Serveur d'exploration sur la musique en Sarre

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

Emotion felt by the listener and expressed by the music: literature review and theoretical perspectives

Identifieur interne : 000190 ( Pmc/Corpus ); précédent : 000189; suivant : 000191

Emotion felt by the listener and expressed by the music: literature review and theoretical perspectives

Auteurs : Emery Schubert

Source :

RBID : PMC:3865445

Abstract

In his seminal paper, Gabrielsson (2002) distinguishes between emotion felt by the listener, here: “internal locus of emotion” (IL), and the emotion the music is expressing, here: “external locus of emotion” (EL). This paper tabulates 16 comparisons of felt versus expressed emotions in music published in the decade 2003–2012 consisting of 19 studies/experiments and provides some theoretical perspectives. The key findings were that (1) IL rating was frequently rated statistically the same or lower than the corresponding EL rating (e.g., lower felt happiness rating compared to the apparent happiness of the music), and that (2) self-select and preferred music had a smaller gap across the emotion loci than experimenter-selected and disliked music. These key findings were explained by an “inhibited” emotional contagion mechanism, where the otherwise matching felt emotion may have been attenuated by some other factor such as social context. Matching between EL and IL for loved and self-selected pieces was explained by the activation of “contagion” circuits. Physiological arousal, personality and age, as well as musical features (tempo, mode, putative emotions) also influenced perceived and felt emotion distinctions. A variety of data collection formats were identified, but mostly using rating items. In conclusion, a more systematic use of terminology appears desirable. Two broad categories, namely matched and unmatched, are proposed as being sufficient to capture the relationships between EL and IL, instead of four categories as suggested by Gabrielsson.


Url:
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00837
PubMed: 24381565
PubMed Central: 3865445

Links to Exploration step

PMC:3865445

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI>
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">Emotion felt by the listener and expressed by the music: literature review and theoretical perspectives</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, Emery" sort="Schubert, Emery" uniqKey="Schubert E" first="Emery" last="Schubert">Emery Schubert</name>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">PMC</idno>
<idno type="pmid">24381565</idno>
<idno type="pmc">3865445</idno>
<idno type="url">http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3865445</idno>
<idno type="RBID">PMC:3865445</idno>
<idno type="doi">10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00837</idno>
<date when="2013">2013</date>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Corpus">000190</idno>
<idno type="wicri:explorRef" wicri:stream="Pmc" wicri:step="Corpus" wicri:corpus="PMC">000190</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title xml:lang="en" level="a" type="main">Emotion felt by the listener and expressed by the music: literature review and theoretical perspectives</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, Emery" sort="Schubert, Emery" uniqKey="Schubert E" first="Emery" last="Schubert">Emery Schubert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
<series>
<title level="j">Frontiers in Psychology</title>
<idno type="eISSN">1664-1078</idno>
<imprint>
<date when="2013">2013</date>
</imprint>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">
<p>In his seminal paper, Gabrielsson (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B35">2002</xref>
) distinguishes between emotion felt by the listener, here: “internal locus of emotion” (IL), and the emotion the music is expressing, here: “external locus of emotion” (EL). This paper tabulates 16 comparisons of felt versus expressed emotions in music published in the decade 2003–2012 consisting of 19 studies/experiments and provides some theoretical perspectives. The key findings were that (1) IL rating was frequently rated statistically the same or lower than the corresponding EL rating (e.g., lower felt happiness rating compared to the apparent happiness of the music), and that (2) self-select and preferred music had a smaller gap across the emotion loci than experimenter-selected and disliked music. These key findings were explained by an “inhibited” emotional contagion mechanism, where the otherwise matching felt emotion may have been attenuated by some other factor such as social context. Matching between EL and IL for loved and self-selected pieces was explained by the activation of “contagion” circuits. Physiological arousal, personality and age, as well as musical features (tempo, mode, putative emotions) also influenced perceived and felt emotion distinctions. A variety of data collection formats were identified, but mostly using rating items. In conclusion, a more systematic use of terminology appears desirable. Two broad categories, namely matched and unmatched, are proposed as being sufficient to capture the relationships between EL and IL, instead of four categories as suggested by Gabrielsson.</p>
</div>
</front>
<back>
<div1 type="bibliography">
<listBibl>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ali, S O" uniqKey="Ali S">S. O. Ali</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Peynircio Lu, Z F" uniqKey="Peynircio Lu Z">Z. F. Peynircioǧlu</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Alwin, D F" uniqKey="Alwin D">D. F. Alwin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bakker, A B" uniqKey="Bakker A">A. B. Bakker</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Heuven, E" uniqKey="Heuven E">E. Heuven</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barrett, F S" uniqKey="Barrett F">F. S. Barrett</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grimm, K J" uniqKey="Grimm K">K. J. Grimm</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Robins, R W" uniqKey="Robins R">R. W. Robins</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wildschut, T" uniqKey="Wildschut T">T. Wildschut</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sedikides, C" uniqKey="Sedikides C">C. Sedikides</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Janata, P" uniqKey="Janata P">P. Janata</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barthomeuf, L" uniqKey="Barthomeuf L">L. Barthomeuf</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Droit Volet, S" uniqKey="Droit Volet S">S. Droit-Volet</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rousset, S" uniqKey="Rousset S">S. Rousset</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Baumgartner, H" uniqKey="Baumgartner H">H. Baumgartner</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Belia, S" uniqKey="Belia S">S. Belia</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fidler, F" uniqKey="Fidler F">F. Fidler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Williams, J" uniqKey="Williams J">J. Williams</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cumming, G" uniqKey="Cumming G">G. Cumming</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bharucha, J" uniqKey="Bharucha J">J. Bharucha</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Curtis, M" uniqKey="Curtis M">M. Curtis</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Paroo, K" uniqKey="Paroo K">K. Paroo</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Blood, A J" uniqKey="Blood A">A. J. Blood</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zatorre, R J" uniqKey="Zatorre R">R. J. Zatorre</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bombari, D" uniqKey="Bombari D">D. Bombari</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mast, M S" uniqKey="Mast M">M. S. Mast</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brosch, T" uniqKey="Brosch T">T. Brosch</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sander, D" uniqKey="Sander D">D. Sander</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bono, J E" uniqKey="Bono J">J. E. Bono</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vey, M A" uniqKey="Vey M">M. A. Vey</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brattico, E" uniqKey="Brattico E">E. Brattico</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brattico, P" uniqKey="Brattico P">P. Brattico</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jacobsen, T" uniqKey="Jacobsen T">T. Jacobsen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cacioppo, J T" uniqKey="Cacioppo J">J. T. Cacioppo</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gardner, W L" uniqKey="Gardner W">W. L. Gardner</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cacioppo, J T" uniqKey="Cacioppo J">J. T. Cacioppo</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gardner, W L" uniqKey="Gardner W">W. L. Gardner</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Berntson, G G" uniqKey="Berntson G">G. G. Berntson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Calder, A J" uniqKey="Calder A">A. J. Calder</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Keane, J" uniqKey="Keane J">J. Keane</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Manes, F" uniqKey="Manes F">F. Manes</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Antoun, N" uniqKey="Antoun N">N. Antoun</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Young, A W" uniqKey="Young A">A. W. Young</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chan, A W" uniqKey="Chan A">A.-W. Chan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Altman, D G" uniqKey="Altman D">D. G. Altman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chan, L P" uniqKey="Chan L">L. P. Chan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Livingstone, S R" uniqKey="Livingstone S">S. R. Livingstone</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Russo, F A" uniqKey="Russo F">F. A. Russo</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Charland, L" uniqKey="Charland L">L. Charland</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cheng, S T" uniqKey="Cheng S">S.-T. Cheng</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Collins, S C" uniqKey="Collins S">S. C. Collins</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Colombetti, G" uniqKey="Colombetti G">G. Colombetti</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Coren, S" uniqKey="Coren S">S. Coren</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Girgus, J S" uniqKey="Girgus J">J. S. Girgus</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Coutinho, E" uniqKey="Coutinho E">E. Coutinho</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cangelosi, A" uniqKey="Cangelosi A">A. Cangelosi</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cumming, G" uniqKey="Cumming G">G. Cumming</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Finch, S" uniqKey="Finch S">S. Finch</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Davies, S" uniqKey="Davies S">S. Davies</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Davies, S" uniqKey="Davies S">S. Davies</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dawes, J" uniqKey="Dawes J">J. Dawes</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dibben, N" uniqKey="Dibben N">N. Dibben</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Diener, E" uniqKey="Diener E">E. Diener</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, H" uniqKey="Smith H">H. Smith</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fujita, F" uniqKey="Fujita F">F. Fujita</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dimberg, U" uniqKey="Dimberg U">U. Dimberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thunberg, M" uniqKey="Thunberg M">M. Thunberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Elmehed, K" uniqKey="Elmehed K">K. Elmehed</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dwan, K" uniqKey="Dwan K">K. Dwan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Altman, D G" uniqKey="Altman D">D. G. Altman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Arnaiz, J A" uniqKey="Arnaiz J">J. A. Arnaiz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bloom, J" uniqKey="Bloom J">J. Bloom</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chan, A W" uniqKey="Chan A">A.-W. Chan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cronin, E" uniqKey="Cronin E">E. Cronin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dyck, M" uniqKey="Dyck M">M. Dyck</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Loughead, J" uniqKey="Loughead J">J. Loughead</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kellermann, T" uniqKey="Kellermann T">T. Kellermann</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Boers, F" uniqKey="Boers F">F. Boers</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gur, R C" uniqKey="Gur R">R. C. Gur</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mathiak, K" uniqKey="Mathiak K">K. Mathiak</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Eerola, T" uniqKey="Eerola T">T. Eerola</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vuoskoski, J K" uniqKey="Vuoskoski J">J. K. Vuoskoski</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ekman, P" uniqKey="Ekman P">P. Ekman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Evans, P" uniqKey="Evans P">P. Evans</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Evans, P" uniqKey="Evans P">P. Evans</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Frances, R" uniqKey="Frances R">R. Frances</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Frijda, N H" uniqKey="Frijda N">N. H. Frijda</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kuipers, P" uniqKey="Kuipers P">P. Kuipers</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ter Schure, E" uniqKey="Ter Schure E">E. Ter Schure</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gabrielsson, A" uniqKey="Gabrielsson A">A. Gabrielsson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gabrielsson, A" uniqKey="Gabrielsson A">A. Gabrielsson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Garrido, S" uniqKey="Garrido S">S. Garrido</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Garrido, S" uniqKey="Garrido S">S. Garrido</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Garrido, S" uniqKey="Garrido S">S. Garrido</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Garrido, S" uniqKey="Garrido S">S. Garrido</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gaver, W W" uniqKey="Gaver W">W. W. Gaver</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mandler, G" uniqKey="Mandler G">G. Mandler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gendolla, G H E" uniqKey="Gendolla G">G. H. E. Gendolla</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kruesken, J" uniqKey="Kruesken J">J. Kruesken</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Goldman, A" uniqKey="Goldman A">A. Goldman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gray, E" uniqKey="Gray E">E. Gray</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Watson, D" uniqKey="Watson D">D. Watson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Payne, R" uniqKey="Payne R">R. Payne</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cooper, C" uniqKey="Cooper C">C. Cooper</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Green, M C" uniqKey="Green M">M. C. Green</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grewe, O" uniqKey="Grewe O">O. Grewe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nagel, F" uniqKey="Nagel F">F. Nagel</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Altenmuller, E" uniqKey="Altenmuller E">E. Altenmüller</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kopiez, R" uniqKey="Kopiez R">R. Kopiez</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grewe, O" uniqKey="Grewe O">O. Grewe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nagel, F" uniqKey="Nagel F">F. Nagel</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kopiez, R" uniqKey="Kopiez R">R. Kopiez</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Altenmuller, E" uniqKey="Altenmuller E">E. Altenmüller</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gross, J J" uniqKey="Gross J">J. J. Gross</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="John, O P" uniqKey="John O">O. P. John</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Richards, J M" uniqKey="Richards J">J. M. Richards</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hahn, S" uniqKey="Hahn S">S. Hahn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Williamson, P" uniqKey="Williamson P">P. Williamson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hutton, J" uniqKey="Hutton J">J. Hutton</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Harkness, K L" uniqKey="Harkness K">K. L. Harkness</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jacobson, J A" uniqKey="Jacobson J">J. A. Jacobson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Duong, D" uniqKey="Duong D">D. Duong</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sabbagh, M A" uniqKey="Sabbagh M">M. A. Sabbagh</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Herbert, R" uniqKey="Herbert R">R. Herbert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hevner, K" uniqKey="Hevner K">K. Hevner</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hevner, K" uniqKey="Hevner K">K. Hevner</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hoeckner, B" uniqKey="Hoeckner B">B. Hoeckner</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wyatt, E W" uniqKey="Wyatt E">E. W. Wyatt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Decety, J" uniqKey="Decety J">J. Decety</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nusbaum, H" uniqKey="Nusbaum H">H. Nusbaum</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hunter, P G" uniqKey="Hunter P">P. G. Hunter</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schellenberg, E G" uniqKey="Schellenberg E">E. G. Schellenberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schimmack, U" uniqKey="Schimmack U">U. Schimmack</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hunter, P G" uniqKey="Hunter P">P. G. Hunter</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schellenberg, E G" uniqKey="Schellenberg E">E. G. Schellenberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schimmack, U" uniqKey="Schimmack U">U. Schimmack</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Huron, D" uniqKey="Huron D">D. Huron</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Iacoboni, M" uniqKey="Iacoboni M">M. Iacoboni</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ilie, G" uniqKey="Ilie G">G. Ilie</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thompson, W F" uniqKey="Thompson W">W. F. Thompson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ilie, G" uniqKey="Ilie G">G. Ilie</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thompson, W F" uniqKey="Thompson W">W. F. Thompson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jakobs, E" uniqKey="Jakobs E">E. Jakobs</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Manstead, A S" uniqKey="Manstead A">A. S. Manstead</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fischer, A H" uniqKey="Fischer A">A. H. Fischer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Friberg, A" uniqKey="Friberg A">A. Friberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bresin, R" uniqKey="Bresin R">R. Bresin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Laukka, P" uniqKey="Laukka P">P. Laukka</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Liljestrom, S" uniqKey="Liljestrom S">S. Liljeström</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Laukka, P" uniqKey="Laukka P">P. Laukka</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="V Stfj Ll, D" uniqKey="V Stfj Ll D">D. Västfjäll</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lundqvist, L O" uniqKey="Lundqvist L">L.-O. Lundqvist</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lindstrom, E" uniqKey="Lindstrom E">E. Lindström</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="V Stfj Ll, D" uniqKey="V Stfj Ll D">D. Västfjäll</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kallinen, K" uniqKey="Kallinen K">K. Kallinen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ravaja, N" uniqKey="Ravaja N">N. Ravaja</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kawakami, A" uniqKey="Kawakami A">A. Kawakami</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Furukawa, K" uniqKey="Furukawa K">K. Furukawa</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Katahira, K" uniqKey="Katahira K">K. Katahira</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kamiyama, K" uniqKey="Kamiyama K">K. Kamiyama</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Okanoya, K" uniqKey="Okanoya K">K. Okanoya</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kenealy, P" uniqKey="Kenealy P">P. Kenealy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Khalfa, S" uniqKey="Khalfa S">S. Khalfa</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Isabelle, P" uniqKey="Isabelle P">P. Isabelle</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jean Pierre, B" uniqKey="Jean Pierre B">B. Jean-Pierre</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Manon, R" uniqKey="Manon R">R. Manon</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kivy, P" uniqKey="Kivy P">P. Kivy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kivy, P" uniqKey="Kivy P">P. Kivy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kivy, P" uniqKey="Kivy P">P. Kivy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kivy, P" uniqKey="Kivy P">P. Kivy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Knobloch, S" uniqKey="Knobloch S">S. Knobloch</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zillmann, D" uniqKey="Zillmann D">D. Zillmann</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Koelsch, S" uniqKey="Koelsch S">S. Koelsch</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fritz, T" uniqKey="Fritz T">T. Fritz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Muller, K" uniqKey="Muller K">K. Müller</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Friederici, A D" uniqKey="Friederici A">A. D. Friederici</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kone Ni, V J" uniqKey="Kone Ni V">V. J. Konečni</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kone Ni, V J" uniqKey="Kone Ni V">V. J. Konečni</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kone Ni, V J" uniqKey="Kone Ni V">V. J. Konečni</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brown, A" uniqKey="Brown A">A. Brown</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wanic, R A" uniqKey="Wanic R">R. A. Wanic</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Krause, A E" uniqKey="Krause A">A. E. Krause</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hargreaves, D J" uniqKey="Hargreaves D">D. J. Hargreaves</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kreutz, G" uniqKey="Kreutz G">G. Kreutz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bongard, S" uniqKey="Bongard S">S. Bongard</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Von Jussis, J" uniqKey="Von Jussis J">J. Von Jussis</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kreutz, G" uniqKey="Kreutz G">G. Kreutz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ott, U" uniqKey="Ott U">U. Ott</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Teichmann, D" uniqKey="Teichmann D">D. Teichmann</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Osawa, P" uniqKey="Osawa P">P. Osawa</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vaitl, D" uniqKey="Vaitl D">D. Vaitl</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kreutz, G" uniqKey="Kreutz G">G. Kreutz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mitchell, L A" uniqKey="Mitchell L">L. A. Mitchell</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Krumhansl, C L" uniqKey="Krumhansl C">C. L. Krumhansl</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Krumhansl, C L" uniqKey="Krumhansl C">C. L. Krumhansl</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Langer, S L" uniqKey="Langer S">S. L. Langer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rudd, M E" uniqKey="Rudd M">M. E. Rudd</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Syrjala, K L" uniqKey="Syrjala K">K. L. Syrjala</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Larcom, M J" uniqKey="Larcom M">M. J. Larcom</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Isaacowitz, D M" uniqKey="Isaacowitz D">D. M. Isaacowitz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Larsen, J T" uniqKey="Larsen J">J. T. Larsen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcgraw, A P" uniqKey="Mcgraw A">A. P. Mcgraw</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cacioppo, J T" uniqKey="Cacioppo J">J. T. Cacioppo</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lee, V" uniqKey="Lee V">V. Lee</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Liljestrom, S" uniqKey="Liljestrom S">S. Liljeström</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="V Stfj Ll, D" uniqKey="V Stfj Ll D">D. Västfjäll</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lundqvist, L O" uniqKey="Lundqvist L">L.