanticity hinges on the presence of still other images. This complex of unverbalized information about the details of appropriate action in a great number of specific contexts, Aijmer concludes, draws into the behaviors of individuals 'implications of morality, righteousness, correctness, order, and ultimately the force of blessing' (Aijmer, 1995: 4­5). The generalization that human beings learn the specific behaviors that have been established or institutionalized by the members of the social group into which they are born and that these practices, which carry considerable emotional weight, shape their thoughts and deeds is an anthropological truism. For most of human existence, there has been considerable stability and Greenfield: Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories 285 continuity in the content of the beliefs and behaviors learned by the members of social groups across the generations. Though this has become less the case in recent years, studies find at least some central aspects of cultures are trans- mitted, if only in modified form, over relatively long periods of time. This certainly does not mean that fluctuations do not occur.10 Researchers whose thinking is consistent with modernization theory have decried the impli- cations that some of the changes would have on the future of the peoples they study. New accounts tell of those who have been uprooted by commercial expansion, civil or proxy wars and other factors beyond their control that have forced them from their ancestral homes. Relocated on the margins, and peripheral to newly independent national societies, some tens and even hundreds of millions of people now must seek employment11 in order to survive in a world they are not prepared for. Generations are being born for whom the ways of their ancestors are no longer viable and are being discarded and forgotten. The reconstruction of a social past is a conscious and intentional act. People whose forebears came from the same place and who may have learned fragments of an ancestral language and other behaviors leading them to assume that they once shared a way of life or a view of the world, reflect on and discuss with each other the content of this seemingly forgotten past. The specifics of the cultural beliefs and behaviors whose memory is sought, however, were learned implicitly and state dependently; consequently, it is not available in conscious memory that can be expressed in words. How can individuals not conscious of what they may have acquired from their ances- tors, living in different settings, speaking new languages and behaving in different ways, possibly remember and reconstruct their social past? The answer may be related, in some way, to participation in the rapidly expanding religions characterized by their acceptance and advocacy of trance and other ASC in their ritual practices. This relationship between recon- structing state-dependent, implicit, non-conscious memories and ASC is discussed in the next section. Hypnosis and the Reconstruction of State-Dependent Learning 'Perceptive surgeons', Rossi and Cheek (1988: 107) write, 'have long reported cases in which their patients were apparently able to hear and remember significant events and controversial remarks while under general anesthesia.' Assuming a patient was 'asleep', a surgeon may inadvertently say, 'it's gangrenous', before removing some infected tissue, or 'we missed it' ­ refer- ring to something else ­ and then uttering, 'let's close' after excising a tumor. The patient, unable to see what was happening and relying only on what was heard, 'believed' that they 'had gangrene', or that the surgeon had closed 286 Current Sociology Vol. 53 No. 2 Monograph 1 without removing the tumor. As a result, their recovery was negatively affected and hampered. Rossi and Cheek present numerous such examples and explain how with psychotherapy and hypnosis, the patients were able to reconstruct the events, move them to the level of consciousness, making their resolution possible through discussion. The relevance for us is that while hypnotized, the patients, with the help of suggestions from the therapist, were able to reconstruct events that had been learned state dependently and stored non-consciously and non-verbally. Elsewhere,12 Rossi (1993) presents examples of behaviors ranging from phobias to traumatic amnesia and multiple personalities that were learned state dependently and retained implicitly. Hypnotherapy enabled the sufferer to reconstruct the original learning context, leading to eventual recovery. I suggest that apparently 'lost' cultural items may also be recovered under hypnosis. That is, in response to suggestions made while an individual is in trance,13 the person may be able to reconstruct not consciously acquired early