-O. Lundqvist</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Carlsson, F" uniqKey="Carlsson F">F. Carlsson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hilmersson, P" uniqKey="Hilmersson P">P. Hilmersson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lychner, J A" uniqKey="Lychner J">J. A. Lychner</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mann, S" uniqKey="Mann S">S. Mann</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Manne, S L" uniqKey="Manne S">S. L. Manne</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Norton, T R" uniqKey="Norton T">T. R. Norton</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ostroff, J S" uniqKey="Ostroff J">J. S. Ostroff</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Winkel, G" uniqKey="Winkel G">G. Winkel</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fox, K" uniqKey="Fox K">K. Fox</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grana, G" uniqKey="Grana G">G. Grana</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mardia, K V" uniqKey="Mardia K">K. V. Mardia</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jupp, P E" uniqKey="Jupp P">P. E. Jupp</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Matsumoto, D" uniqKey="Matsumoto D">D. Matsumoto</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Matsumoto, D" uniqKey="Matsumoto D">D. Matsumoto</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kupperbusch, C" uniqKey="Kupperbusch C">C. Kupperbusch</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Matsumoto, J" uniqKey="Matsumoto J">J. Matsumoto</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Menon, V" uniqKey="Menon V">V. Menon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Levitin, D J" uniqKey="Levitin D">D. J. Levitin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meyer, L B" uniqKey="Meyer L">L. B. Meyer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Miall, D S" uniqKey="Miall D">D. S. Miall</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mitchell, G" uniqKey="Mitchell G">G. Mitchell</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Moher, D" uniqKey="Moher D">D. Moher</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Liberati, A" uniqKey="Liberati A">A. Liberati</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tetzlaff, J" uniqKey="Tetzlaff J">J. Tetzlaff</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Altman, D G" uniqKey="Altman D">D. G. Altman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nagel, F" uniqKey="Nagel F">F. Nagel</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kopiez, R" uniqKey="Kopiez R">R. Kopiez</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grewe, O" uniqKey="Grewe O">O. Grewe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Altenmuller, E" uniqKey="Altenmuller E">E. Altenmüller</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Novembre, G" uniqKey="Novembre G">G. Novembre</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ticini, L F" uniqKey="Ticini L">L. F. Ticini</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schutz Bosbach, S" uniqKey="Schutz Bosbach S">S. Schütz-Bosbach</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Keller, P E" uniqKey="Keller P">P. E. Keller</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Oatley, K" uniqKey="Oatley K">K. Oatley</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Panagiotidi, M" uniqKey="Panagiotidi M">M. Panagiotidi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Samartzi, S" uniqKey="Samartzi S">S. Samartzi</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Panksepp, J" uniqKey="Panksepp J">J. Panksepp</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Payne, E" uniqKey="Payne E">E. Payne</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Peretz, I" uniqKey="Peretz I">I. Peretz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Peretz, I" uniqKey="Peretz I">I. Peretz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gagnon, L" uniqKey="Gagnon L">L. Gagnon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hebert, S" uniqKey="Hebert S">S. Hebert</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Macoir, J" uniqKey="Macoir J">J. Macoir</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Perlovsky, L" uniqKey="Perlovsky L">L. Perlovsky</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pouivet, R" uniqKey="Pouivet R">R. Pouivet</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Preston, S D" uniqKey="Preston S">S. D. Preston</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Waal, F" uniqKey="De Waal F">F. De Waal</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pugh, S D" uniqKey="Pugh S">S. D. Pugh</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Radford, C" uniqKey="Radford C">C. Radford</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Raghunathan, R" uniqKey="Raghunathan R">R. Raghunathan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Corfman, K" uniqKey="Corfman K">K. Corfman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Reid, S C" uniqKey="Reid S">S. C. Reid</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kauer, S D" uniqKey="Kauer S">S. D. Kauer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dudgeon, P" uniqKey="Dudgeon P">P. Dudgeon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sanci, L A" uniqKey="Sanci L">L. A. Sanci</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shrier, L A" uniqKey="Shrier L">L. A. Shrier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Patton, G C" uniqKey="Patton G">G. C. Patton</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rentfrow, P J" uniqKey="Rentfrow P">P. J. Rentfrow</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Goldberg, L R" uniqKey="Goldberg L">L. R. Goldberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Levitin, D J" uniqKey="Levitin D">D. J. Levitin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rentfrow, P J" uniqKey="Rentfrow P">P. J. Rentfrow</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gosling, S D" uniqKey="Gosling S">S. D. Gosling</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rickard, N S" uniqKey="Rickard N">N. S. Rickard</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Robinson, J" uniqKey="Robinson J">J. Robinson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct></biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Roy, M" uniqKey="Roy M">M. Roy</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mailhot, J P" uniqKey="Mailhot J">J.-P. Mailhot</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gosselin, N" uniqKey="Gosselin N">N. Gosselin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Paquette, S" uniqKey="Paquette S">S. Paquette</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Peretz, I" uniqKey="Peretz I">I. Peretz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Roy, M" uniqKey="Roy M">M. Roy</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Peretz, I" uniqKey="Peretz I">I. Peretz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rainville, P" uniqKey="Rainville P">P. Rainville</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Russell, J A" uniqKey="Russell J">J. A. Russell</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Russell, J A" uniqKey="Russell J">J. A. Russell</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Russell, J A" uniqKey="Russell J">J. A. Russell</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lewicka, M" uniqKey="Lewicka M">M. Lewicka</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Niit, T" uniqKey="Niit T">T. Niit</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Russell, J A" uniqKey="Russell J">J. A. Russell</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weiss, A" uniqKey="Weiss A">A. Weiss</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mendelsohn, G A" uniqKey="Mendelsohn G">G. A. Mendelsohn</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Saarikallio, S" uniqKey="Saarikallio S">S. Saarikallio</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Erkkil, J" uniqKey="Erkkil J">J. Erkkilä</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Salimpoor, V N" uniqKey="Salimpoor V">V. N. Salimpoor</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Benovoy, M" uniqKey="Benovoy M">M. Benovoy</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Longo, G" uniqKey="Longo G">G. Longo</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cooperstock, J R" uniqKey="Cooperstock J">J. R. Cooperstock</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zatorre, R J" uniqKey="Zatorre R">R. J. Zatorre</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sato, W" uniqKey="Sato W">W. Sato</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fujimura, T" uniqKey="Fujimura T">T. Fujimura</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kochiyama, T" uniqKey="Kochiyama T">T. Kochiyama</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Suzuki, N" uniqKey="Suzuki N">N. Suzuki</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schenker, N" uniqKey="Schenker N">N. Schenker</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gentleman, J F" uniqKey="Gentleman J">J. F. Gentleman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Scherer, K R" uniqKey="Scherer K">K. R. Scherer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Scherer, K R" uniqKey="Scherer K">K. R. Scherer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Scherer, K R" uniqKey="Scherer K">K. R. Scherer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zentner, M R" uniqKey="Zentner M">M. R. Zentner</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schimmack, U" uniqKey="Schimmack U">U. Schimmack</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rainer, R" uniqKey="Rainer R">R. Rainer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcpherson, G E" uniqKey="Mcpherson G">G. E. McPherson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schwarz, N" uniqKey="Schwarz N">N. Schwarz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Strack, F" uniqKey="Strack F">F. Strack</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Scruton, R" uniqKey="Scruton R">R. Scruton</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sevdalis, V" uniqKey="Sevdalis V">V. Sevdalis</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Keller, P E" uniqKey="Keller P">P. E. Keller</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Silvia, P J" uniqKey="Silvia P">P. J. Silvia</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Silvia, P J" uniqKey="Silvia P">P. J. Silvia</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brown, E M" uniqKey="Brown E">E. M. Brown</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sloboda, J A" uniqKey="Sloboda J">J. A. Sloboda</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sloboda, J A" uniqKey="Sloboda J">J. A. Sloboda</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sloboda, J A" uniqKey="Sloboda J">J. A. Sloboda</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Swanwick, K" uniqKey="Swanwick K">K. Swanwick</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thaut, M H" uniqKey="Thaut M">M. H. Thaut</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="De L Etoile, S K" uniqKey="De L Etoile S">S. K. De L'etoile</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thayer, J F" uniqKey="Thayer J">J. F. Thayer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Townsend, D" uniqKey="Townsend D">D. Townsend</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Valentine, C W" uniqKey="Valentine C">C. W. Valentine</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Zijl, A G W" uniqKey="Van Zijl A">A. G. W. Van Zijl</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sloboda, J A" uniqKey="Sloboda J">J. A. Sloboda</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vastfjall, D" uniqKey="Vastfjall D">D. Vastfjall</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vieillard, S" uniqKey="Vieillard S">S. Vieillard</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Peretz, I" uniqKey="Peretz I">I. Peretz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gosselin, N" uniqKey="Gosselin N">N. Gosselin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Khalfa, S" uniqKey="Khalfa S">S. Khalfa</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gagnon, L" uniqKey="Gagnon L">L. Gagnon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bouchard, B" uniqKey="Bouchard B">B. Bouchard</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Viswanathan, M" uniqKey="Viswanathan M">M. Viswanathan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sudman, S" uniqKey="Sudman S">S. Sudman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnson, M" uniqKey="Johnson M">M. Johnson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vuoskoski, J K" uniqKey="Vuoskoski J">J. K. Vuoskoski</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Eerola, T" uniqKey="Eerola T">T. Eerola</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vuoskoski, J K" uniqKey="Vuoskoski J">J. K. Vuoskoski</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thompson, W F" uniqKey="Thompson W">W. F. Thompson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcilwain, D" uniqKey="Mcilwain D">D. Mcilwain</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Eerola, T" uniqKey="Eerola T">T. Eerola</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wang, H L" uniqKey="Wang H">H. L. Wang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cheong, L F" uniqKey="Cheong L">L. F. Cheong</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weld, H P" uniqKey="Weld H">H. P. Weld</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Werner, K" uniqKey="Werner K">K. Werner</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Roberts, N" uniqKey="Roberts N">N. Roberts</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rosen, H" uniqKey="Rosen H">H. Rosen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dean, D" uniqKey="Dean D">D. Dean</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kramer, J" uniqKey="Kramer J">J. Kramer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weiner, M" uniqKey="Weiner M">M. Weiner</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wilhelm, K" uniqKey="Wilhelm K">K. Wilhelm</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gillis, I" uniqKey="Gillis I">I. Gillis</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schubert, E" uniqKey="Schubert E">E. Schubert</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Whittle, E L" uniqKey="Whittle E">E. L. Whittle</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Williamson, P" uniqKey="Williamson P">P. Williamson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gamble, C" uniqKey="Gamble C">C. Gamble</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Altman, D" uniqKey="Altman D">D. Altman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hutton, J" uniqKey="Hutton J">J. Hutton</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yorke, M" uniqKey="Yorke M">M. Yorke</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zentner, M R" uniqKey="Zentner M">M. R. Zentner</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zentner, M R" uniqKey="Zentner M">M. R. Zentner</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grandjean, D" uniqKey="Grandjean D">D. Grandjean</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Scherer, K R" uniqKey="Scherer K">K. R. Scherer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
</listBibl>
</div1>
</back>
</TEI>
<pmc article-type="review-article">
<pmc-dir>properties open_access</pmc-dir>
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="nlm-ta">Front Psychol</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="iso-abbrev">Front Psychol</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="publisher-id">Front. Psychol.</journal-id>
<journal-title-group>
<journal-title>Frontiers in Psychology</journal-title>
</journal-title-group>
<issn pub-type="epub">1664-1078</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Frontiers Media S.A.</publisher-name>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmid">24381565</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmc">3865445</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00837</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Review Article</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>Emotion felt by the listener and expressed by the music: literature review and theoretical perspectives</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>Emery</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="author-notes" rid="fn001">
<sup>*</sup>
</xref>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<aff>
<institution>Empirical Musicology Group, School of the Arts and Media, University of New South Wales</institution>
<country>Sydney, NSW, Australia</country>
</aff>
<author-notes>
<fn fn-type="edited-by">
<p>Edited by: Daniel J. Levitin, McGill University, Canada</p>
</fn>
<fn fn-type="edited-by">
<p>Reviewed by: Bradley W. Vines, Nielsen, USA; Gunter Kreutz, Carl von Ossietzky Universität Oldenburg, Germany</p>
</fn>
<corresp id="fn001">*Correspondence: Emery Schubert, Empirical Musicology Group, School of the Arts and Media, University of New South Wales, Street 2460, Sydney, NSW 2052, Australia e-mail:
<email xlink:type="simple">e.schubert@unsw.edu.au</email>
</corresp>
<fn fn-type="other" id="fn002">
<p>This article was submitted to Emotion Science, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology.</p>
</fn>
</author-notes>
<pub-date pub-type="epreprint">
<day>03</day>
<month>7</month>
<year>2013</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="epub">
<day>17</day>
<month>12</month>
<year>2013</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="collection">
<year>2013</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>4</volume>
<elocation-id>837</elocation-id>
<history>
<date date-type="received">
<day>26</day>
<month>4</month>
<year>2013</year>
</date>
<date date-type="accepted">
<day>22</day>
<month>10</month>
<year>2013</year>
</date>
</history>
<permissions>
<copyright-statement>Copyright © 2013 Schubert.</copyright-statement>
<copyright-year>2013</copyright-year>
<license license-type="open-access" xlink:href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/">
<license-p>This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.</license-p>
</license>
</permissions>
<abstract>
<p>In his seminal paper, Gabrielsson (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B35">2002</xref>
) distinguishes between emotion felt by the listener, here: “internal locus of emotion” (IL), and the emotion the music is expressing, here: “external locus of emotion” (EL). This paper tabulates 16 comparisons of felt versus expressed emotions in music published in the decade 2003–2012 consisting of 19 studies/experiments and provides some theoretical perspectives. The key findings were that (1) IL rating was frequently rated statistically the same or lower than the corresponding EL rating (e.g., lower felt happiness rating compared to the apparent happiness of the music), and that (2) self-select and preferred music had a smaller gap across the emotion loci than experimenter-selected and disliked music. These key findings were explained by an “inhibited” emotional contagion mechanism, where the otherwise matching felt emotion may have been attenuated by some other factor such as social context. Matching between EL and IL for loved and self-selected pieces was explained by the activation of “contagion” circuits. Physiological arousal, personality and age, as well as musical features (tempo, mode, putative emotions) also influenced perceived and felt emotion distinctions. A variety of data collection formats were identified, but mostly using rating items. In conclusion, a more systematic use of terminology appears desirable. Two broad categories, namely matched and unmatched, are proposed as being sufficient to capture the relationships between EL and IL, instead of four categories as suggested by Gabrielsson.</p>
</abstract>
<kwd-group>
<kwd>expressed and felt emotion in music</kwd>
<kwd>emotion locus</kwd>
<kwd>contagion</kwd>
<kwd>normative dissociation</kwd>
<kwd>contrast effect</kwd>
<kwd>affect valence</kwd>
<kwd>literature review</kwd>
</kwd-group>
<counts>
<fig-count count="1"></fig-count>
<table-count count="2"></table-count>
<equation-count count="0"></equation-count>
<ref-count count="181"></ref-count>
<page-count count="18"></page-count>
<word-count count="18273"></word-count>
</counts>
</article-meta>
</front>
<body>
<p>The distinction between emotion felt by a listener (internal locus of emotion) and emotion expressed by a piece of music (external locus of emotion) has become a firmly established part of research agenda of music psychologists in the last decade. Since the seminal work of Gabrielsson (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B35">2002</xref>
) we have seen evidence that emotion felt in response to music (e.g., “the music makes me feel happy”) is sometimes the same as (“the music is happy”) and sometimes different from (“the music is sad”) the emotion expressed by the music—so called “perceived emotion.” This paper aims to push the debate further by examining the data in the literature published in the decade after Gabrielsson (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B35">2002</xref>
) and explaining why the emotions between the two loci (felt vs. expressed) are sometimes systematically different and sometimes the same. The paper is structured as follows. First, (1) the inclusion criteria and limitations of the review are laid out, (2) some early music psychology research related to emotion locus is presented including an overview of Gabrielsson's paper, followed by (3) a collation of the target literature of this review. Then, (4) the theoretical implications of the literature are discussed, with (5) a proposed reworking of Gabrielsson's locus relationships to accommodate a developed understanding, and to highlight some of the key research questions emerging in the field.</p>
<sec>
<title>Inclusion criteria and limitations of this review</title>
<sec>
<title>Inclusion criteria</title>
<p>The inclusion criteria for the research tabulated for the review are as follows: (1) studies which made a direct comparison between external locus emotion and internal locus emotion in connection with music listening; (2) studies which use the same response regime for both external locus and internal responses; and (3) studies appearing in peer-reviewed journals in the 10-year period of 2003–2012 (that is, the decade since Gabrielsson's publication). Nineteen studies met all three criteria. The reason for excluding studies that have some connection with emotion locus and music are now explained.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Limitation 1: expressed and felt emotion data compatibility</title>
<p>When discussion of locus is presented in the research literature on emotion in music, it is most frequently an acknowledgment that a locus distinction exists, but that the study limits the investigation to one locus or the other (internal or external), without comparing both. Sometimes the data in each locus are not directly comparable (e.g., a rating of felt emotion, but a categorical,
<italic>a priori</italic>
label for the emotion expressed by the stimulus), as will be discussed below. Such studies were not included in the tabulated literature of this review.</p>
<p>Thus, studies that could allow comparison of locus by rating of expressed emotion
<italic>a priori</italic>
(e.g., by a panel of experts, as in numerous mood induction studies), or comparison of physiological measures with one locus or the other (e.g., Krumhansl,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B88">1997</xref>
; Grewe et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B47">2007</xref>
; Nagel et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B106">2007</xref>
; for a critical review, see Konečni,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B82">2008</xref>
; Grewe et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B46">2009</xref>
, pp. 263–265) were not included. The bulk of the
<italic>a priori</italic>
rated expressed locus data are found in the well-established music mood induction literature
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="fn0001">
<sup>1</sup>
</xref>
.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Limitation 2: terminology to describe the felt/expressed distinction</title>
<p>As discussed below, the terminology to describe locus of emotion is varied, making online keyword searching alone inappropriate for locating articles that fulfill the inclusion criteria
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="fn0002">
<sup>2</sup>
</xref>
. Instead, a number of sources and databases were consulted—Google Scholar, PsycINFO, Scopus, and Web of Knowledge, as well as papers that cited Gabrielsson's (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B35">2002</xref>
) paper.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Limitation 3: emotion locus research in non-music research</title>
<p>Although an interest in comparing locus in other fields of research can be found—for example, in social reception (e.g., Jakobs et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B61">2001</xref>
; Bombari et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B8a">2013</xref>
), cross-modal (e.g., Calder et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B13">2000</xref>
), film (e.g., Matsumoto and Kupperbusch,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B100">2001</xref>
; Wang and Cheong,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B167">2006</xref>
; Werner et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B169">2007</xref>
), literature (e.g., Oatley,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B108">1995</xref>
; Green,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B45">2004</xref>
; Miall,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B104">2011</xref>
), business (e.g., Pugh,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B117">2001</xref>
), and facial expression of emotion (e.g., Dimberg et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B27">2000</xref>
; Barthomeuf et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B4">2012</xref>
; Sato et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B134">2013</xref>
) research—it appears that most interest in the direct comparison of internal with external loci of emotion via empirical means is rooted in music perception research, making the transference of findings from other sub-disciplines limited at this point in time. However, some relevant issues from non-music studies are mentioned in this review.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Limitation 4: philosophical issues and mood induction</title>
<p>The review is limited to empirical data from music psychology research in which data from each emotion locus (felt by listener and expressed by music) are gathered and compared. It should be noted that music psychology has been influenced by ideas about emotion locus that were primarily in the realm of philosophy and aesthetics. It is those scholars who introduced terms of emotivism and cognitivism, often quoted in the music psychology literature (Baumgartner,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B5">1992</xref>
; Goldman,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B43">1995</xref>