learning that may be moved to explicit memory and then discussed. Inducing patients into altered states for therapeutic purposes goes back to the work of Franz Anton Mesmer a little over two centuries ago, anti- dating the advent of modern psychology, psychiatry and psychoanalysis.14 Since that time, a large body of evidence has accumulated demonstrating the significant improvement in treatment obtained when patients are in hypnotic (altered) states (see Greenfield, in press; Barber, 1999, 1984; Ewin, 1984; Holroyd, 1992; Marmer, 1959; Ley and Freeman, 1984; Moore and Kaplan, 1983; Ruzyla-Smith et al., 1995; Rossi, 2002, 1993). Although some still question whether hypnosis, and by implication all altered states, actually exist, and contend that only a small percentage of people are good hypnotic subjects and able to respond with or without formal induction, Rossi (1993, 1996, 2002), following in the tradition of Erickson, in contrast, maintains that entering an altered state is part of the normal, everyday biological experience of all human beings.15 Referring to this as 'the wave nature of consciousness and being' (Rossi, 1996: 120), he adds that half a century ago researchers observed that every 90 minutes or so throughout the night, sleep became a very 'active process' for about 10­30 minutes, during which oxygen consumption increased and more blood flowed to the brain. Breathing, heart rate, blood pressure and gastrointestinal movements became more variable than during wakefulness. During these periods of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, brain wave patterns, as measured by electroencephalograph (EEG), became similar to the active pattern when awake. Researchers later confirmed that the 90- to 120-minute dream rhythm apparently continues during the day. Kleitman (1963, 1969), one of the first to observe the REM activity pattern, refers to a basic rest­activity cycle (BRAC) in sleep and wakefulness. The early therapists to use hypnosis, Rossi (1996: 124) believes, may have Greenfield: Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories 287 been intuitively aware of the wave nature of human experience. But it was Erickson who effectively applied it. Unlike his colleagues who saw patients for a 50-minute session, he preferred to meet for an hour and a half or more. 'He claimed that people in everyday life also naturally drifted between subtle but distinct mind­body states. When he worked with patients for at least an hour and a half or two, he found, they were almost certain to go through distinct changes in their consciousness and states of being' (Rossi, 1996: 129).16 This is the time it takes for the gene expression/protein synthesis cycle to complete one ultradian cycle (Rossi, 2002: 69). 'What has been tradition- ally called "therapeutic suggestion" may be, in essence, the accessing, entrain- ment, and utilization of ultradian/circadian replays of mind­body communication on all levels, from the cellular-genomic to the behavioral, that are responsive to psychosocial cues' (Rossi, 2002: 70; emphasis in the original). Conclusion Like surgical patients who retrieved emotionally laden information they were not consciously aware they held until entering a hypnotic state, I suggest that people wishing to reproduce their cultural past might do so when they enter into a trance state and are prodded by suggestions that, though unintended, can be as effective as those made by a therapist. Multitudes of people, especially those in the southern hemisphere, have been dislocated from the lands of their ancestors and now find themselves on the periphery looking in on a new, postcolonial world. While engaging in (religious) rituals that last for more than one and a half to two hours (one BRAC cycle) and encouraged to go into trance, they find, especially if they are actively wishing to reconstruct their social pasts, that in response to probes suggested by leaders who understand their plight, they may in fact be able to move into conscious memory aspects of long forgotten (cultural) learning. That is, indi- viduals in an ASC desiring to remember may transduce external information contained in sermons or performances to the level of their cells where proteins are made and activate synapses that in turn will bring to conscious memory aspects of beliefs and behaviors acquired implicitly so long ago. More importantly, while participating in the rituals, or after their completion, in the presence of others of like (social) background with similar aspirations, the remembered fragments are verbalized and discussed. People can share these emerging thoughts, distant feelings and partial behaviors. The combi- nation of individual recollections and shared reminiscences is often conceived of as remembering a way of life that no longer