; Scherer and Zentner,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B138">2001</xref>
; Rickard,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B123">2004</xref>
; Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B141">2007a</xref>
; Konečni,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B82">2008</xref>
; Konečni et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B83">2008</xref>
; Roy et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B127">2008</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B126">2009</xref>
; Coutinho and Cangelosi,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B21">2009</xref>
; Lundqvist et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B95">2009</xref>
; Garrido and Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B38">2011b</xref>
; Hoeckner et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B54">2011</xref>
; Panagiotidi and Samartzi,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B109">2012</xref>
). However, I do not focus on these philosophical writings not just because they fall outside the “empirical research” gamut, but because they generally have a different focus from that of music psychology: where music psychologists seek to understand the nature and relationship between emotion loci in music, philosophers of aesthetics are frequently concerned with identifying the
<italic>value</italic>
of music that evokes emotion (the emotivist perspective) vs. expresses emotion (the cognitivist perspective). For example, Scruton (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B149">1983</xref>
) writes, “To describe a piece of music as expressive of melancholy is to give reason for listening to it; to describe it as arousing of evoking melancholy is to give a reason for avoiding it” (p. 49), and (Kivy,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B75">1989</xref>
) “one substantial group of listeners who report that sad music makes them sad are simply (and understandably) mistaken” (p. 163). This
<italic>value</italic>
perspective has relevance in music psychology, particularly when value is operationalized as a variable such as liking (some empirical research has provided evidence supporting the spirit of the just cited statements—see, e.g., Konečni et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B83">2008</xref>
; Hunter et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B56">2010</xref>
; Vuoskoski and Eerola,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B165">2012</xref>
); however, the literature review is limited to
<italic>understanding the relationships</italic>
between emotion loci from the perspective of listener self-reports (rather than judging the value of the music). Outside such circumstances, the more purely philosophical research will be used to inform the reviewed material, rather than be part of it. Hence, a limitation of the present review will be to examine exclusively music-psychology literature (for excellent aesthetician accounts, see Radford,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B118">1989</xref>
; Kivy,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B76">1990</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B77">1999</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B78">2002</xref>
; Davies,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B23">1994</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B24">2003</xref>
; Robinson,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B124">1994</xref>
).</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Limitation 5: within-locus distinctions</title>
<p>On a related matter, this review will not attempt to separate “within emotion locus” distinctions, such as the internal locus distinction between inducing mood vs. feeling an emotion (e.g., Weld,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B168">1912</xref>
; Diener et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B26">1995</xref>
; Lychner,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B96">1998</xref>
; Gray et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B44">2001</xref>
; Sloboda and Juslin,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B155">2010</xref>
). Another within-internal-locus distinction is between “truly,” internally felt emotion vs. the emotion one displays in an intrapersonal, social, or work setting. This kind of distinction is covered in non-music research on emotional regulation, which includes protective buffering (Langer et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B90">2007</xref>
; Manne et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B98">2007</xref>
), display rules (Ekman,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B30a">1972</xref>
; Matsumoto,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B100a">1990</xref>
), and emotional dissonance and emotional labor (Bono and Vey,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B9">2005</xref>
; Mann,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B97">2005</xref>
; Bakker and Heuven,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B2">2006</xref>
). Such distinctions within internal locus are not reported here because they have not been cited in empirical music perception investigations that meet the inclusion criteria, and do not at first
<italic>seem</italic>
to be of relevance because one would imagine that knowing how to behave in front of a piece of music is not relevant in the way that knowing how to behave in front of other people is relevant. That is, it seems unlikely that we would need to distinguish between how we actually feel and the felt emotion that we display (for example, through facial, bodily or written/typed response) in the study of emotion in music. Although this internal locus distinction is not covered in the empirical data of the literature reviewed, it will be relevant in future research (for a discussion in non-music contexts, see Gross et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B48">2000</xref>
).</p>
<p>Within-external locus distinctions, such as whether music is
<italic>portraying</italic>
emotion,
<italic>expressing</italic>
emotion,
<italic>trying</italic>
to portray/express emotion, or “is” emotional (the music
<italic>is</italic>
sad), are also not covered in this review, again because empirical data investigating these distinctions are rare and mostly concerned with semantics. Instead, in this review, the terminology used to discriminate external and internal locus (rather than its semantic/linguistic utility) is reported (see subsection “Locus Terminology”, below), with the exception of one study that is included (Van Zijl and Sloboda,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B161">2011</xref>
) because it raises the matter of the “performer” locus of emotion, which is external from the listener perspective unless the listener
<italic>is</italic>
the performer. Again, future empirical studies will be needed to examine the various external locus possibilities, specifically the emotion that the composer(s), performer(s) or other (perhaps imaginary) listener(s) are thought to be experiencing (according to the perceiver), and whether a further distinction between these other people and the music should be made when considering external locus. For example, some recent research has considered emotion ratings that others would make about a piece of music as a way of managing possible bias in external locus response. That is, instead of being asked what emotion they believe the music expresses, a participant is asked, “How would normal people feel when listening to this musical stimulus?” (Kawakami et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B72">2013</xref>
).</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Previous psychological accounts leading up to gabrielsson's publication</title>
<p>Pre-review period (before 2003) accounts by music psychologists demonstrating an awareness of the distinction between felt and expressed emotion are frequent (e.g., Weld,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B168">1912</xref>
; Valentine,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B160">1962</xref>
, p. 10; Swanwick,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B156">1975</xref>
; Payne,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B111">1980</xref>
; Thayer,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B158">1986</xref>
; Gaver and Mandler,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B41">1987</xref>
; Frances,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B33">1988</xref>
, p. 243; Sloboda,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B153">1992</xref>
; Scherer and Zentner,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B138">2001</xref>
; for a discussion of key pre-review period studies, of course see Gabrielsson (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B35">2002</xref>
) but in particular pp. 124–127 and pp. 132–133), but almost none of these explicitly compare locus responses empirically (exceptions among which are Lee,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B93">1932</xref>
; Collins,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B18">1989</xref>
; Zentner,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B173a">2000</xref>
).</p>
<p>From a historical point of view, and from a theory-building perspective, it is worth dwelling for a moment on the pioneering study by Lee (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B93">1932</xref>
). A questionnaire was developed to investigate responses to music made by music lovers, to tease out the role of emotion (among other things) reported by participants, and to identify any skepticism about whether music was capable of stimulating human expression and emotion. Over 100 responses were collected, spanning a period of more than 25 years. One part of the analysis was to classify “listeners” who had aesthetic interest in music and “hearers” who were more focused on human-emotional interests in music listening. Lee asked explicitly about locus, but upon analysis she noticed that participants treated the three options of the questions as “one of three” rather than two of three (A or B—internal locus, and then C—external locus):
<list list-type="alpha-upper">
<list-item>
<p>Does music put you into emotional conditions (or moods) different from the one you happen to be in? or</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>Does it merely intensify already existing moods?</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>Do you merely recognize, without participating, that music represents varieties of human emotion and mood? (p. 202)</p>
</list-item>
</list>
</p>
<p>Lee's openness and regret are explicit as she acknowledges:
<disp-quote>
<p>What I wanted to know was whether the Answerer merely recognised that a given piece of music had (i.e., might be described as having) a given emotional character, e.g., was sad or cheerful, or whether hearing that piece of music made him feel sad or cheerful when he had not been so before? This fairly simple query, clearly worded as “do you merely recognize without participation that music
<italic>represents</italic>
varieties of human emotion and mood?” was the real subject under examination and ought therefore to have been put first and foremost. Instead of that, and from a mistaken hope of additional clearness, it was put after the queries (intended to be supplementary to it) whether the Answerer's already existing mood could be altered or whether that mood was merely intensified by the emotion which was not merely recognized as characterizing the music, but actually participated in when hearing that music. As a result of this strategical blunder, the majority of Answerers did not notice the main question of Participation versus Recognition. (p. 203, italics in original)</p>
</disp-quote>
</p>
<p>Nevertheless, Lee identified participants who explicitly reported the link between felt and expressed emotion; for example, two participants, Bob and Lewis, respectively, wrote:
<disp-quote>
<p>“Music generally substitutes new moods and emotions: if the emotion is tragic or tender, it seems that my mood becomes tragic or tender.” (This is recognition producing a sort of sympathetic imitation.)</p>
<p>[…]</p>
<p>“It is not that the music expressed one's own feelings, but that the feelings or mood which the music expresses awaken these very feelings in oneself. Music never intensifies existing feelings, it either awakens feelings which I haven't got or merely represents them.” (p. 204, Lee's annotation of Bob's comment is shown in the parenthetical)</p>
</disp-quote>
</p>
<p>Hence, Lee's work appears to be the first explicit attempt in English language music psychology research to collect empirical data on the distinction between locus of emotion, a pioneering effort that has received regrettably little attention in subsequent research. Her explanation of “sympathetic imitation” will be highly relevant in the theory development of this review. Aside from the data and analysis reported by Lee, it was not until 2002 when Gabrielsson acknowledged in explicit terms the possible relationships between loci (Gabrielsson is also an important researcher, as was Lee, in compiling a large body of data on individuals' self-reported aesthetic/emotional reactions to music: Gabrielsson,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B36">2011</xref>
).</p>
<p>Gabrielsson's (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B35">2002</xref>
) publication
<italic>was</italic>
noticed and paved the way for a new era of research, by laying out the possible relationships between felt and expressed emotions. Gabrielsson provided a cautious, broad definition of emotion, which is used as a starting point in the present review: “Not to get trapped in … terminological confusion already from the beginning, I will use ‘emotion’ and ‘feeling’ in a generic and broad-minded sense, often involving cognitive components; “mood” and “affect” will be used when employed by authors referred to in the text” (p. 123–124). He then presented a detailed argument explaining that participants can only logically make distinctions between felt and perceived emotion in music through verbal report, as distinct from, say, physiological measurement.</p>
<p>Gabrielsson discusses some of the reasons for the absence of direct emotion locus comparisons in music perception. For example, he recounts the belief that there were different listener types, some who focused on the music, and others who focused on their own feelings when listening to music (Lee, discussed above, provides a case in point). Music was seen by many researchers and lay people as “an object for perception and reflection” and the emotional response as a listener's reaction. As a result, Gabrielsson concluded that the distinction between emotion loci in music “is not always clearly observed, neither in everyday conversation about emotions, nor in scientific papers” (Gabrielsson,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B35">2002</xref>
, p. 124). Another reason for the neglect is the influence of aestheticians upon music psychologists, as mentioned in Limitation 4, above, where the interest was in whether music that expresses emotion should be more valued than music that evokes emotion in the listener. This perspective suggests that each locus has a distinct aesthetic function, providing one reason why researchers have been distracted from the psychological
<italic>relationships</italic>
between the emotion loci.</p>
<p>According to Gabrielsson, in broad terms, felt and expressed emotions could be related through a “positive” relationship (felt emotion is the same as expressed emotion—feeling sad when hearing sad music), or it could be “negative” (i.e., opposite, e.g., feeling angry when hearing happy music). Furthermore, felt and expressed emotions could exhibit “no systematic relationship” (e.g., feeling various emotions when hearing calm music), or have no relationship at all (such as feeling no emotion, or identifying no emotion in the music). Gabrielsson traces back the presence of positive relationships to the ancient Greeks where music was thought to be able to directly affect the listener, in a manner similar to more contemporary research on mood management and regulation theory (Knobloch and Zillmann,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B79">2002</xref>
; Saarikallio and Erkkilä,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B132">2007</xref>
; Wilhelm et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B170">2013</xref>
), through a process that would later be called contagion by Juslin and Västfjäll (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B70">2008</xref>
). That is, contagion explains how music can express an emotion that then “infects” its listener.</p>
<p>Opposite relationships, Gabrielsson explains, and is confirmed by subsequent research, are more idiosyncratic (e.g., when an event of a contrary emotion happens in one's life that becomes associated with music heard at the same time), or a sad piece of music makes the listener happy because it is relaxing, cathartic and pleasurable (Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B139">1996</xref>
; Matsumoto,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B101">2002</xref>
; Huron,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B57">2011</xref>
; Vuoskoski and Eerola,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B165">2012</xref>
; Vuoskoski et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B166">2012</xref>
; Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B146">2013</xref>
), or the listener, perhaps in attempting to improve mood, actually makes their mood worse, perhaps due to some complicating circumstances such as a mood disorder (Garrido and Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B38">2011b</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B40">2013</xref>
).</p>
<p>In the case of no systematic relationship, the listener might not be affected by the music at all, but be able to observe the music as expressing some emotion—Gabrielsson characterizes this with the “analytic listener”—or that different emotions are evoked in the listener at different occasions—the “zero correlation” relationship. The final category, of “no relationship at all,” is characterized by the absence or unreliable presence of emotions in one or both loci, such as the internal locus (human) emotions as identified by Scherer and Zentner (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B138">2001</xref>
) that music cannot express with reasonable agreement, which is likely to include gratitude, fascination, disgust, jealousy, safety, warmth and humility. Gabrielsson's relationships are not intended as clear-cut, and polychotomous: The differentiation between internal and external locus can be blurred, and the experience felt vs. the perceived emotion in the music may not even be distinguishable or meaningful to some. Gabrielsson writes, “We may think of them as opposite extremes on a continuum from ‘pure’ emotion-free perception at the one end to intense emotional reaction at the other end. Rather than being at any of these extremes, in most situations listeners are probably somewhere along this continuum, depending on many circumstances” (p. 124).</p>
<p>In the years following Gabrielsson's influential paper, research on explicitly collected self-report felt and expressed emotion in music grew, and 16 peer-reviewed publications that met the inclusion criteria were located. Some of the publications reported more than one study that was concerned with emotion locus and music, and two reported locus data from previously published sources (Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B141">2007a</xref>
; Ilie and Thompson,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B60">2011</xref>
), bringing the total number of included, unique data sets (studies) comparing emotion loci in music to 19. Fifteen of the studies were experimental (Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B141">2007a</xref>
is not added to this count because it is a reanalysis of Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B142">2007b</xref>
), three were survey based (no music played), and one was a qualitative study (Van Zijl and Sloboda,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B161">2011</xref>
).</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Review of the contemporary literature: methodological issues</title>
<p>A summary of the included studies is tabulated in Supplementary Table
<xref ref-type="supplementary-material" rid="SM1">1</xref>
. The reviewed papers are discussed according to terminologies and methodologies, followed by key results.</p>
<sec>
<title>Locus terminology</title>
<p>The labels used to denote internal and external locus varied considerably across the tabulated studies. Naming the locus
<italic>variable</italic>
produced several alternatives: instruction condition (Vieillard et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B163">2008</xref>
: “instructed to report own emotion or instructed to describe the music”), “response” (Hunter et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B56">2010</xref>
), “mode” (Dibben,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">2004</xref>
), “modality” (Zentner et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B173">2008</xref>
), “type of rating” (Ali and Peynircioǧlu,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B1">2010</xref>
), “point of view” (Kallinen and Ravaja,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B71">2006</xref>
), and “locus” (Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B142">2007b</xref>
).</p>
<p>The labeling of each of the two levels of the variable demonstrates a rich variety of ways of understanding the phenomena in question. Lee's pairing (“participate” for internal locus and “recognize” for external) is absent in all the contemporary literature reviewed, although Vieillard et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B163">2008</xref>
) use the term “recognize” in their recognized-experienced level labels. Collins (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B18">1989</xref>
)—not part of the tabulated review—provides a rather detailed distinction between internal and external loci: “own emotional response—emotional content of the music” and “describe music—describe human emotion,” as do Kallinen and Ravaja in asking their participants to respond to the music from two different “points of view”:
<disp-quote>
<p>Participants were first asked to evaluate the emotions the music aroused in them during listening (i.e., emotion felt; “How did you feel when you listened [to] the music?”), and second, evaluate the emotional quality of the music regardless of the experiences it aroused in them (i.e., emotion perceived; “What is the [more objective] emotional nature of music, regardless of your personal reactions to it?”). (Kallinen and Ravaja,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B71">2006</xref>
, p. 200)</p>
</disp-quote>
</p>
<p>Other variants were expressed-felt (Dibben,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">2004</xref>
); perceiving-feeling (Hunter et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B56">2010</xref>
); expressed-induced (Zentner et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B173">2008</xref>
); perception-induction; perceive-experience (Juslin and Laukka,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B67">2004</xref>
); conveyed-elicited (Ali and Peynircioǧlu,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B1">2010</xref>
); and participant believed the composer was intending to convey—felt in response to the musical excerpt (Salimpoor et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B133">2009</xref>
). Van Zijl and Sloboda (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B161">2011</xref>
) refer to musical emotion—own emotion. This study is interesting from the perspective that it tracks the responses of the listeners who are also the performers of the music. Such an approach may be incorporated into the “locus” nomenclature used by Schubert, by extending the meaning of external locus beyond the “perspective” of the listener. That is, if the listener is asked to judge the emotion that the performer is experiencing while playing, or the emotion the composer was experiencing while composing, or even the emotion experienced by any other listener (or an imagined agent), the locus would still be external, but focussed on another person, rather than on the music (a detailed investigation of the appreciation of the performer/composer emotion is beyond the scope of this review, but see, for example, Juslin,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B63">2000</xref>
; Juslin et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B66">2001</xref>
; Kreutz et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B87">2008b</xref>
).</p>
<p>In summary, no single pair of designations for each locus level was used consistently across the studies. This reflects a richness in the descriptions, subtle distinctions, but also lack of systematic classification. The recommendation of this review is to label two levels of the variable as deemed appropriate in each study: felt, induced (although this may be related to mood according to some researchers), evoked, internal locus vs. expressed, portrayed, “in the music,” and external locus. “Perceived” is frequently used to describe external locus, however, the term could be confused because a participant can perceive many things, including their own feelings in some cases (Konečni,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B82">2008</xref>
). Context and grammar is important here: to make the locus distinction clear from the
<italic>listener's</italic>
perspective, and when the listener is the subject of the sentence, she or he feels an emotion, but
<italic>perceives</italic>
it (rather than expresses it) in the music—“express” does not designate external locus when the listener is the subject of the sentence (“the listener expresses happiness”). When the
<italic>music</italic>
is the subject of the sentence,
<italic>expressed</italic>
emotion is the external locus designation (obviously not “perceived,” because music is not an agent that can literally perceive).</p>
<p>Using the term “locus” to describe the variable frees up labels that are commonly used to describe other variables, such as type and mode. “Perspective” is another possible term that could be used to describe the variable but was not cited with any regularity in the tabulated literature.</p>
<p>Table
<xref ref-type="table" rid="T1">1</xref>
attempts to organize all of the terms used to describe each of the two levels of loci located in the literature. It should be understood, then, that the unambiguous use of the terminology depends on the explicit and implied grammatical subject (that is, if stated from the perspective of the listener or of the music). Any of the internal-external pairings are satisfactory but need to have the subject (music or listener) explicitly stated or made clear. The term “communicates” is listed under internal locus from the music perspective because it suggests a context: “the music communicates to me.” But since the term “communication” refers to the
<italic>transmission</italic>
of information from one source to another, it may lead to some ambiguity (e.g., “the music is communicating an emotion”—external locus?).</p>
<table-wrap id="T1" position="float">
<label>Table 1</label>
<caption>
<p>
<bold>Terminology of emotion locus levels by grammatical subject (perspective)</bold>
.</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>Locus</bold>
</th>
<th align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>Listener perspective</bold>
</th>
<th align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>Music perspective</bold>
</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Internal</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Felt, experienced, self, own, reactivity</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Conveys, induces, evokes, arouses, elicits, (communicates)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">External</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Perceived, recognized, sensed, noticed</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Expresses, portrays, depicts, conveys, sounds, describes, has character, “is”</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</table-wrap>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Participants</title>
<p>All included experimental studies in this review had over 25 participants in each experiment, with the Konečni et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B83">2008</xref>
) study having 144 participants. The two questionnaire-based studies had 262 (Zentner et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B173">2008</xref>
) and 141 (Juslin and Laukka,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B67">2004</xref>
) participants. The open-ended study of the musicians' perspectives used 8 performers covering a variety of musical instruments (Van Zijl and Sloboda,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B161">2011</xref>
). Participants had a range of ages across studies, with one study examining results of older participants separately (Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B142">2007b</xref>
; Experiment 3). A wide range of musical experiences were reported, though none of the studies treated musical experience (e.g., high vs. low) as an experimental variable.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Types of music used</title>
<p>The bulk of music used in the studies tabulated comes from the common practice period (CPP) of Western art music, which is sometimes referred to as “classical” music. Twelve publications using experimenter-selected pieces used exclusively such music. Classical music, and in particular music from the romantic art music period, is considered particularly good for expressing and evoking emotions (Romantic(ism),
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B125">2013</xref>
).</p>
<p>Since studies by Panksepp (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B110">1995</xref>
), Blood and Zatorre (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B8">2001</xref>
), and Rickard (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B123">2004</xref>
), it has become evident that another effective way to evoke strong emotional responses is to use music that the participant, rather than the experimenter, selects. The Rickard study comes near the halfway mark of the review sample chronology, and it is after this date that we start to see self-selected pieces being used for locus of emotion in music studies. The locus studies reviewed first commenced using self-selected music from 2007 (Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B141">2007a</xref>
). Salimpoor et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B133">2009</xref>
) directly compared participant-selected and experimenter-selected pieces in their study.</p>
<p>Some research deliberately selected music that is unfamiliar. In the two experiments investigating emotion locus by Dibben (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">2004</xref>
), she verified that the music was not familiar to any of the participants. Hunter et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B56">2010</xref>
) also selected unfamiliar music by Bach—eight selections—which were manipulated by tempo and mode to generate the 30 stimuli presented via MIDI playback. Thus, music of the common practice period presented a great range of choices for a Western-enculturated participant. Vieillard et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B163">2008</xref>
) is the only study in the tabulated literature where specially composed music was created for the purpose of exerting a high degree of control over the stimuli. This was balanced with some more familiar film music excerpts, used at the beginning of the procedure. The study by Van Zijl and Sloboda (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B161">2011</xref>
) used self-selected pieces, but these were for the purpose of learning to play—so that participants would be getting to know their stimuli in a highly intimate way.</p>
<p>Two publications reviewed included no explicit musical stimuli (Juslin and Laukka,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B67">2004</xref>
; Zentner et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B173">2008</xref>
) because data about music in general were collected via questionnaire. The questionnaires provided critical data directly addressing the question of locus. In this respect, they fulfilled the inclusion criteria of this literature review because the participants were asked to think about music in general, or a favorite piece (see also Evans and Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B32">2008</xref>
) or genre of music. In addition, Juslin and Laukka (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B67">2004</xref>
) was one of the first published studies (along with Dibben) to directly address the question of locus in the post-Gabrielsson period.</p>
<p>When musical examples were selected, they were usually chosen on the basis of which emotion they would express or evoke, with the intention commonly being to produce a range of emotions, whether based on the theory of basic emotions—such as “joy, sadness, anger, and fear” (Kallinen and Ravaja,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B71">2006</xref>
) or a sample from each quadrant of an emotion space (e.g., Dibben,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">2004</xref>
; see also Collins,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B18">1989</xref>
, though not tabulated for the present review). For the experimental studies, the typical length of the excerpts used ranged from 1 to 3 minutes. Ali and Peynircioǧlu (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B1">2010</xref>
) used stimuli thought to be unfamiliar, each lasting approximately 20 seconds, but in one condition, participants were familiarized by listening to an excerpt five times in succession. In one of the two Ilie and Thompson (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B59">2006</xref>
) studies the stimuli used had an average duration of 6 seconds.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Design and procedures</title>
<p>Many of the tabulated studies ask the participant to perform tasks in a laboratory setting and often in groups. However, the group-listening laboratory setting does not necessarily reflect the typical day-to-day listening experience or environment (for a discussion of the matter of naturalistic vs. experimental research, see Mitchell,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B104a">2012</xref>
). Since the emerging ubiquity of personal, private online computer facilities, tablets and mobile phones (Krause and Hargreaves,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B84">2012</xref>
), it has become possible to collect sophisticated data outside the laboratory and in a private, individual environment (e.g., see Reid et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B120">2009</xref>
). Such technological advances will allow locus data to be collected in a variety of settings. For example, one of the studies outside the review epoch (Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B146">2013</xref>
) used an online survey to collect locus data, requesting participants to use YouTube or some other online streaming resource to listen to their self-selected pieces, and provide information about the piece and the URL (that is, the participants pasted the link they used to access the selected music to allow later inspection by the researcher). The survey could be completed in private. Further developments of this approach will be able to better ascertain the reliability and validity of collecting data in such a way, as compared to group settings with a researcher present and with predetermined ordering of stimulus presentation.</p>
<p>Juslin and Laukka (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B67">2004</xref>
) used a survey with no musical stimuli to gather data on a range of issues regarding musical experiences and beliefs, including emotion perception and emotion induction. The survey included questions about the utility of certain words for describing emotion felt and emotion expressed. This kind of task is easy to administer and produces a rich source of data about emotion locus. Zentner et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B173">2008</xref>
) also used a survey-based approach with a direct rating for each of the two loci for a series of 146 “feeling terms” for a variety of musical styles. Participants first rated the terms as emotions felt for a selected (favorite) musical style, and then again for the emotion perceived in that style.</p>
<p>Indeed, one concern in the literature is the timing of the locus tasks. Should they both be completed immediately after hearing the musical stimulation, one after the other, or should only one be completed (internal only
<italic>or</italic>
external only) in case one rating influences the rating of the other locus? Completing the two rating items in immediate sequence has the advantage of requiring only a single pass of the music stimulus to obtain both ratings, but has a drawback if the participant (consciously or otherwise) responds to the second rating item under the influence of the first, through what is known as a “contrast” effect (Cacioppo and Gardner,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B11">1999</xref>
; Schwarz and Strack,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B148">1999</xref>
; Cheng,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B17">2004</xref>
), where the second response is made relative to the first, while the first rating item response is (probably) not. For example, a high rating of felt emotion might be exaggerated (rated even higher) if the external locus rating is made immediately before but is also rated as high.</p>
<p>Kallinen and Ravaja (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B71">2006</xref>
) asked participants to rate felt emotion first so as not to dilute the felt emotion caused by the delay of rating expressed emotion first. This ordering was used in several other tabulated experiments (e.g., Dibben,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">2004</xref>
; Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B142">2007b</xref>
, Study 2 and 3; Evans and Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B32">2008</xref>
). However, Konečni et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B83">2008</xref>
) argued that by rating external locus first (i.e., the emotion in the music), the participant will be more cognizant in distinguishing their own internal locus response and not confuse it with the expressed emotion. They resolved the matter by counterbalancing—half the participants made their internal locus rating first, while the other half made their external locus rating first. Dibben (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">2004</xref>
; Experiment 2), Vieillard et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B163">2008</xref>
), and Ali and Peynircioǧlu (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B1">2010</xref>
) each had one internal locus group and another external locus group, each group completing questions for one locus only as a between-subjects design. Dibben (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">2004</xref>
) concluded that when participants make a judgment in one locus alone, they do not differentiate between emotion and locus as well as they do when loci are presented together (p. 111), suggesting that some “contrasting” of rating items may be methodologically beneficial, as small differences are amplified. Schubert (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B142">2007b</xref>
) findings were different but led to a similar conclusion: a contrast effect was not observed, and in fact, locus tasks performed together produced some “interference” (Gabrielsson,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B35">2002</xref>
, p. 127; Dibben,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">2004</xref>
, p. 95) leading to a more blended response, compared to performing one locus task at a time. But the same
<italic>trend</italic>
in response was noted when compared to the “locus-separate” condition (recording response to one locus only, and then the other on a second hearing of the stimulus), leading Schubert to conclude that responding to both loci in sequence at the same time had efficiency (almost halving experiment time, or “doubling” the data pool, and as a result increasing statistical power), perhaps compensating for the possible disadvantages. The recommendation of this review, then, is that it is more efficient to collect both locus ratings together, but to counterbalance the order of loci questions, as did Konečni et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B83">2008</xref>
) when possible. Most of the tabulated studies, when presenting the loci responses together, requested internal locus rating first. It is not always clear in the literature whether participants could change their answers, though it seems that no effort was made to prevent or withhold the option of checking or changing responses. Explicit investigation of this counterbalancing is recommended.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Response format</title>
<p>Fifteen of the nineteen tabulated studies were defined here as “experimental,” and for several of those, as well as the three survey studies (see Supplementary Table
<xref ref-type="supplementary-material" rid="SM1">1</xref>
, rows 3, 10, and 12), a wide range of emotions were presented as entities to be rated by the participant after listening to an extract of music, once for internal and once for external locus. This subsection therefore examines these item-rating response formats according to (1) the emotions rated, (2) the number of steps available on each rating item, (3) the number of items rated in each locus for each piece of music, and (4) unipolar versus bipolar item labeling issues.</p>
<sec>
<title>Emotions rated</title>
<p>The most frequently used pole labels (whether rating item was bipolar or unipolar) were happy (including happiness and very-happy) and sad (including sadness and very sad), both used in at least
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="fn0003">
<sup>3</sup>
</xref>
eight studies (Supplementary Table
<xref ref-type="supplementary-material" rid="SM1">1</xref>
, rows 1, 2, 4, 9, 11, 13, 14, and 16. Note: All subsequent parenthetical references to row numbers in this subsection refer to this table, with focus on the Measure column). In (at least) five studies an item for rating happiness and another for rating sadness were presented (rows 1, 2, 11, 14, and 16), but on three occasions terms related to happy and sad were presented together at the opposite poles of a single, bipolar rating item (rows 4, 9, and 13), such as very happy to very sad, as used by Konečni et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B83">2008</xref>
, row 9). Wording related to “arousal/aroused” was located in four studies (rows 4, 5, 13, and 18—one of which had poles labeled “energetic/peppy” and “bored/vegetated:” Kallinen and Ravaja,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B71">2006</xref>
, row 4). If we group together the remaining labels according to similarity of meanings, the next most frequently used rating item labels in the tabulated literature are five occurrences related to anger (“angry”, “fear”, and “scary”
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="fn0004">
<sup>4</sup>
</xref>
: rows 1, 2, 4, 11, and 16) and four related to calm (“calmness”, “relax”, and “peacefulness”: rows 4, 11, 16, and 18).</p>
<p>Typically, the rating items were combined and manipulated to create dependent variables for statistical analysis and hypothesis testing. This transformation process is summarized in the Measure column at “DV” for each of the relevant tabulated studies in Supplementary Table
<xref ref-type="supplementary-material" rid="SM1">1</xref>
. The most frequently used labels for dependent variables were related to “valence” (positive, negative, and/or valence: rows 1, 2, 4, and 18) and “arousal” (rows 4, 8, 13, and 18), although untransformed rating items also received these labels in some studies (e.g., row 5). Thus, the emotion constructs of valence and arousal have endured as methods of operationalizing emotion locus response.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Number of steps per rating item</title>
<p>When making a response via a rating item, participants are given a number of graded steps (points) along a continuum from which they are to make a single selection. The range of the available steps per rating item across the tabulated studies was from 4 to 13, with 5-point rating items used most frequent (seven studies, rows 1, 2, 4, 10, 14, 16, and 18). Researchers have had to balance the coarseness of completing rating items with fewer points with the greater difficulty for the participant in making a selection when a larger number of steps are offered (Alwin,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B1a">1997</xref>
; Viswanathan et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B164a">2004</xref>
; Dawes,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B24a">2008</xref>
). While there is no “magic number” of rating scale steps to use, in the tabulated literature the number of steps is generally informed by precedence (e.g., using a rating item previously published) and the number of items to be rated—a large number of items is usually associated with a smaller number of steps per item, as discussed next.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Number of emotions rated</title>
<p>Konečni et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B83">2008</xref>
, row 9) deliberately used a single rating item for each stimulus/locus combination, arguing that a large number of rating items might be unrealistic to recall and impractical to complete in response to an excerpt of music (in accord with Viswanathan et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B164a">2004</xref>
). Typically 4–6 emotion item ratings were requested per piece, per locus across the tabulated studies. In the open-ended responses, Van Zijl and Sloboda (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B161">2011</xref>
, row 19) asked performers explicitly about emotions they felt when preparing a piece and the emotions that the music was expressing, meaning that there was no explicit limit to the number of emotions that could be reported. Two of the survey studies (Juslin and Laukka,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B67">2004</xref>
; Zentner et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B173">2008</xref>
, rows 3 and 12) requested participants to rate how frequently a word from a list of emotion terms was appropriate to describe music, on a four step rating item ranging from never to always. The checklist approach pioneered by Hevner (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B52">1936</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B53">1937</xref>
), which lists a relatively large number of emotion words from which the participant can select, is completely absent in the experimental studies tabulated, suggesting utility and ease of statistical analysis of the highly prevalent rating items.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Unipolar vs. bipolar rating items</title>
<p>In the experimental studies, response items were most frequently presented in a unipolar format (9 studies: rows 1, 2, 8, 11, 13, 14, 15, 16, and 18), whereas bipolar labels were used for rating items in six studies (rows 4, 5, 6, 9, 13, and 15). Dependent variables were generated from combinations of the rated items in 9 studies, of which 4 were unipolar (rows 1, 2, 11, and 16) and 4 were bipolar (rows 4, 9, 15, and 18), with one study generating an angular variable from the combination of arousal and valence ratings (row 8). Some researchers converted unipolar item ratings into bipolar dependent variables, such as Ilie and Thompson (row 18), who took differences between pairs of unipolar item ratings (e.g., “pleasant” and “unpleasant”) to generate the dependent variable score (e.g., “valence”). That study is also interesting from the point of view that it is one of the few to apply a more contemporary model of emotion dimensions to the data, with energy-arousal and tension-arousal scores generated, instead of the more common “arousal” dimension alone (Schimmack and Rainer,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B138a">2002</xref>
).</p>
<p>The use of bipolar vs. unipolar rating items presents some interesting challenges (Yorke,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B172">2001</xref>
). A bipolar rating item is labeled at one pole with a term that is opposite in meaning to the other pole to the extent possible (such as “happy” at one end and “sad” at the other). But some researchers have found that supposedly opposite constructs such as happy and sad or excited and calm do not traverse from one pole to the other in a linear, unique, proportionally exclusive manner—that is, they are not exact opposites, do not refer to the identically opposite semantic construct, and do not transition from one to the other in a mutually exclusive manner (it is possible to feel happy and sad at the same time). While space does not permit the discussion of the important question of the distinction between response item formats (see Cacioppo et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B12">1997</xref>
; Larsen et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B92">2001</xref>
; Yorke,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B172">2001</xref>
), the semantics of rating item labels may interact with interpretations of magnitude when conclusions about locus distinctions are made. This is a crucial matter given that negative emotion rating items (e.g., “Rate how sad” vs. “Rate along a happy-sad bipolar continuum”) and responses to emotions with putative negative emotions (e.g., a piece of
<italic>a priori</italic>
angry music) are routine design matters in investigations of emotion locus. The increase in experimental efficiency of collecting a single bipolar rating item response (instead of two unipolar rating item responses) weighed against the methodological challenges of using bipolar ratings is an issue that has not been systematically addressed in the emotion locus literature but may have important ramifications.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Key finding</title>
<p>In nine of the tabulated entries, it was possible to perform a simple count of the number of times the mean internal locus rating was greater than the corresponding mean external locus rating (e.g., rating of internal locus sadness vs. rating of external locus sadness): this is shown as a fraction in the first entry in the Main Findings column of Supplementary Table
<xref ref-type="supplementary-material" rid="SM1">1</xref>
. The counts are based on the cell pairs found in the tables and figures of the publications where direct comparisons of locus pairs were presented. Forty-five out of 178 cell-pair comparisons were higher for internal locus means compared to the external locus mean. If there was no trend, 89 (50%) was the expected count, and so a significantly
<italic>small</italic>
number of cases had internal locus rated higher [χ
<sup>2</sup>
<sub>(1,
<italic>N</italic>
= 178)</sub>
= 43.51,
<italic>p</italic>
< 0.001]. Furthermore, where significant main effects of locus were reported, external locus ratings were rated higher than corresponding internal locus ratings in eight analyses, and vice-versa in two analyses. On other occasions, difference in mean locus ratings was not significantly different or not reported.</p>
<p>The data from the two Ilie and Thompson studies were collated and compared for the current review because those data were designed to allow such comparison. The results reflect the overall findings across the tabulated literature, and so are summarized here. One group of participants (
<italic>n</italic>
= 27) rated external locus responses in one experiment (Ilie and Thompson,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B59">2006</xref>