exists. Further social inter- action can strengthen and reinforce the hoped for reconstruction. Bartlett's (1932) conclusion that memories, although derived from learning, are actually reconstructions assembled after the fact and based only 288 Current Sociology Vol. 53 No. 2 Monograph 1 in part on the original acquisition, raises some unsettling implications, however. If the pervasive efforts to reconstruct social pasts result in images that actually represent only a fraction of what was originally learned, would it not be erroneous to think of them, as they so often are presented, as 'authentic' representations of the past? The answer is 'yes'. They are, instead, reinventions, produced in the course of social interaction, from fragments held in the unconscious memories of individuals. What is important for our purposes, nevertheless, is that the process, which combines neurobiology and culture in the bodies of individuals in a religious setting, that is then embell- ished socially, results in something that is providing hope for so many other- wise frustrated and often desperate people. Notes 1 Scholars who found agreement in modernization theory were as diverse as econ- omists and political scientists, on the one hand, and philosophers, ethnologists and folklorists, on the other. 2 Religion, the reader should be reminded, is a folk category of modern, western culture. It developed historically out of the dualism that emerged after the birth of science. Unlike the picture Kuhn (1970) presents of paradigm replacement within science, the scientific view of the world did not completely replace its predecessor. Instead, Christianity, which had provided a total picture of the universe in the West for more than a millennium, was set off in a new category, composed of that which was placed outside the purview of science. 3 In its name, believers of even the most traditional creed could master and apply the technologies of the western world to strike out in anger in response to both real and perceived offenses with startling and devastating results. 4 We see later that this body of knowledge focuses almost exclusively on the biophysiology of learning and memory of single individuals. The reason for this, I suggest, is that while many psychologists have reached out and embraced the new framework, few anthropologists, sociologists and historians have done so. 5 Because of its enormous associative capabilities, the dynamic core (that they hypoth- esize as basic in the brain) would be in an ideal position to link or hierarchically organize a series of preexisting unconscious routines into a particular sequence. (Edelman and Tononi, 2000: 187) 6 While this might evoke laughter among native speakers of English, in some speech contexts the indiscriminate use of a voiced variant in place of the unvoiced one could have embarrassing consequences. 7 When he formulated the 'dogma' of DNA RNA protein, attributing onto- logical priority to the genes (Crick, 1957). 8 That is with negative emotions. 9 The same may be said about covering the body and those parts an individual finds comfortable to leave exposed. Deprived completely of anything with which to Greenfield: Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories 289 cover oneself, the average westerner might find it difficult to function in public. The reader can imagine the reaction if while taking a shower a voice shouted out that the building was on fire and the only way to be saved was to jump immedi- ately out the window. Would one look first for a towel or a bathrobe? 10 A fact fully recognized by ethnographers. 11 Far too often, where no jobs are to be found or pay depressed wages. 12 Using cases taken from his own practices along with some reported by Milton H. Erickson. 13 Which is a state like hypnosis. 14 After being utilized by the Marquis de Puységur, Charcot, Janet and Bernheim in Europe, there was a period of abeyance, after which research into the subject was initiated in the US by Hull, Erickson, Hilgard, and others, and its use in therapy reintroduced by Milton H. Erickson (Gauld, 1992). 15 According to Winkelman (2000: 115), 'A (near) universal of human culture is the existence of institutionalized procedures for altering consciousness.' 16 Erickson called these natural periods 'the Common Everyday Trance'. They are times of 'openness and vulnerability to outside influences; suggestions made during this time are sometimes more easily accepted' (Rossi, 1996: 130). References AIJMER, G. (1995) Introduction to Syncretism and the Commerce of Symbols. Göteborg: The Institute for Advanced Studies in Social Anthropology at Göteborg University. BARBER, T. X. (1984) 'Changing "Unchangeable" Bodily Processes by (Hypnotic) Suggestions: A New Look at Hypnosis, Cognitions, Imagining, and the Mind­Body Problem', in A. A. Sheik (ed.) Imagination and Healing, pp. 69­127. Farmingdale, NY: Baywood Publishing. BARBER, T. X. (1999) 'A Comprehensive Three Dimensional Theory of Hypnosis', in I. Kirsch et al. (eds) Clinical Hypnosis and Self Regulation. Cognitive-Behavioral Perspectives, pp. 21­48. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. BARTLETT, F. C. (1932) Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CRICK, F. H. C. (1957) Symposium of the Society for Experimental Biology 12: 138­63. EDELMAN, G. M. (1987) Neural Darwinism: The Theory of Neuronal Group Selection. New York: Basic Books. EDELMAN, G. M. and TONONI, G. (2000) A Universe of Consciousness: How Matter Become Imagination. New York: Basic Books. EWIN, D. M. (1984) 'Hypnosis in Surgery and Anesthesia', in W. C. Western and A. H. Smith (eds) Clinical Hypnosis: A Multidisciplinary Approach, pp. 210­35. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott. GAULD, A. A. (1992) History of Hypnotism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. GREENFIELD, Sidney M. (in press) 'The Cultural Biology of Brazilian Spiritist Surgery and Other Alternatives to Biomedical Healing', International Journal of Para- psychology. HEBB, D. O. ( 1949) The Organization of Behavior. New York: John Wiley. 290 Current Sociology Vol. 53 No. 2 Monograph 1 HOLROYD, J. (1992) 'Hypnosis as a Methodology in Psychological Research', in E. Fromm and M. R. Nash (eds) Contemporary Hypnosis Research, pp. 201­26. New York: Guilford Press. JENKINS, P. (2002) The Next Christendom: The Coming of Global Christianity. New York: Oxford University Press. KANDEL, E. R. (1999) 'Biology and the Future of Psychoanalysis: A New Intellectual Framework for Psychiatry Revisited', American Journal of Psychiatry 56: 505­24. KLEITMAN, N. (1963) Sleep and Wakefulness as Alternating Phases in the Cycle of Existence. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. KLEITMAN, N. (1969) 'Basic Rest­Activity Cycle in Relation to Sleep and Wakeful- ness', in E. Hartman (ed.) Sleep: Physiology and Pathology, pp. 33­8. Philadel- phia, PA: Lippincott. KROEBER, A. L. (1948) Anthropology: Race, Language, Culture, Psychology, Prehis- tory. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Co. (Orig. pub. 1921.) KUHN, Thomas S. (1970) The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, International Encyclopedia of Unified Science, 2nd edn, enlarged. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. LEDOUX, Joseph (2002) Synaptic Self: How Our Brains Become Who We Are. New York: Viking Press. LEY, R. G. and FREEMAN, R. J. (1984) 'Imagery, Cerebral Laterality and the Healing Process', in A. A. Sheik (ed.) Imagination and Healing, pp. 51­68. Farmingdale, NY: Baywood Publishing. MARMER, M. J. (1959) Hypnosis in Anesthesiology. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. MILNER, B., SQUIRE, L. and KANDEL, E. (1998) 'Cognitive Neuroscience and the Study of Memory', Neuron 20: 445­68. MOORE, L. E. and KAPLAN, J. Z. (1983) 'Hypnotically Accelerated Burn Wound Healing', American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis 26: 16­19. PINKER, Steven (1994) The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. New York: Harper. ROSSI, E. L. (1993) The Psychobiology of Mind­Body Healing: New Concepts of Therapeutic Hypnosis. New York and London: W. W. Norton. (Orig. pub. 1986.) ROSSI, E. L. (1996) The Symptom Path to Enlightenment. The New Dynamics of Self- Organization in Hypnotherapy: An Advanced Manual for Beginners. Pacific Palisades, CA: Palisades Gateway Publishing. ROSSI, E. L. (2002) The Psychobiology of Gene Expression: Neuroscience and Neuro- genesis in Hypnosis and the Healing Arts. New York and London: W. W. Norton. ROSSI, E. L. and CHEEK, D. (1988) Mind­Body Therapy: Methods of Ideodynamic Healing in Hypnosis. New York and London: W. W. Norton. RUZYLA-SMITH, P., BARABASZ, A., BARABASZ, M. and WARNER, D. (1995) 'Effects of Hypnosis on the Immune Response: B-Cells, T-Cells, Helper and Supressor Cells', American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis 38(2): 71­9. TYLOR, E. B. (1970) Religion in Primitive Culture. Gloucester, MA: Peter Smith. (Orig. pub. as part of Primitive Culture, 1871.) WINKELMAN, M. (2000) Shamanism: The Neural Ecology of Consciousness and Healing. Westport, CT: Bergin and Garvey. WOLF, E. R. (1974) Anthropology. New York: Norton. Greenfield: Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories 291</meta-value>
</custom-meta>
</custom-meta-wrap>
</article-meta>
</front>
<back>
<notes>
<p>
<list list-type="order">
<list-item>
<p>1 Scholars who found agreement in modernization theory were as diverse as economists and political scientists, on the one hand, and philosophers, ethnologists and folklorists, on the other.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>2 Religion, the reader should be reminded, is a folk category of modern, western culture. It developed historically out of the dualism that emerged after the birth of science. Unlike the picture Kuhn (1970) presents of paradigm replacement within science, the scientific view of the world did not completely replace its predecessor. Instead, Christianity, which had provided a total picture of the universe in the West for more than a millennium, was set off in a new category, composed of that which was placed outside the purview of science.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>3 In its name, believers of even the most traditional creed could master and apply the technologies of the western world to strike out in anger in response to both real and perceived offenses with startling and devastating results.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>4 We see later that this body of knowledge focuses almost exclusively on the biophysiology of learning and memory of single individuals. The reason for this, I suggest, is that while many psychologists have reached out and embraced the new framework, few anthropologists, sociologists and historians have done so.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>5 Because of its enormous associative capabilities, the dynamic core (that they hypothesize as basic in the brain) would be in an ideal position to link or hierarchically organize a series of preexisting unconscious routines into a particular sequence. (Edelman and Tononi, 2000: 187)</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>6 While this might evoke laughter among native speakers of English, in some speech contexts the indiscriminate use of a voiced variant in place of the unvoiced one could have embarrassing consequences.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>7 When he formulated the ‘dogma’ of DNA → RNA → protein, attributing ontological priority to the genes (Crick, 1957).</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>8 That is with negative emotions.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>9 The same may be said about covering the body and those parts an individual finds comfortable to leave exposed. Deprived completely of anything with which to cover oneself, the average westerner might find it difficult to function in public. The reader can imagine the reaction if while taking a shower a voice shouted out that the building was on fire and the only way to be saved was to jump immediately out the window. Would one look first for a towel or a bathrobe?</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>10 A fact fully recognized by ethnographers.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>11 Far too often, where no jobs are to be found or pay depressed wages.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>12 Using cases taken from his own practices along with some reported by Milton H. Erickson.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>13 Which is a state like hypnosis.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>14 After being utilized by the Marquis de Puységur, Charcot, Janet and Bernheim in Europe, there was a period of abeyance, after which research into the subject was initiated in the US by Hull, Erickson, Hilgard, and others, and its use in therapy reintroduced by Milton H. Erickson (Gauld, 1992).</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>15 According to Winkelman (2000: 115), ‘A (near) universal of human culture is the existence of institutionalized procedures for altering consciousness.’</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>16 Erickson called these natural periods ‘the Common Everyday Trance’. They are times of ‘openness and vulnerability to outside influences; suggestions made during this time are sometimes more easily accepted’ (Rossi, 1996: 130).</p>
</list-item>
</list>
</p>
</notes>
<ref-list>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>AIJMER, G.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1995</year>
)
<source>Introduction to Syncretism and the Commerce of Symbols</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Göteborg</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>The Institute for Advanced Studies in Social Anthropology at Göteborg University</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>BARBER, T. X.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1984</year>
)
<article-title>‘Changing “Unchangeable” Bodily Processes by (Hypnotic) Suggestions: A New Look at Hypnosis, Cognitions, Imagining, and the Mind-Body Problem’</article-title>
, in
<name name-style="western">
<surname>A. A. Sheik</surname>
</name>
(ed.)
<source>Imagination and Healing</source>
, pp.
<fpage>69</fpage>
-
<lpage>127</lpage>
.