), and another group (
<italic>n</italic>
= 64) in a separate experiment rated internal locus emotions (Ilie and Thompson,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B60">2011</xref>
; Experiment 1). The same design and procedure as well as similar stimulus manipulations were used for both experiments with the exception of the musical repertoire used: 5–7 seconds long baroque and classical excerpts for the 2006 study, and a single Mozart piece lasting 7 minutes for the 2011 study. The stimuli were all digitally processed to produce manipulation of pitch, tempo and intensity (two levels for each). The comparisons of mean loci data are summarized in Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F1">1</xref>
. The figure demonstrates the trend found throughout the literature that felt emotions tend to be rated the same or lower than expressed emotions for the corresponding emotion rating and independent variable level combinations.</p>
<fig id="F1" position="float">
<label>Figure 1</label>
<caption>
<p>
<bold>Collation of data from two experiments (Ilie and Thompson,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B59">2006</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B60">2011</xref>
, Experiment 1) comparing external and internal locus results across the combinations of independent variables—intensity (loud, soft), rate (fast, slow) and pitch (high, low) for each of three dependent variables (valence, energy arousal and tension arousal) which were formed from combinations of two five-point (0–4) unipolar rating items hence a range of 0–8</bold>
. Error bars indicate ±1
<italic>SE</italic>
.</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="fpsyg-04-00837-g0001"></graphic>
</fig>
<p>The use of different stimuli between locus conditions, as is the case in this example, may raise some concerns about the validity of such a comparison, but in that case, one might expect that either a systematic variable has led to the identified trend, or that the number of felt mean scores would be less than the perceived mean score in 50% of pairs (that is, it would be distributed according to chance). As shown in Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F1">1</xref>
, two comparisons have higher felt means than expressed mean ratings: valence rating for the soft, slow, high pitch condition—7.13 vs. 7.01, and tension rating for the loud, fast, low pitch condition—5.25 vs. 5.22. But for each of these pairs, the (1SE) error bars overlap.</p>
<p>While the tabulated literature did not allow a complete, direct statistical comparison of the relative mean magnitude of felt and expressed locus responses, a crude comparison using error bar overlapping was conducted, shown in the magnitude column of Supplementary Table
<xref ref-type="supplementary-material" rid="SM1">1</xref>
when available or extractable (see note for that column for more details and limitations). A count of these crude “significance” tests revealed that overall there were 99 cases where mean felt emotions were rated as lower in magnitude (regardless of the emotion rated) than the mean expressed rating (for the corresponding emotion rating item), but only nine occasions where the reverse was the case (mean expressed magnitude lower than mean felt magnitude). In 77 tests, error bars between mean locus pairs overlapped, suggesting a relatively high proportion of cases where participants rank emotions as being well matched in magnitude across locus.</p>
<p>In one of the two studies where mean internal locus was rated as higher than mean external locus, it was valence that exhibited a main effect, with mean felt emotions rated as above zero, and mean expressed emotions as below zero (Kallinen and Ravaja,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B71">2006</xref>
). In the same study, a second main effect was reported where negative activation ratings were also rated in this order across loci. Furthermore, an interaction was identified for the valence score, where pieces that were expected to represent negative emotions (fear and sadness) were rated with higher felt negative emotions than expressed negative emotions (see Main Findings column in Supplementary Table
<xref ref-type="supplementary-material" rid="SM1">1</xref>
, row 4). The second study where mean internal locus was rated as higher than the mean external locus (Vieillard et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B163">2008</xref>
) produced an overall main effect of higher rating for felt than expressed emotion, using the “best label” score approach.</p>
<p>Apart from these two studies, the overwhelming evidence presented in Supplementary Table
<xref ref-type="supplementary-material" rid="SM1">1</xref>
is that when emotion loci are not statistically the same, emotion felt is rated lower than emotion expressed by the music—for example, higher positive expressed emotion than positive felt emotion (Dibben,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">2004</xref>
; Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B142">2007b</xref>
; Konečni et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B83">2008</xref>
; Zentner et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B173">2008</xref>
; Ali and Peynircioǧlu,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B1">2010</xref>
; Hunter et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B56">2010</xref>
). The study by Salimpoor et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B133">2009</xref>
) provides evidence that when there is a difference in mean locus rating, the difference is constituted as felt emotion being lower, rather than expressed emotion being higher. This evidence is drawn from the comparison of music selection condition results: in the self-selected condition both felt and expressed emotions were rated with a statistically equal mean of about 7 on a pleasure scale of 1 (neutral) to 10 (extremely pleasurable), but in the experimenter music-selected condition, felt emotions ratings dropped to four while expressed emotion ratings remained at around seven.</p>
<p>Across the tabulated literature a number of interacting variables were investigated to see what other influences bear on locus response, including physiological state, personality, age, music-selection responsibility (participant or experimenter), musical features and, as already described above, the putative emotional connotation expected in the music—for example, when the music is expected to represent negative emotion. Some studies reported relationships between interacting variables and the gap across emotion loci (“GAEL,” the difference between the internal and external locus scores, usually reported as an absolute value). With regard to personality differences, participants exhibiting high scores in trait neuroticism-anxiety and on a scale measuring the behavioral inhibition system (“BIS”, the system that specializes in dealing with aversive signals) responded statistically with a larger GAEL score than did those scoring low in neurotic-anxiety and BIS (Kallinen and Ravaja,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B71">2006</xref>
). Kallinen and Rajava argued that high neurotic-anxiety participants suppressed emotional
<italic>experiences</italic>
more than their extraverted counterparts (p. 195). In the Salimpoor et al. study, the method of stimulus selection mattered, with participant-selected music producing more equal ratings across loci (smaller GAEL) than experimenter-selected ratings (Salimpoor et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B133">2009</xref>
). Furthermore, the statistically equal ratings between felt and expressed emotion are found when liked music is used, compared to disliked music (Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B141">2007a</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B144">2010</xref>
). This is consistent with Salimpoor et al. because the self-selected pieces were presumably liked more than the experimenter-selected pieces in that study.</p>
<p>Hunter et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B56">2010</xref>
) identified interactions between loci and each of the musical feature variable manipulations investigated—tempo and mode—for their specially manipulated stimuli. While they found the overall lower emotion ratings for internal compared to external locus conditions, happiness ratings amplified this difference when the tempo was fast, and sadness ratings amplified this GAEL when tempo was slow (for a summary of other interactions in this study, see Main Findings column of Supplementary Table
<xref ref-type="supplementary-material" rid="SM1">1</xref>
, row 14). Schubert (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B141">2007a</xref>
) reported systematic differences in locus due to preference, with loved music producing a close match between locus scores (small GAEL), a result that was replicated (Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B144">2010</xref>
), albeit with fairly marginal significance (
<italic>p</italic>
= 0.045—see Supplementary Table
<xref ref-type="supplementary-material" rid="SM1">1</xref>
, row 15).</p>
<p>Apart from a main effect of internal locus emotions being rated lower than external locus emotions under many conditions, few of the additional independent variables investigated in the tabulated literature have produced consistent responses, most importantly because of a lack of replication. The most commonly repeated investigated interaction across the tabulated studies is the putative emotion of the musical stimuli. But, as mentioned above, the results are not altogether consistent across studies, and furthermore, there are different ways of collecting emotion ratings (e.g., different ways of labeling of the rating items—see discussion under Response Format, above), transforming those data into dependent variables, and the nature of the musical stimuli.</p>
<p>The clearest, new trend, then, that has emerged in the literature since Gabrielsson's review is that when there is a mismatch between felt and expressed emotion,
<italic>and</italic>
when these data are gathered via rating items (as distinct from selection of discrete emotions: see Eerola and Vuoskoski,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B30">2011</xref>
), results rarely show that felt emotions are rated as statistically higher than expressed emotions. As a consequence, the next section of this review focuses on building theory that explains this finding about emotion locus in music.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Theoretical considerations</title>
<sec>
<title>Felt emotion less than expressed: emotional contagion theory</title>
<p>As an extension of Gabrielsson's thinking described in the opening of this paper, it could be assumed that there is no differentiation between the conceptualization of internal and external loci of emotion: it is all part of the same emotional response, and being asked to make the distinction may even be considered artificial. But the findings of the literature reviewed, and in particular the explicit investigation by Juslin and Laukka (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B67">2004</xref>
), demonstrate that a majority of people can make distinctions between these perspectives, understanding that music can appear to express an emotion and that the emotion can be felt. External and internal loci do not necessarily meld into the one experience, at least not for the wide majority of the participants in the Juslin and Laukka study. Even if this distinction is a purely artificial or cultural one, it appears to be widely present and is in need of theoretical understanding.</p>
<p>However, the reason for the quantitatively different rating between loci found in the tabulated literature, when it occurs, could be something as basic as the instructions. Konečni (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B82">2008</xref>
) demonstrated that the wording and detail of the task can impose a secondary influence on the results. He revealed different amounts of detail about a publication on music and emotion and asked the participants to determine which locus the paper was referring to at the different stages. The locus indicated in the article was external and revealed in the title (“… perceived intensity of emotion”)
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="fn0005">
<sup>5</sup>
</xref>
. In Konečni's study, only 25% of participants identified the correct locus, suggesting they tend to think in terms of their own feelings when there is “ambiguity” in wording. The Konečni study demonstrates the importance of clarity of communication. But a theoretically interesting question is why such bias in interpretation may be present.</p>
<p>To explain locus relationships, Evans and Schubert (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B32">2008</xref>
) drew on the distinction made between absolutism and referentialism in the experience of music, as proposed by Meyer (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B103">1956</xref>
) and reinterpreted by Schubert and McPherson (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B147">2006</xref>
). Referentialism suggests that connections between music, emotions, and other situations/events are made by association primarily as a result of life experiences and cultural knowledge but also through highly individual and even idiosyncratic connections. A mismatch in musical emotion and felt emotion can be explained by these kinds of idiosyncratic, arbitrary pairings, as Gabrielsson (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B35">2002</xref>
) points out. Schubert and McPherson then proposed that meaning can also be encoded more directly into the music (as according to Meyer's account of absolutism, or “absolute-expressionism”), where emotions are directly decoded by the listener [an idea found in the “lens model” proposed by Juslin (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B62">1997</xref>
); Juslin and Lindström (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B69">2010</xref>
)] through an act of mimicry, and neurophysiologically via the mirror neuron mechanism (Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B142">2007b</xref>
). An influential, related explanation was proposed by Juslin and Västfjäll (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B70">2008</xref>
), who labeled this kind of process as “emotion contagion.” In other words, emotional contagion is the direct influence upon the listener of the emotion that the music portrays, in the absence of outside “interference” through, for example, idiosyncratic connections—such as the unhappy break-up with a partner when otherwise happy music is playing (“referentialism” according to Evans and Schubert, and “episodic memory” according to Juslin and Västfjäll). Emotional contagion may then be taken as one theoretical position for understanding relationships between emotion loci in music.</p>
<p>Put simply, emotional contagion in music refers to the transmission of an emotion via the auditory sense alone. Bharucha et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B7">2006</xref>
) explain:</p>
<disp-quote>
<p>Unlike other types of contagions, the germs of emotion transmitted by music seem to require no social interaction—musical emotions are airborne contagions. The social contagion of emotions is thought to stem from the tendency to automatically mimic the social cues of others, such as body posture, movement, facial expressions, and vocal expressions. It is perhaps the latter that leads to social contagion in music. (p. 156)</p>
</disp-quote>
<p>Several of the reviewed studies (Gabrielsson,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B35">2002</xref>
; Dibben,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">2004</xref>
; Kallinen and Ravaja,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B71">2006</xref>
; Ali and Peynircioǧlu,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B1">2010</xref>
; Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B144">2010</xref>
) refer to the possible role of social inhibition and the laboratory setting. Under such circumstances, strong emotional outbursts can be considered inappropriate, leading participants to suppress felt emotional response relative to external locus rating. By adopting an emotional contagion framework, what this means theoretically is that inhibition of experienced emotion can take place when making internal locus responses. Evidence from non-music literature about social inhibition and its influence on felt emotion can be found in the within-internal-locus distinctions identified in social psychology where public displays of emotions do not identically map onto actually felt emotions (Bono and Vey,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B9">2005</xref>
; Mann,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B97">2005</xref>
; Bakker and Heuven,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B2">2006</xref>
; Langer et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B90">2007</xref>
; Manne et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B98">2007</xref>
; see also discussion in Limitation 5, above). However, further investigation will be required to examine whether this kind of social, contextual adjustment of felt emotion occurs in response to music.</p>
<p>If we apply contagion theory to explain emotion locus relationships in music perception, that expressed emotions are transmitted to (or infect) the listener, then we can explain inhibition as a plausible explanation for reduced felt ratings, and the inhibition may be a product of social context. Evidence from a study where internal locus emotions were rated in response to various emotion expressing film excerpts (Jakobs et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B61">2001</xref>
) suggests there is an influence of social context. When a film extract was viewed alone, felt sadness ratings were higher than when viewed with another person (see also Raghunathan and Corfman,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B119">2006</xref>
; and for a similar design, but using music stimuli, see Liljeström et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B94">2012</xref>
).</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Felt emotion more varied than expressed: decoding theory and the lens model</title>
<p>A further complication of the locus relationship debate is the variability in responses to either locus. If the properties of the artwork—in this case, the relationships among musical features over time—are consistent upon repeated exposures, then it may seem logical to assume that the emotion expressed by that stimulus is also stable, and it is the internal locus that might be more variable, depending, for example, on the mood of the listener/perceiver, as Gabrielsson points out in the “zero correlation” case of his “no-systematic relation” (Gabrielsson,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B35">2002</xref>
p. 136). Juslin's lens model, discussed above, can be used to help interpret this situation. If a performer and/or composer encodes particular emotions into a piece of music, the listener's decoding will to some extent be a statistical process, meaning that decoding will not necessarily be the same as the encoded emotion. Felt emotion may be characterized as “encoded emotion plus noise.” Schubert (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B142">2007b</xref>
– see Supplementary Table
<xref ref-type="supplementary-material" rid="SM1">1</xref>
, row 5) tested this explicitly by comparing the variance for each emotion rating pair across loci to assess the “stability” of the loci, arguing that if one locus had a lower variance than the other, it was more stable. Six
<italic>F-tests</italic>
out of 20 (4 emotions rated × 5 pieces of music) were significant at
<italic>p</italic>
= 0.05, with felt emotions demonstrating larger variance than expressed emotions, with three of these being in response to one piece, “Jupiter” from
<italic>The Planets</italic>
by Holst (for ratings of emotional strength, arousal and valence). One out of 20 may have been significant by chance alone, and so the study concluded that expressed emotions are overall more stable than felt. It suggests that when all things are equal, internal locus equals external locus plus noise, a claim that requires more research (Schubert, submitted).</p>
<p>Thus, the theoretical underpinning of emotional contagion has not yet been fully addressed in the current literature of emotion locus for music. Further studies will need to falsify the idea that felt emotion is lower in absolute magnitude than expressed emotion because of inhibited contagion, and whether there is a systematic difference in variance between the two loci. Cases of felt emotion being greater than expressed will need to be better explained from a theoretical stance before the inhibited contagion account can be fully supported. I will examine one further theoretical position that is able to explain some of the results identified in the tabulated studies.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Mixed responses: dissociation theory</title>
<p>I am not explicitly concerned in this review with research on music expressing conflicting emotions, such as happy and sad emotions, at the same time (Hunter et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B55">2008</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B56">2010</xref>
). But peculiar to the locus debate is when a complex combination of emotion matches and non-matches occur between and within loci, such as music
<italic>expressing</italic>
fear but the listener reacts with feelings of embarrassment
<italic>and</italic>
joy. Let us suppose now that the joyful reaction was due to the memory that the music evokes about something quite personal and private (as per the “episodic memory mechanism” proposed by Juslin and Västfjäll,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B70">2008</xref>
), and that upon realizing the response was possibly inappropriate in the current setting (e.g., the music was heard in a concert setting where the other audience members were quiet and calm), the listener became embarrassed. Thus, three potentially mismatching emotions are at play here—one the fear expressed by the music, and the two felt emotions (embarrassment and joy).</p>
<p>After the work of Charland and Colombetti (Charland,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B16">2005</xref>
; Colombetti,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B19">2005</xref>
), I (Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B145">2012</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B146">2013</xref>
) proposed a solution to the conundrum of mixed emotions in music by arguing that there are two qualitatively different kinds of “feeling” (to use the term in a way similar to Zentner et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B173">2008</xref>
) experiences: emotion valence and affect valence. Emotion valence is specific to emotional contemplation, without any necessary approach or withdrawal action readiness (Frijda et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B34">1989</xref>
). Affect valence, on the other hand, is concerned with the action/evaluative response qualities, which can generally be thought of as preferences (including enjoyment, liking and attraction, or lack thereof). Affect valence is the outcome of the music-listening activity and therefore, also encompasses the more powerful aesthetic responses to music, such as awe, spirituality and being moved (Kivy,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B76">1990</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B77">1999</xref>
; Konečni,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B81">2005</xref>
). In the example, the listener was experiencing the positive
<italic>emotion valence</italic>
of joy (internal locus) but then had a negative
<italic>affect</italic>
evaluation of embarrassment. The Van Zijl and Sloboda study (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B161">2011</xref>
, see quote in Supplementary Table
<xref ref-type="supplementary-material" rid="SM1">1</xref>
, row 19, Main Findings) further exemplifies this separation through the felt emotion in response to the utilitarian task of learning the piece (affect valence: frustration, remain calm) and the
<italic>emotion</italic>
valence experienced in response to the music (e.g., again from the quote in Supplementary Table
<xref ref-type="supplementary-material" rid="SM1">1</xref>
, peaceful, happy).</p>
<p>The separation of affect valence and emotion valence, although at times non-trivial, is proposed as a way of resolving previous confusion in the literature about some “mixed” emotional responses (an additional example is provided in Table
<xref ref-type="table" rid="T2">2</xref>
). Simple preference (liking, loving, hating) is a typical example of affect valence found in the literature.</p>
<table-wrap id="T2" position="float">
<label>Table 2</label>
<caption>
<p>
<bold>Revision of Gabrielsson's relationships between felt and expressed emotion loci in music</bold>
.</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>Relationship</bold>
</th>
<th align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>Example</bold>
</th>
<th align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>Possible mechanism/theory</bold>
</th>
<th align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>Notes</bold>
</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">1. Matched relationships</td>
<td rowspan="1" colspan="1"></td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Contagion</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Same as Gabrielsson's “positive” relationship.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">• positive to positive</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">The happy music makes me feel happy.</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Contagion</td>
<td rowspan="1" colspan="1"></td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">• negative to negative</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">The sad music makes me feel sad.</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Contagion</td>