<publisher-loc>Farmingdale, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Baywood Publishing</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>BARBER, T. X.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1999</year>
)
<article-title>‘A Comprehensive Three Dimensional Theory of Hypnosis’</article-title>
, in
<name name-style="western">
<surname>I. Kirsch</surname>
</name>
<name name-style="western">
<surname>et al.</surname>
</name>
(eds)
<source>Clinical Hypnosis and Self Regulation. Cognitive-Behavioral Perspectives</source>
, pp.
<fpage>21</fpage>
-
<lpage>48</lpage>
.
<publisher-loc>Washington, DC</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>American Psychological Association</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>BARTLETT, F. C.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1932</year>
)
<source>Remembering</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="confproc" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>CRICK, F. H. C.</surname>
</name>
(1957)
<conf-name>Symposium of the Society for Experimental Biology</conf-name>
12:
<fpage>138</fpage>
-
<lpage>163</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>EDELMAN, G. M.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1987</year>
)
<source>Neural Darwinism: The Theory of Neuronal Group Selection</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Basic Books</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>EDELMAN, G. M.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>TONONI, G.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2000</year>
)
<source>A Universe of Consciousness: How Matter Become Imagination</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Basic Books</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>EWIN, D. M.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1984</year>
)
<article-title>‘Hypnosis in Surgery and Anesthesia’</article-title>
, in
<name name-style="western">
<surname>W. C. Western</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>A. H. Smith</surname>
</name>
(eds)
<source>Clinical Hypnosis: A Multidisciplinary Approach</source>
, pp.
<fpage>210</fpage>
-
<lpage>235</lpage>
.
<publisher-loc>Philadelphia, PA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Lippincott</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>GAULD, A. A.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1992</year>
)
<source>History of Hypnotism</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>GREENFIELD, Sidney M.</surname>
</name>
(in press)
<article-title>‘The Cultural Biology of Brazilian Spiritist Surgery and Other Alternatives to Biomedical Healing’</article-title>
,
<source>International Journal of Parapsychology</source>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>HEBB, D. O.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1949</year>
)
<source>The Organization of Behavior</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>John Wiley</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>HOLROYD, J.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1992</year>
)
<article-title>‘Hypnosis as a Methodology in Psychological Research’</article-title>
, in
<name name-style="western">
<surname>E. Fromm</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>M. R. Nash</surname>
</name>
(eds)
<source>Contemporary Hypnosis Research</source>
, pp.
<fpage>201</fpage>
-
<lpage>226</lpage>
.
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Guilford Press</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>JENKINS, P.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2002</year>
)
<source>The Next Christendom: The Coming of Global Christianity</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>KANDEL, E. R.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1999</year>
)
<article-title>‘Biology and the Future of Psychoanalysis: A New Intellectual Framework for Psychiatry Revisited’</article-title>
,
<source>American Journal of Psychiatry</source>
<volume>56</volume>
:
<fpage>505</fpage>
-
<lpage>524</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>KLEITMAN, N.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1963</year>
)
<source>Sleep and Wakefulness as Alternating Phases in the Cycle of Existence</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Chicago, IL</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Chicago Press</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>KLEITMAN, N.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1969</year>
)
<article-title>‘Basic Rest-Activity Cycle in Relation to Sleep and Wakefulness’</article-title>
, in
<name name-style="western">
<surname>E. Hartman</surname>
</name>
(ed.)
<source>Sleep: Physiology and Pathology</source>
, pp.
<fpage>33</fpage>
-
<lpage>38</lpage>
.
<publisher-loc>Philadelphia, PA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Lippincott</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>KROEBER, A. L.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1948</year>
)
<source>Anthropology: Race, Language, Culture, Psychology, Prehistory</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Harcourt, Brace and Co.</publisher-name>
(Orig. pub. 1921.)</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>KUHN, Thomas S.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1970</year>
)
<source>The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, International Encyclopedia of Unified Science</source>
,
<edition>2nd edn, enlarged</edition>
.
<publisher-loc>Chicago, IL</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Chicago Press</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>LEDOUX, Joseph</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2002</year>
)
<source>Synaptic Self: How Our Brains Become Who We Are</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Viking Press</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>LEY, R. G.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>FREEMAN, R. J.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1984</year>
)
<article-title>‘Imagery, Cerebral Laterality and the Healing Process’</article-title>
, in
<name name-style="western">
<surname>A. A. Sheik</surname>
</name>
(ed.)
<source>Imagination and Healing</source>
, pp.
<fpage>51</fpage>
-
<lpage>68</lpage>
.
<publisher-loc>Farmingdale, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Baywood Publishing</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>MARMER, M. J.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1959</year>
)
<source>Hypnosis in Anesthesiology</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Springfield, IL</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Charles C. Thomas</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>MILNER, B.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>SQUIRE, L.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>KANDEL, E.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1998</year>
)
<article-title>‘Cognitive Neuroscience and the Study of Memory’</article-title>
,
<source>Neuron</source>
<volume>20</volume>
:
<fpage>445</fpage>
-
<lpage>468</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>MOORE, L. E.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>KAPLAN, J. Z.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1983</year>
)
<article-title>‘Hypnotically Accelerated Burn Wound Healing’</article-title>
,
<source>American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis</source>
<volume>26</volume>
:
<fpage>16</fpage>
-
<lpage>19</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>PINKER, Steven</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1994</year>
)
<source>The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Harper</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>ROSSI, E. L.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1993</year>
)
<source>The Psychobiology of Mind-Body Healing: New Concepts of Therapeutic Hypnosis</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York and London</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>W. W. Norton</publisher-name>
. (Orig. pub. 1986.)</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>ROSSI, E. L.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1996</year>
)
<source>The Symptom Path to Enlightenment. The New Dynamics of Self- Organization in Hypnotherapy: An Advanced Manual for Beginners</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Pacific Palisades, CA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Palisades Gateway Publishing</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>ROSSI, E. L.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2002</year>
)
<source>The Psychobiology of Gene Expression: Neuroscience and Neurogenesis in Hypnosis and the Healing Arts</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York and London</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>W. W. Norton</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>ROSSI, E. L.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>CHEEK, D.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1988</year>
)
<source>Mind-Body Therapy: Methods of Ideodynamic Healing in Hypnosis</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York and London</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>W. W. Norton</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>RUZYLA-SMITH, P.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>BARABASZ, A.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>BARABASZ, M.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>WARNER, D.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1995</year>
)
<article-title>‘Effects of Hypnosis on the Immune Response: B-Cells, T-Cells, Helper and Supressor Cells’</article-title>
,
<source>American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis</source>
<volume>38</volume>
(
<issue>2</issue>
):
<fpage>71</fpage>
-
<lpage>79</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>TYLOR, E. B.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1970</year>
)
<source>Religion in Primitive Culture</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Gloucester, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Peter Smith</publisher-name>
.</citation>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">(Orig. pub. as part of
<source>Primitive Culture</source>
,
<year>1871</year>
.)</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>WINKELMAN, M.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>2000</year>
)
<source>Shamanism: The Neural Ecology of Consciousness and Healing</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Westport, CT</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Bergin and Garvey</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>WOLF, E. R.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1974</year>