<td rowspan="1" colspan="1"></td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">2. Unmatched relationships</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="5" colspan="1">The music is happy but makes me feel sad.</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="5" colspan="1">Episodic memory/Evaluative conditioning/Referentialism</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="5" colspan="1">Presented as a broader alternative to Gabrielsson's “negative”/“opposite,” encompassing any non-matching emotion/affect.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">• positive to non-positive</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">• non-positive to positive</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">• negative to non-negative</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">• non-negative to negative</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<italic>3. Complex/Mixed</italic>
</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">The music expresses several emotions/I feel several emotions, some different from the music.</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Dissociation/several</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">This
<italic>optional</italic>
category can be reduced to simultaneously multiple occurrences of matched and/or unmatched relationships.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">4. Interesting Subcategories, (classified within category 1, 2 or 3 above)</td>
<td rowspan="1" colspan="1"></td>
<td rowspan="1" colspan="1"></td>
<td rowspan="1" colspan="1"></td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">a. Felt level magnitude</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">The music expresses a lot of sadness, but I only feel a little bit sad.</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Inhibited contagion/partial Dissociation</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Reflects the common finding that could be a result of inhibited or incomplete contagion, and/or partial dissociation.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">b. Affect/emotion blend</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">The music expresses grief, and it makes me feel a sense of sadness but also a sense of awe.</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Contagion + Dissociation</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Music expresses an emotion (in the example, it is a negative emotion valence), but, in the example, the listener feels sadness (possible matching, and a negative valence emotion), as well as a positive affect valence (awe). This is a subcategory of either matched and/or unmatched relationships. The example suggests both (or Complex)</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">c. Differential variance across loci</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Felt emotion ratings more varied than expressed emotion ratings</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Contagion, Decoding theory</td>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Contagion occurs, but decoding of emotion occurs with statistical noise (as suggested by the Lens model), which might be explained, for example, by the mood of the listener, their cultural immersion and their familiarity with the style of the music.</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</table-wrap>
<p>The affect/emotion valence distinction is explained from the cognitive theoretical standpoint of dissociation theory, where when listening to music we are usually in a state where we “switch off” pain circuits
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="fn0006">
<sup>6</sup>
</xref>
, meaning that we can enjoy negative emotions without the unpleasant negative affect valence (Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B139">1996</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B143">2009–2010</xref>
). In the example above, the embarrassed (negative
<italic>affect</italic>
valence) response meant that the individual was not in a dissociated state and could, as a result, not (or no longer) enjoy the music that, in another context, he or she may have liked very much (positive affect valence). Recent classifications of descriptive adjectives have started to separate groups of terms in ways compatible with dissociation theory. For example, Juslin and Laukka (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B67">2004</xref>
) propose that some emotions are more suitable for inducing in the listener, while others are more apt for being expressed by the music. Being
<italic>moved</italic>
,
<italic>amazed</italic>
, or
<italic>enchanted</italic>
are presented as examples of induced (but not expressed) emotions. By revising the way affect qualities are conceptualized, these may be understood as unique to induction (rather than expression)
<italic>because</italic>
they are affect valences (rather than emotion valences). Being moved might be a result of feeling sad, or happy (emotion valence), or some other experience(s) which led to the affective response of being moved.</p>
<p>My point is that by differentiating (dissociating) between emotion valence and affect valence, affect/emotion blends can be more simply understood than the otherwise complex responses we appear to have to music. “The music makes me sad, and that gives me pleasure” suggests a negative
<italic>emotion</italic>
valence of sadness, but a positive
<italic>affect</italic>
valence of pleasure. There is no need to view sadness and pleasure as conflicting. From a theoretical stance, the pleasure indicates that the listener is in a dissociated state, meaning that negative valence affects are inhibited, and so all emotions (negative and positive) can be enjoyed. Zentner et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B173">2008</xref>
) present a statistically determined grouping of music evoked adjectives, producing nine clusters of word groups, two of which—“wonder” and “transcendence”—actually fit well with the affect valence concept (with terms such as “amazed”, “moved”, feeling of “spirituality”), while the other clusters indicate adjectives more representative of emotion valence. However, terms such as “irritated” (part of the “tension” cluster) are more typically concerned with affect valence—being irritated by a piece of music is a reason that the listener might stop listening, rather than experience as an emotion that she or he can contemplate (Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B146">2013</xref>
). Dissociation theory may provide a solution to one of the enduring debates on emotion locus and whether there exists a special set of aesthetic or musical emotions that are activated only when an artistic (musical) activity or thought takes place, distinct from utilitarian emotions experienced in everyday life (for further discussion, see Kivy,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B75">1989</xref>
; Krumhansl,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B88">1997</xref>
; Pouivet,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B115">2000</xref>
; Khalfa et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B74">2002</xref>
; Krumhansl,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B89">2002</xref>
; Scherer,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B137">2005</xref>
; Silvia and Brown,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B152">2007</xref>
; Silvia,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B151">2009</xref>
; Barrett et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B3">2010</xref>
; Peretz,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B112">2010</xref>
; Perlovsky,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B114">2010</xref>
; Juslin,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B64">2011</xref>
; Juslin et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B68">2011</xref>
; Chan et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B15">2013</xref>
; Juslin,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B65">2013</xref>
).</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Revision of gabrielsson's locus relationships</title>
<p>Gabrielsson's categories of emotion locus relationships have provided an important framework for encouraging direct engagement with and awareness of the question of emotion locus in music. The current review suggests that the categories may be reworked and calibrated to reflect the current distinctions between loci of which the research community is now aware. Table
<xref ref-type="table" rid="T2">2</xref>
summarizes the revised relationships and reports possible explanatory mechanisms (Juslin,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B62">1997</xref>
; Juslin and Västfjäll,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B70">2008</xref>
; Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B142">2007b</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B143">2009–2010</xref>
). Reworded are the terms “positive” vs. “negative” (or “opposite”) relationships—now “matched” vs. “unmatched,” respectively to reserve the former terms for the conventional use of emotion and affect valence (positive/negative). “No systematic relationship” has been absorbed into “unmatched” to reflect the non-matching nature of emotion pairs that are neither “opposite” nor “positive” (referred to as contrapositive by Evans and Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B31">2006</xref>
), such as sad and excited, not just those that are directly “opposite” on an emotion-space geometry (such as sad and happy).</p>
<p>If we eliminate the no-systematic relationship category in those cases when it is due to instability of responses over time, or those that do not concern emotional relationships between expressed and felt emotions on any occasions (no systematic relationship, and no relationship), we end up with two broad categories: matched vs. unmatched relationships. The omission of the non-systematic and no relationships are justified by two findings (1) when no emotion is reported in both the felt and expressed loci, the categorization becomes irrelevant (see, e.g., Sloboda and Juslin,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B155">2010</xref>
, p. 83)—the participant may be having formalist, cognitive or no responses to the music, but without emotion, and (2) no emotion in one locus and some emotion, or even none, in another can be subsumed by the unmatched emotion locus relationship.</p>
<p>The proposed scheme attempts to revise Gabrielsson's set of relationships to bring them into line with the current state of research on emotion locus in music. The theoretical organizing principle of the revision is based on our understanding, assumptions, and limitations of the emotional contagion processes, and its interactions with a dissociation mechanism and the Lens model inspired decoding theory, discussed above.</p>
<p>Thus, in the revised format two main categories of relationships are proposed: matched and unmatched, referring to whether the expressed emotion is reflected in the felt emotion. A third main category of relationships is included, which is referred to as complex/mixed relationships to account for the possibility of both matched and/or unmatched relationships occurring at the same time. This category is reducible to matched and/or unmatched relationships occurring multiple times and/or simultaneously, and so the complex/mixed relationship is provided for completeness rather than necessity. Furthermore, this category allows for convenient discussion of the interesting area of research concerning mixed emotions portrayed and evoked by music (Evans and Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B32">2008</xref>
; Hunter et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B55">2008</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B56">2010</xref>
; Barrett et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B3">2010</xref>
; Juslin,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B64">2011</xref>
; Juslin et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B68">2011</xref>
).</p>
<p>Subcategories can be attached to the two main categories based on the valence of the relations (for matched, positive [expressed] to positive [felt] or negative to negative, and for unmatched, positive to negative and vice versa—see Table
<xref ref-type="table" rid="T2">2</xref>
). The key issue here is the position of the boundary delineating matched vs. unmatched emotion pairs. For magnitude comparisons of the same emotion variable (e.g., rating happiness on a 1–10 continuum for felt and expressed emotion), conventional inferential statistical procedures can be used to determine matched (no difference) or unmatched (different) loci. However, for discrete emotion words, or when emotions are plotted on an affect grid (Russell,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B129">1980</xref>
; Russell et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B131">1989b</xref>
), the analysis can be more involved. For example, in some contexts it might be sufficient to refer to a calm emotion expressed as being matched with a happy emotion felt. The current taxonomy does not explicitly dictate where the boundary between a matched vs. unmatched emotion pair lies—e.g., whether “happy” and “calm” are matched emotions or not. Furthermore, for discrete emotion words the boundary may be fuzzy or ill-defined. Evans and Schubert (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B32">2008</xref>
) developed a criterion using Euclidean distance between valence (x-axis) and arousal (y-axis) (assuming the two variables to be orthogonal as per Russell,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B128">1979</xref>
,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B129">1980</xref>
), and selected a (more or less arbitrary, but conservative) angle in the space about which to identify whether a pair of emotions are matched vs. unmatched. After some experimentation, they selected an angle of 45° in the polar coordinate system within which the emotions were classified as matched. A “within same quadrant” (in a two-dimensional emotion space) approach is also a plausible, though less conservative, approach.</p>
<p>If this geometric approach to differentiating matched vs. unmatched emotions continues to be adopted, then it would be interesting to see if this empirically determined boundary angle could be reproduced in such a way as to group qualitatively similar emotions together, such as applying inferential statistical tests to subtended angles (Mardia and Jupp,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B99">2000</xref>
). In practice, it is unlikely that such boundaries will be fixed and stable given that a single emotion does not necessarily occupy the same location on an emotion-space, because there exists some fluidity of meaning within and across individuals and cultures (Russell et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B130">1989a</xref>
; Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B140">1999</xref>
). Furthermore, investigation of locus relationships using discrete emotions is in need of further attention, because nearly all of the tabulated studies used rating items, reflecting an interest in magnitude of emotion rather than the semantic distinctions of Russell's “circumplex” model of emotion.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Discussion and conclusions</title>
<p>Gabrielsson's rediscovery and systematic organization of the relationships between felt and perceived emotion during a musical experience can be considered significantly responsible for opening up a new topic of music psychology and in regard to emotion research in general because it brought to focus the distinction between the locus of emotion when the reactions are not between two sentient beings, but between one sentient being and a piece of music. This review identified the key issues that have emerged in subsequent years and proposed three cognitive based theoretical frameworks to help focus further research. The theories of emotional contagion, decoding and dissociation were presented.</p>
<p>Emotional contagion explains why we tend to feel the emotion that music is conveying. Through the inhibition of this felt emotion, we feel emotion at a lower magnitude than the corresponding expressed emotion. This “inhibited-contagion” account will need more research to determine why there would then be any situations when felt emotions were rated as stronger (in magnitude) than expressed emotions. According to the inhibited contagion account, these situations should not occur, but they do (albeit relatively infrequently in the studies reviewed). Furthermore, is inhibited-contagion a satisfactory explanation for unmatched emotions across loci when the emotions are discrete (such as happy, sad, angry, calm)? And does the related idea of decoding theory mean that internal locus will have more variability than external locus? Such a view needs to account for the question of whether the judgment of an observed object (or piece of music) is really sensed in a fixed, stable way by the perceiver (for philosophical views on this matter, see Townsend,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B159">1987</xref>
). That is, contagion theory has the implication that it is possible to objectively “know” the external locus emotions, for example through examination of musical features. However, external locus of emotion must also be simulated by the perceiver—for example, illusions demonstrate that the thought-to-be-observed object is not always isomorphic with the physical stimulus (Coren and Girgus,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B20">1978</xref>
)—and this is a view that has not been tested in the tabulated literature, even though it is important in philosophical aesthetics and is attracting interest in neuroscientific research (e.g., Sevdalis and Keller,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B150">2009</xref>
; Novembre et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B107">2012</xref>
).</p>
<p>Dissociation theory attempts to address confusion in simultaneously differential responses to music within locus—such as experiencing high preference for music that makes that listener
<italic>feel</italic>
sad (both internal-locus responses). This theory allows researchers to understand that different, apparently conflicting feelings can be experienced at the same time, without having to dogmatically attribute them to one or the other locus of emotion (e.g., the music is sad, and I like that). Contagion and dissociation principles may work hand in hand in the perception of music: under normal, real-life, day-to-day circumstances it might be undesirable and even dangerous to acquire emotion solely through contagion transfer (e.g., feeling angry when someone else is angry—experiencing fear might be more adaptive. See Preston and De Waal,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B116">2002</xref>
, who also provide a neuroscientific explanation of inhibited contagion). That is, the individual experiences “utilitarian” rather than “aesthetic” emotions (Scherer,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B136">2004</xref>
). The day-to-day circumstance is one where emotion valence and affect valence are not dissociated, allowing adaptive responses (the anger leads to a negative affect valence). In music listening and other aesthetic contexts, dissociation of emotion valence from affect valence allows contagion to operate unhindered by day-to-day, utilitarian circumstances and can be enjoyed. In other words, according to dissociation theory, affect valence is always positive in an aesthetic context. The context is the cause of the dissociation between emotion valence and affect valence.</p>
<p>Preference being higher when internal and external loci are matched also requires further investigation. It could be that preference can be implicitly measured by how well the two loci are matched—that is, the smaller that gap across emotion loci, the greater the preference. But determining the causal chain will be of interest, too—whether the preference causes the locus to be different, whether the difference in locus causes preference to change, or whether some other variables are involved. Another interesting research question is which mechanism can explain this finding. Dissociation theory and contagion theory can explain the preference through the activation (which is pleasurable) of contagion circuits. Contagion circuits are the same or related to mirror circuits (e.g., Koelsch et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B80">2006</xref>
) and to the positive affects of empathy. But the nature of those circuits, as applied to the proposed cognitive theoretical frameworks, is in need of further investigation. For example, no studies have explicitly attempted to examine whether neural pathways for processing felt emotion might be different from the neural pathways for processing expressed emotions (e.g., see Blood and Zatorre,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B8">2001</xref>
; Peretz et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B113">2004</xref>
; Menon and Levitin,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B102">2005</xref>
; Brattico et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B10">2009</xref>
). Contagion-circuit theory assumes both loci have shared pathways (Preston and De Waal,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B116">2002</xref>
), but none of the data reported explicitly aims to test this assertion.</p>
<p>Trait and personality effects, including behavioral inhibition/activation systems (Kallinen and Ravaja,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B71">2006</xref>
), absorption (Kreutz et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B86">2008a</xref>
; Garrido and Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B37">2011a</xref>
; Herbert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B51">2011</xref>
), rumination (Garrido and Schubert,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B40">2013</xref>
; Wilhelm et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B170">2013</xref>
) and so on (e.g., Rentfrow and Gosling,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B122">2003</xref>
; Rentfrow et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B121">2011</xref>
), may each impact the way individuals differ in their felt and perceived judgments of music, but apart from Kallinen and Ravaja, little attention has been given to the effect of trait upon emotion locus. Psychological, physical and physiological states may also influence emotion loci relationships in music, but only a single study has examined this in the tabulated literature (Dibben,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">2004</xref>
, see Supplementary Table
<xref ref-type="supplementary-material" rid="SM1">1</xref>
, rows 1 and 2).</p>
<p>The wide range of task wordings may be in need of some standardization, with the name of the locus variable producing little agreement across studies and therefore unnecessary confusion (for example, when performing a database search on the topic). While the wording of the levels within the locus variable can be quite flexible, Konečni (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B82">2008</xref>
) demonstrated that the wording and detail of the task can also affect the results. Furthermore, the reader needs to be clear on the grammatical subject of the locus level (the
<italic>music</italic>
expresses and the
<italic>listener</italic>
perceives external locus emotion). More consistency in the labeling used to describe the variable in question is urged. In this review, the term “locus of emotion” or “emotion locus” has been adopted.</p>
<p>The present review calls for a firmer theoretical stance to help direct future research. Interestingly, one of the rarely cited, early writers on the locus relationship had a premonition of a useful theoretical framework for understanding emotion locus in music, with Vernon Lee's idea of “sympathetic imitation,” which in contemporary literature resembles emotional contagion—the dominant theoretical framework that provides a basis for explaining a large portion of the results of the literature investigated in this review.</p>
<p>Finally, based on an examination of the literature published in the decade after Gabrielsson's seminal work, some rearrangements and suggestions have been made that may assist future researchers investigating relationships between emotion expressed by music and emotion felt by the listener in response to music. The data examined and newly arising findings were used to rearrange Gabrielsson's categorization. Reflecting the growing interest in explaining why emotion ratings are different across locus in certain situations, the simplifying nomenclature of “matched” vs. “unmatched” emotion pairs across loci were used in this review as the basis of formulating locus relationships. There is much work to be done in understanding locus relationships, and this review, if successful, should soon become an interim report on the state of the art in the locus of emotion in music.</p>
<sec>
<title>Conflict of interest statement</title>
<p>The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
</body>
<back>
<ack>
<p>I am grateful for the detailed and helpful suggestions made by the reviewers. This research was supported by an Australian Research Council Future Fellowship, FT120100053.</p>
</ack>
<fn-group>
<fn id="fn0001">
<p>
<sup>1</sup>
Music is used in clinical and laboratory settings to induce mood. For a review, see Vastfjall (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B162">2001</xref>
). When music is selected for such mood induction, it is inferred that the music will affect the listener because the music is expressing the corresponding emotion. This is borne out by some of the studies in mood induction used in fields of research such as music therapy. Although there is a substantial literature that documents these effects (e.g., Kenealy,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B73">1988</xref>
; Thaut and De L'etoile,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B157">1993</xref>
; Gendolla and Kruesken,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B42">2001</xref>
; Kreutz et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B85">2002</xref>
; Larcom and Isaacowitz,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B91">2009</xref>
; Harkness et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B50">2010</xref>
; Dyck et al.,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B29">2011</xref>
), these research outputs are not reviewed here because the participant is not rating the emotion the music expresses, but (when required to) the way the music makes them feel.</p>
</fn>
<fn id="fn0002">
<p>
<sup>2</sup>
This issue and the relatively small number of comparable studies made it difficult to sufficiently fulfill the “Checklist of items to include when reporting a systematic review or meta-analysis” proposed by Moher et al. (
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B105">2009</xref>
). Relevant details (mean,
<italic>SD</italic>
and
<italic>N</italic>
) for this review are nevertheless tabulated when available.</p>
</fn>
<fn id="fn0003">
<p>
<sup>3</sup>
“At least” is indicated because use of the same labels in subsequent studies with an author who had already used these labels is not, in general, added to the count. Exceptions are made when the author has used substantially different labels (e.g., Dibben,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B25">2004</xref>
: compare Measure column for her Experiments 1 and 2, rows 1 and 2 of Supplementary Table
<xref ref-type="supplementary-material" rid="SM1">1</xref>
—both of these experiments are added to the count). Furthermore, when the term happy, sad, etc. is used to
<italic>exemplify</italic>
a label (such has “happy, calm, joy” to illustrate the meaning of the label “positive emotion”), it is not included in this count. The same applies for all summary data presented in this subsection. Where a term was presented in a language other than English, the English term, as reported in the target article, is used for tallying.</p>
</fn>
<fn id="fn0004">
<p>
<sup>4</sup>
Caution is urged in making such a grouping since “fear” and “scary” are importantly different along a dominance dimension (angry is a dominant emotion; fear/scared are submissive). The grouping is justified only in terms of similarity on typical valence-arousal semantic spaces (Russell,
<xref ref-type="bibr" rid="B129">1980</xref>
).</p>
</fn>
<fn id="fn0005">
<p>
<sup>5</sup>
Reading only the title of the article produced the greatest confusion in participant belief about the target locus of the article. However, this might not be so unreasonable given the ambiguity of the term “perceived”, particularly in the absence of the grammatical subject of the sentence, discussed earlier, and summarized in Table
<xref ref-type="table" rid="T1">1</xref>
.</p>
</fn>
<fn id="fn0006">
<p>
<sup>6</sup>
The term “circuits” is used here in a cognitive psychology, metaphorical context, rather than in a literal neuroscience way.</p>
</fn>
</fn-group>
<sec sec-type="supplementary material" id="s1">
<title>Supplementary material</title>
<p>The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.frontiersin.org/journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00837/abstract">http://www.frontiersin.org/journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00837/abstract</ext-link>
</p>
<supplementary-material content-type="local-data" id="SM1">
<label>Supplementary Table 1</label>
<caption>
<p>
<bold>Summary of emotion locus in music studies reviewed</bold>
.</p>
</caption>
<media xlink:href="Data_Sheet_1.PDF">
<caption>
<p>Click here for additional data file.</p>
</caption>
</media>
</supplementary-material>
</sec>
<ref-list>
<title>References</title>
<ref id="B1">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ali</surname>
<given-names>S. O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Peynircioǧlu</surname>
<given-names>Z. F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Intensity of emotions conveyed and elicited by familiar and unfamiliar music</article-title>
.
<source>Music Percept</source>
.
<volume>27</volume>
,
<fpage>177</fpage>
<lpage>182</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1525/mp.2010.27.3.177</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B1a">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Alwin</surname>
<given-names>D. F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1997</year>
).
<article-title>Feeling thermometers versus 7-point scales: Which are better?</article-title>
<source>Soc. Methods Res</source>
.
<volume>25</volume>
,
<fpage>318</fpage>
<lpage>340</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0049124197025003003</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B2">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bakker</surname>
<given-names>A. B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Heuven</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Emotional dissonance, burnout, and in-role performance among nurses and police officers</article-title>
.
<source>Int. J. Stress Manage</source>
.
<volume>13</volume>
,
<fpage>423</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/1072-5245.13.4.423</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B3">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Barrett</surname>
<given-names>F. S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Grimm</surname>
<given-names>K. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Robins</surname>
<given-names>R. W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wildschut</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sedikides</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Janata</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Music-evoked nostalgia: affect, memory, and personality</article-title>
.
<source>Emotion</source>
<volume>10</volume>
,
<fpage>390</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0019006</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20515227</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B4">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Barthomeuf</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Droit-Volet</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rousset</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>How emotions expressed by adults' faces affect the desire to eat liked and disliked foods in children compared to adults</article-title>
.
<source>Br. J. Dev. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>30</volume>
,
<fpage>253</fpage>
<lpage>266</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02033.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22550947</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B5">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Baumgartner</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1992</year>
).
<article-title>Remembrance of things past: music, autobiographical memory, and emotion</article-title>
.
<source>Adv. Consum. Res</source>
.
<volume>19</volume>
,
<fpage>613</fpage>
<lpage>620</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B6">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Belia</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fidler</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Williams</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cumming</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Researchers misunderstand confidence intervals and standard error bars</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Methods</source>
<volume>10</volume>
,
<fpage>389</fpage>
<lpage>396</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/1082-989X.10.4.389</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16392994</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B7">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bharucha</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Curtis</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Paroo</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Varieties of musical experience</article-title>
.
<source>Cognition</source>
<volume>100</volume>
,
<fpage>131</fpage>
<lpage>172</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.cognition.2005.11.008</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16412410</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B8">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Blood</surname>
<given-names>A. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zatorre</surname>
<given-names>R. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion</article-title>
.
<source>Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A</source>
.
<volume>98</volume>
,
<fpage>11818</fpage>
<lpage>11823</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1073/pnas.191355898</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11573015</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B8a">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bombari</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mast</surname>
<given-names>M. S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brosch</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sander</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>How interpersonal power affects empathic accuracy: differential roles of mentalizing vs. mirroring?</article-title>
<source>Front Hum. Neurosci</source>
.
<volume>7</volume>
:
<issue>375</issue>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3389/fnhum.2013.00375</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23882206</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B9">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bono</surname>
<given-names>J. E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Vey</surname>
<given-names>M. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Toward understanding emotional management at work: a quantitative review of emotional labor research</article-title>
, in
<source>Emotions in Organizational Behavior</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Härtel</surname>
<given-names>C. E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zerbe</surname>
<given-names>W. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ashkanasy</surname>
<given-names>N. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Mahwah, NJ</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Lawrence Erlbaum Associates</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>213</fpage>
<lpage>233</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B10">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Brattico</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brattico</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jacobsen</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>The origins of the aesthetic enjoyment of music—a review of the literature</article-title>
.
<source>Musicae Sci</source>
.
<volume>13</volume>
,
<fpage>15</fpage>
<lpage>39</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/1029864909013002031</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B11">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cacioppo</surname>
<given-names>J. T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gardner</surname>
<given-names>W. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<article-title>Emotion</article-title>
.
<source>Annu. Rev. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>50</volume>
,
<fpage>191</fpage>
<lpage>214</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.191</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10074678</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B12">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cacioppo</surname>
<given-names>J. T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gardner</surname>
<given-names>W. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Berntson</surname>
<given-names>G. G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1997</year>
).
<article-title>Beyond bipolar conceptualizations and measures: the case of attitudes and evaluative space</article-title>
.
<source>Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev</source>
.
<volume>1</volume>
,
<fpage>3</fpage>
<lpage>25</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1207/s15327957pspr0101_2</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15647126</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B13">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Calder</surname>
<given-names>A. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Keane</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Manes</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Antoun</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Young</surname>
<given-names>A. W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Impaired recognition and experience of disgust following brain injury</article-title>
.
<source>Nat. Neurosci</source>
.
<volume>3</volume>
,
<fpage>1077</fpage>
<lpage>1078</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1038/80586</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11036262</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B14">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Chan</surname>
<given-names>A.-W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Altman</surname>
<given-names>D. G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Identifying outcome reporting bias in randomised trials on PubMed: review of publications and survey of authors</article-title>
.
<source>BMJ</source>
<volume>330</volume>
,
<fpage>753</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1136/bmj.38356.424606.8F</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15681569</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B15">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Chan</surname>
<given-names>L. P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Livingstone</surname>
<given-names>S. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Russo</surname>
<given-names>F. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Facial mimicry in response to song</article-title>
.
<source>Music Percept</source>
.
<volume>30</volume>
,
<fpage>361</fpage>
<lpage>367</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1525/mp.2013.30.4.361</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B16">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Charland</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>The heat of emotion: valence and the demarcation problem</article-title>
.
<source>J. Conscious. Stud</source>
.
<volume>12</volume>
,
<fpage>82</fpage>
<lpage>102</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B17">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cheng</surname>
<given-names>S.-T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Endowment and contrast: the role of positive and negative emotions on well-being appraisal</article-title>
.
<source>Pers. Indiv. Diff</source>
.
<volume>37</volume>
,
<fpage>905</fpage>
<lpage>915</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.paid.2003.10.013</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B18">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Collins</surname>
<given-names>S. C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1989</year>
).
<source>Subjective and Autonomic Responses to Western Classical Music</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Manchester</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Doctoral Dissertation, University of Manchester</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B19">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Colombetti</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Appraising valence</article-title>
.
<source>J. Conscious. Stud</source>
.
<volume>12</volume>
,
<fpage>103</fpage>
<lpage>126</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B20">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Coren</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Girgus</surname>
<given-names>J. S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1978</year>
).
<source>Seeing is Deceiving: The Psychology of Visual Illusions</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Oxford, England</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Lawrence Erlbaum</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B21">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Coutinho</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cangelosi</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>The use of spatio-temporal connectionist models in psychological studies of musical emotions</article-title>
.
<source>Music Percept</source>
.
<volume>27</volume>
,
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>15</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1525/mp.2009.27.1.1</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B22">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cumming</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Finch</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Inference by eye: confidence intervals and how to read pictures of data</article-title>
.
<source>Am. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>60</volume>
,
<fpage>170</fpage>
<lpage>180</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0003-066X.60.2.170</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15740449</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B23">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Davies</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<source>Musical Meaning and Expression</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Ithaca, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cornell University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B24">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Davies</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Philosophy, music and emotion</article-title>
.
<source>Austr. J. Philos</source>
.
<volume>81</volume>
,
<fpage>281</fpage>
<lpage>283</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/713659615</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B24a">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Dawes</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Do data characteristics change according to the number of scale points used? An experiment using 5 point, 7 point and 10 point scales</article-title>
.
<source>Int. J. Mark. Res</source>
.
<volume>51</volume>
,
<fpage>61</fpage>
<lpage>77</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B25">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Dibben</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>The role of peripheral feedback in emotional experience with music</article-title>
.
<source>Music Percept</source>
.
<volume>22</volume>
,
<fpage>79</fpage>
<lpage>115</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1525/mp.2004.22.1.79</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B26">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Diener</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fujita</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>The personality structure of affect</article-title>
.
<source>J. Person. Soc. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>69</volume>
,
<fpage>130</fpage>
<lpage>130</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.69.1.130</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B27">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Dimberg</surname>
<given-names>U.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thunberg</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Elmehed</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci</source>
.
<volume>11</volume>
,
<fpage>86</fpage>
<lpage>89</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/1467-9280.00221</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11228851</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B28">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Dwan</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Altman</surname>
<given-names>D. G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Arnaiz</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bloom</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Chan</surname>
<given-names>A.-W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cronin</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Systematic review of the empirical evidence of study publication bias and outcome reporting bias</article-title>
.
<source>PLoS ONE</source>
<volume>3</volume>
:
<fpage>e3081</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0003081</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18769481</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B29">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Dyck</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Loughead</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kellermann</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Boers</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gur</surname>
<given-names>R. C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mathiak</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Cognitive versus automatic mechanisms of mood induction differentially activate left and right amygdala</article-title>
.
<source>Neuroimage</source>
<volume>54</volume>
,
<fpage>2503</fpage>
<lpage>2513</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.013</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20946960</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B30">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Eerola</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Vuoskoski</surname>
<given-names>J. K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>A comparison of the discrete and dimensional models of emotion in music</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>39</volume>
,
<fpage>18</fpage>
<lpage>49</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735610362821</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B30a">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ekman</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1972</year>
).
<article-title>Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion</article-title>
, in
<source>Nebraska Symposium on Motivation</source>
, ed.
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Cole</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Lincoln</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Nebraska Press</publisher-name>
).</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B31">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Evans</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Quantification of Gabrielsson's relationships between felt and expressed emotions in music</article-title>
, in
<source>Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Music and Cognition</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Baroni</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Addessi</surname>
<given-names>A. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Caterina</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Costa</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Bologna</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>European Scoiety for the Cognitive Sciences of Music</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>446</fpage>
<lpage>454</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B32">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Evans</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Relationships between expressed and felt emotions in music</article-title>
.
<source>Musicae Sci</source>
.
<volume>12</volume>
,
<fpage>75</fpage>
<lpage>99</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/102986490801200105</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B33">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Frances</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1988</year>
).
<source>The Perception of Music</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Hillsdale, NJ</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B34">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Frijda</surname>
<given-names>N. H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kuipers</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ter Schure</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1989</year>
).
<article-title>Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness</article-title>
.
<source>J. Person. Soc. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>57</volume>
,
<fpage>212</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.57.2.212</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B35">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gabrielsson</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Perceived emotion and felt emotion: same or different?</article-title>
<source>Musicae Sci</source>
.
<volume>6</volume>
,
<fpage>123</fpage>
<lpage>148</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B36">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gabrielsson</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<source>Strong Experiences with Music: Music is Much More Than Just Music</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199695225.001.0001</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B37">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Garrido</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011a</year>
).
<article-title>Individual differences in the enjoyment of negative emotion in music: a literature review and experiment</article-title>
.
<source>Music Percept</source>
.
<volume>28</volume>
,
<fpage>279</fpage>
<lpage>295</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1525/mp.2011.28.3.279</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B38">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Garrido</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011b</year>
).
<article-title>Negative emotion in music: what is the attraction? A qualitative study</article-title>
.
<source>Empir. Musicol. Rev</source>
.
<volume>6</volume>
,
<fpage>214</fpage>
<lpage>230</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B39">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Garrido</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011c</year>
).