)
<source>Anthropology</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Norton</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</article>
</istex:document>
</istex:metadataXml>
<mods version="3.6">
<titleInfo lang="en">
<title>Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories: A Psychosocial Genomic Approach to Reconstituting Social Memory during Religious Rituals</title>
</titleInfo>
<titleInfo type="alternative" lang="en" contentType="CDATA">
<title>Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories: A Psychosocial Genomic Approach to Reconstituting Social Memory during Religious Rituals</title>
</titleInfo>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Sidney M.</namePart>
<namePart type="family">Greenfield</namePart>
</name>
<typeOfResource>text</typeOfResource>
<genre type="research-article" displayLabel="research-article" authority="ISTEX" authorityURI="https://content-type.data.istex.fr" valueURI="https://content-type.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/XTP-1JC4F85T-7">research-article</genre>
<originInfo>
<publisher>Sage Publications</publisher>
<place>
<placeTerm type="text">Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</placeTerm>
</place>
<dateIssued encoding="w3cdtf">2005-03</dateIssued>
<copyrightDate encoding="w3cdtf">2005</copyrightDate>
</originInfo>
<language>
<languageTerm type="code" authority="iso639-2b">eng</languageTerm>
<languageTerm type="code" authority="rfc3066">en</languageTerm>
</language>
<abstract lang="en">This article explores the relationship between efforts by members of diverse social groups to regain, reconstruct or remember disappearing cultural pasts - in which life was believed to have been better and more meaningful - and the distinctive forms taken in the ritual and other practices of so many of today’s religions. The author proposes that religious rituals that enable participants to enter into trance, or other altered states of consciousness (ASC), contribute positively to the reconstruction of social memories and make it possible for seemingly forgotten beliefs and practices to be retrieved. A combination of recent developments in research, theory and conjecture in neuroscience and older theory, data and speculation in anthropology are employed to explicate this idea. The analysis focuses on the (individual human) body within which social practice (or culture) and biophysiological processes converge. For so many in the contemporary world who see their lives as shattered by modernity’s secularizing and dehumanizing forces, what is taking place as they strive to reconstruct their social pasts is a cultural-biological response manifesting itself in a range of social practices in which religion is pre-eminent.</abstract>
<subject>
<genre>keywords</genre>
<topic>altered states of consciousness (ASC)</topic>
<topic>BRAC (one basic rest-activity cycle)</topic>
<topic>ritual</topic>
<topic>social memory</topic>
<topic>trance</topic>
</subject>
<relatedItem type="host">
<titleInfo>
<title>Current sociology</title>
</titleInfo>
<genre type="journal" authority="ISTEX" authorityURI="https://publication-type.data.istex.fr" valueURI="https://publication-type.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/JMC-0GLKJH51-B">journal</genre>
<identifier type="ISSN">0011-3921</identifier>
<identifier type="eISSN">1461-7064</identifier>
<identifier type="PublisherID">CSI</identifier>
<identifier type="PublisherID-hwp">spcsi</identifier>
<part>
<date>2005</date>
<detail type="volume">
<caption>vol.</caption>
<number>53</number>
</detail>
<detail type="issue">
<caption>no.</caption>
<number>2</number>
</detail>
<extent unit="pages">
<start>275</start>
<end>291</end>
</extent>
</part>
</relatedItem>
<identifier type="istex">ABD10C82C70746199D2FCB093F471336E1921078</identifier>
<identifier type="ark">ark:/67375/M70-C4VG936P-F</identifier>
<identifier type="DOI">10.1177/0011392105049540</identifier>
<identifier type="ArticleID">10.1177_0011392105049540</identifier>
<recordInfo>
<recordContentSource authority="ISTEX" authorityURI="https://loaded-corpus.data.istex.fr" valueURI="https://loaded-corpus.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/XBH-0J1N7DQT-B">sage</recordContentSource>
</recordInfo>
</mods>
<json:item>
<extension>json</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>application/json</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/ABD10C82C70746199D2FCB093F471336E1921078/metadata/json</uri>
</json:item>
</metadata>
<serie></serie>
</istex>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Wicri/Psychologie/explor/BernheimV1/Data/Istex/Corpus
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 000889 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Istex/Corpus/biblio.hfd -nk 000889 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Wicri/Psychologie
   |area=    BernheimV1
   |flux=    Istex
   |étape=   Corpus
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     ISTEX:ABD10C82C70746199D2FCB093F471336E1921078
   |texte=   Trance States and Accessing Implicit Memories: A Psychosocial Genomic Approach to Reconstituting Social Memory during Religious Rituals
}}

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.33.
Data generation: Mon Mar 5 17:33:33 2018. Site generation: Thu Apr 29 15:49:51 2021