<article-title>Rumination and sad music: the anomaly of maladaptive listening habits</article-title>
, in
<source>‘Power of Music’ - the 34th National Conference of the Musicological Society of Australia, in conjunction with the 2nd International Conference on Music and Emotion</source>
(
<publisher-loc>Perth, WA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>The University of Western Australia</publisher-name>
).</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B40">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Garrido</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Adaptive and maladaptive attraction to negative emotions in music</article-title>
.
<source>Musicae Sci</source>
.
<volume>17</volume>
,
<fpage>147</fpage>
<lpage>166</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/1029864913478305</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B41">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gaver</surname>
<given-names>W. W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mandler</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1987</year>
).
<article-title>Play it again, Sam: on liking music</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Emot</source>
.
<volume>1</volume>
,
<fpage>259</fpage>
<lpage>282</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/02699938708408051</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B42">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gendolla</surname>
<given-names>G. H. E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kruesken</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Mood state and cardiovascular response in active coping with an affect-regulative challenge</article-title>
.
<source>Int. J. Psychophysiol</source>
.
<volume>41</volume>
,
<fpage>169</fpage>
<lpage>180</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00130-1</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11325461</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B43">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Goldman</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>Emotions in music (a postscript)</article-title>
.
<source>J. Aesthet. Art crit</source>
.
<volume>53</volume>
,
<fpage>59</fpage>
<lpage>69</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/431737</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B44">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gray</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Watson</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Payne</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cooper</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Emotion, mood, and temperament: similarities, differences, and a synthesis</article-title>
, in
<source>Emotions at Work: Theory, Research and Applications for Management</source>
. (
<publisher-loc>Chichester</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>John WIley</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>21</fpage>
<lpage>44</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B45">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Green</surname>
<given-names>M. C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Transportation into narrative worlds: the role of prior knowledge and perceived realism</article-title>
.
<source>Discourse Process</source>
.
<volume>38</volume>
,
<fpage>247</fpage>
<lpage>266</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1207/s15326950dp3802_5</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B46">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Grewe</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nagel</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Altenmüller</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kopiez</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Individual emotional reactions towards music: evolutionary-based universals?</article-title>
<source>Musicae Sci</source>
.
<volume>13</volume>
,
<fpage>261</fpage>
<lpage>287</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/1029864909013002121</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B47">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Grewe</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nagel</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kopiez</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Altenmüller</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Emotions over time: synchronicity and development of subjective, physiological, and facial affective reactions to music</article-title>
.
<source>Emotion</source>
<volume>7</volume>
,
<fpage>774</fpage>
<lpage>788</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.774</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18039047</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B48">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gross</surname>
<given-names>J. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>John</surname>
<given-names>O. P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Richards</surname>
<given-names>J. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>The dissociation of emotion expression from emotion experience: a personality perspective</article-title>
.
<source>Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull</source>
.
<volume>26</volume>
,
<fpage>712</fpage>
<lpage>726</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0146167200268006</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B49">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hahn</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Williamson</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hutton</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Investigation of within−study selective reporting in clinical research: follow−up of applications submitted to a local research ethics committee</article-title>
.
<source>J. Eval. Clin. Pract</source>
.
<volume>8</volume>
,
<fpage>353</fpage>
<lpage>359</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1046/j.1365-2753.2002.00314.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12164983</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B50">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Harkness</surname>
<given-names>K. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jacobson</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Duong</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sabbagh</surname>
<given-names>M. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Mental state decoding in past major depression: effect of sad versus happy mood induction</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Emot</source>
.
<volume>24</volume>
,
<fpage>497</fpage>
<lpage>513</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/02699930902750249</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B51">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Herbert</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>An empirical study of normative dissociation in musical and non-musical everyday life experiences</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>41</volume>
,
<fpage>372</fpage>
<lpage>394</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735611430080</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B52">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hevner</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1936</year>
).
<article-title>Experimental studies of the elements of expression in music</article-title>
.
<source>Am. J. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>48</volume>
,
<fpage>246</fpage>
<lpage>268</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/1415746</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19884142</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B53">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hevner</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1937</year>
).
<article-title>The affective value of pitch and tempo in music</article-title>
.
<source>Am. J. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>49</volume>
,
<fpage>621</fpage>
<lpage>630</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/1416385</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B54">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hoeckner</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wyatt</surname>
<given-names>E. W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Decety</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nusbaum</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Film music influences how viewers relate to movie characters</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts</source>
<volume>5</volume>
,
<fpage>146</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0021544</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B55">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hunter</surname>
<given-names>P. G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schellenberg</surname>
<given-names>E. G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schimmack</surname>
<given-names>U.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Mixed affective responses to music with conflicting cues</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Emot</source>
.
<volume>22</volume>
,
<fpage>327</fpage>
<lpage>352</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/02699930701438145</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21707144</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B56">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hunter</surname>
<given-names>P. G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schellenberg</surname>
<given-names>E. G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schimmack</surname>
<given-names>U.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Feelings and perceptions of happiness and sadness induced by music: similarities, differences, and mixed emotions</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts</source>
<volume>4</volume>
,
<fpage>47</fpage>
<lpage>56</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0016873</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B57">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Huron</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Why is sad music pleasurable? A possible role for prolactin</article-title>
.
<source>Musicae Sci</source>
.
<volume>15</volume>
,
<fpage>146</fpage>
<lpage>158</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/1029864911401171</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B58">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Iacoboni</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons</article-title>
.
<source>Annu. Rev. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>60</volume>
,
<fpage>653</fpage>
<lpage>670</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163604</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18793090</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B59">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ilie</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thompson</surname>
<given-names>W. F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>A comparison of acoustic cues in music and speech for three dimensions of affect</article-title>
.
<source>Music Percept</source>
.
<volume>23</volume>
,
<fpage>319</fpage>
<lpage>329</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1525/mp.2006.23.4.319</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B60">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ilie</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thompson</surname>
<given-names>W. F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Experiential and cognitive changes following seven minutes exposure to music and speech</article-title>
.
<source>Music Percept</source>
.
<volume>28</volume>
,
<fpage>247</fpage>
<lpage>264</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1525/mp.2011.28.3.247</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B61">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Jakobs</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Manstead</surname>
<given-names>A. S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fischer</surname>
<given-names>A. H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Social context effects on facial activity in a negative emotional setting</article-title>
.
<source>Emotion</source>
<volume>1</volume>
,
<fpage>51</fpage>
<lpage>69</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/1528-3542.1.1.51</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12894811</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B62">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1997</year>
).
<article-title>Perceived emotional expression in synthesized performances of a short melody: capturing the listener's judgment policy</article-title>
.
<source>Musicae Sci</source>
.
<volume>1</volume>
,
<fpage>225</fpage>
<lpage>256</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B63">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Cue utilization in communication of emotion in music performance: relating performance to perception</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform</source>
.
<volume>26</volume>
,
<fpage>1797</fpage>
<lpage>1813</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0096-1523.26.6.1797</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11129375</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B64">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Music and emotion: seven questions, seven answers</article-title>
, in
<source>Music and the mind: Essays in honour of John Sloboda</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Deliège</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Davidson</surname>
<given-names>J. W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>OUP</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>113</fpage>
<lpage>135</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B65">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>From everyday emotions to aesthetic emotions: towards a unified theory of musical emotions</article-title>
.
<source>Phys. Life Rev</source>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.plrev.2013.05.008</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B66">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Friberg</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bresin</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Toward a computational model of expression in music performance: the GERM model</article-title>
.
<source>Musicae Sci</source>
.
<volume>5</volume>
,
<fpage>63</fpage>
<lpage>122</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/10298649020050S104</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B67">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Laukka</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Expression, perception, and induction of musical emotions: a review and a questionnaire study of everyday listening</article-title>
.
<source>J. New Music Res</source>
.
<volume>33</volume>
,
<fpage>217</fpage>
<lpage>238</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/0929821042000317813</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B68">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Liljeström</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Laukka</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Västfjäll</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lundqvist</surname>
<given-names>L.-O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Emotional reactions to music in a nationally representative sample of Swedish adults Prevalence and causal influences</article-title>
.
<source>Musicae Sci</source>
.
<volume>15</volume>
,
<fpage>174</fpage>
<lpage>207</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/1029864911401169</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B69">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lindström</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Musical expression of emotions: modelling listeners' judgements of composed and performed features</article-title>
.
<source>Music Analysis</source>
<volume>29</volume>
,
<fpage>334</fpage>
<lpage>364</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1468-2249.2011.00323.x</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B70">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Västfjäll</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Emotional responses to music: the need to consider underlying mechanisms</article-title>
.
<source>Behav. Brain Sci</source>
.
<volume>31</volume>
,
<fpage>559</fpage>
<lpage>575</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1017/S0140525X08005293</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18826699</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B71">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kallinen</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ravaja</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Emotion perceived and emotion felt: same and different</article-title>
.
<source>Musicae Sci</source>
.
<volume>10</volume>
,
<fpage>191</fpage>
<lpage>213</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/102986490601000203</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B72">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kawakami</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Furukawa</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Katahira</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kamiyama</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Okanoya</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Relations Between Musical Structures and Perceived and Felt Emotions</article-title>
.
<source>Music Percept</source>
.
<volume>30</volume>
,
<fpage>407</fpage>
<lpage>417</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1525/mp.2013.30.4.407</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17516815</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B73">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kenealy</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1988</year>
).
<article-title>Validation of a music mood induction procedure: some preliminary findings</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Emot</source>
.
<volume>2</volume>
,
<fpage>41</fpage>
<lpage>48</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/02699938808415228</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B74">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Khalfa</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Isabelle</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jean-Pierre</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Manon</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Event-related skin conductance responses to musical emotions in humans</article-title>
.
<source>Neurosci. Lett</source>
.
<volume>328</volume>
,
<fpage>145</fpage>
<lpage>149</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00462-7</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12133576</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B75">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kivy</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1989</year>
).
<source>Sound Sentiment: An Essay on the Musical Emotions, Including the Complete Text of The Corded Shell</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Philadelphia, PA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Temple University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B76">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kivy</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<source>Music Alone: Philosophical Reflections on the Purely Musical Experience</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Ithaca</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cornell University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B77">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kivy</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<article-title>Feeling the musical emotions (A philosophical approach)</article-title>
.
<source>Br. J. Aesthet</source>
.
<volume>39</volume>
,
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>13</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1093/bjaesthetics/39.1.1</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B78">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kivy</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<source>Introduction to a Philosophy of Music</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Oxford, UK</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Clarendon</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B79">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Knobloch</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zillmann</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Mood management via the digital jukebox</article-title>
.
<source>J. Commun</source>
.
<volume>52</volume>
,
<fpage>351</fpage>
<lpage>366</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1460-2466.2002.tb02549.x</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B80">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Koelsch</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fritz</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Müller</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Friederici</surname>
<given-names>A. D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Investigating emotion with music: an fMRI study</article-title>
.
<source>Hum. Brain Mapp</source>
.
<volume>27</volume>
,
<fpage>239</fpage>
<lpage>250</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1002/hbm.20180</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16078183</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B81">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Konečni</surname>
<given-names>V. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>The aesthetic trinity: awe, being moved, thrills</article-title>
.
<source>Bull. Psychol. Arts</source>
<volume>5</volume>
,
<fpage>27</fpage>
<lpage>44</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B82">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Konečni</surname>
<given-names>V. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Does music induce emotion? a theoretical and methodological analysis</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts</source>
<volume>2</volume>
,
<fpage>115</fpage>
<lpage>129</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/1931-3896.2.2.115</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B83">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Konečni</surname>
<given-names>V. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brown</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wanic</surname>
<given-names>R. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Comparative effects of music and recalled life-events on emotional state</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>36</volume>
,
<fpage>289</fpage>
<lpage>308</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735607082621</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B84">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Krause</surname>
<given-names>A. E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hargreaves</surname>
<given-names>D. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>myTunes: digital music library users and their self-images</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>41</volume>
,
<fpage>531</fpage>
<lpage>544</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735612440612</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B85">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kreutz</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bongard</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Von Jussis</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Cardiovascular responses to music listening: the effects of musical expertise and emotional expression</article-title>
.
<source>Musicae Sci</source>
.
<volume>6</volume>
,
<fpage>257</fpage>
<lpage>278</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B86">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kreutz</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ott</surname>
<given-names>U.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Teichmann</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Osawa</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Vaitl</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008a</year>
).
<article-title>Using music to induce emotions: influences of musical preference and absorption</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>36</volume>
,
<fpage>101</fpage>
<lpage>126</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735607082623</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B87">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kreutz</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mitchell</surname>
<given-names>L. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008b</year>
).
<article-title>Cognitive styles of music listening</article-title>
.
<source>Music Percept</source>
.
<volume>26</volume>
,
<fpage>57</fpage>
<lpage>73</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1525/mp.2008.26.1.57</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B88">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Krumhansl</surname>
<given-names>C. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1997</year>
).
<article-title>An exploratory study of musical emotions and psychophysiology</article-title>
.
<source>Can. J. Exp. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>51</volume>
,
<fpage>336</fpage>
<lpage>352</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/1196-1961.51.4.336</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">9606949</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B89">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Krumhansl</surname>
<given-names>C. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Music: a link between cognition and emotion</article-title>
.
<source>Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci</source>
.
<volume>11</volume>
,
<fpage>45</fpage>
<lpage>50</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/1467-8721.00165</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B90">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Langer</surname>
<given-names>S. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rudd</surname>
<given-names>M. E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Syrjala</surname>
<given-names>K. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Protective buffering and emotional desynchrony among spousal caregivers of cancer patients</article-title>
.
<source>Health Psychol</source>
.
<volume>26</volume>
,
<fpage>635</fpage>
<lpage>643</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0278-6133.26.5.635</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17845115</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B91">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Larcom</surname>
<given-names>M. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Isaacowitz</surname>
<given-names>D. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Rapid emotion regulation after mood induction: age and individual differences</article-title>
.
<source>J. Gerontol. B. Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci</source>
.
<volume>64</volume>
,
<fpage>733</fpage>
<lpage>741</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1093/geronb/gbp077</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19808810</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B92">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Larsen</surname>
<given-names>J. T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mcgraw</surname>
<given-names>A. P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cacioppo</surname>
<given-names>J. T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Can People Feel Happy and Sad at the Same Time?</article-title>
<source>J. Person. Soc. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>81</volume>
,
<fpage>684</fpage>
<lpage>696</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.684</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11642354</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B93">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lee</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1932</year>
).
<source>Music and Its Lovers: An Empirical Study of Emotional and Imaginative Responses to Music</source>
.
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>George Allen and Unwin Ltd</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B94">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Liljeström</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Västfjäll</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Experimental evidence of the roles of music choice, social context, and listener personality in emotional reactions to music</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>41</volume>
,
<fpage>579</fpage>
<lpage>599</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735612440615</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B95">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lundqvist</surname>
<given-names>L.-O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Carlsson</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hilmersson</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Emotional responses to music: experience, expression, and physiology</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>37</volume>
,
<fpage>61</fpage>
<lpage>90</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735607086048</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B96">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lychner</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1998</year>
).
<article-title>An empirical study concerning terminology relating to aesthetic response to music</article-title>
.
<source>J. Res. Music Educ</source>
.
<volume>46</volume>
,
<fpage>303</fpage>
<lpage>319</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/3345630</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B97">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Mann</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>A health-care model of emotional labour: an evaluation of the literature and development of a model</article-title>
.
<source>J. Health Organ. Manag</source>
.
<volume>19</volume>
,
<fpage>304</fpage>
<lpage>317</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1108/14777260510615369</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16206916</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B98">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Manne</surname>
<given-names>S. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Norton</surname>
<given-names>T. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ostroff</surname>
<given-names>J. S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Winkel</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fox</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Grana</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Protective buffering and psychological distress among couples coping with breast cancer: the moderating role of relationship satisfaction</article-title>
.
<source>J. Fam. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>21</volume>
,
<fpage>380</fpage>
<lpage>388</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0893-3200.21.3.380</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17874923</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B99">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Mardia</surname>
<given-names>K. V.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jupp</surname>
<given-names>P. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<source>Directional Statistics</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Chichester</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>John Wiley and Sons, Ltd</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B100a">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Matsumoto</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<article-title>Cultural similarities and differences in display rules</article-title>
.
<source>Motiv. Emot</source>
.
<volume>14</volume>
,
<fpage>195</fpage>
<lpage>214</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/BF00995569</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B100">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Matsumoto</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kupperbusch</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Idiocentric and allocentric differences in emotional expression, experience, and the coherence between expression and experience</article-title>
.
<source>Asian J. Soc. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>4</volume>
,
<fpage>113</fpage>
<lpage>131</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1467-839X.2001.00080.x</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B101">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Matsumoto</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Why people listen to sad music: effects of music on sad moods</article-title>
.
<source>Jpn. J. Educ. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>50</volume>
,
<fpage>23</fpage>
<lpage>32</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B102">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Menon</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Levitin</surname>
<given-names>D. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>The rewards of music listening: response and physiological connectivity of the mesolimbic system</article-title>
.
<source>Neuroimage</source>
<volume>28</volume>
,
<fpage>175</fpage>
<lpage>184</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.053</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16023376</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B103">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Meyer</surname>
<given-names>L. B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1956</year>
).
<source>Emotion and Meaning in Music</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Chicago, IL</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Chicago Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B104">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Miall</surname>
<given-names>D. S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Emotions and the structuring of narrative responses</article-title>
.
<source>Poetics Today</source>
<volume>32</volume>
,
<fpage>323</fpage>
<lpage>348</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1215/03335372-1162704</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B104a">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Mitchell</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Revisiting truth or triviality the external validity of research in the psychological laboratory</article-title>
.
<source>Perspect. Psychol. Sci</source>
.
<volume>7</volume>
,
<fpage>109</fpage>
<lpage>117</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/1745691611432343</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B105">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Moher</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Liberati</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tetzlaff</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Altman</surname>
<given-names>D. G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement</article-title>
.
<source>Ann. Int. Med</source>
.
<volume>151</volume>
,
<fpage>264</fpage>
<lpage>269</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19622511</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B106">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Nagel</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kopiez</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Grewe</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Altenmüller</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>EMuJoy: software for continuous measurement of perceived emotions in music</article-title>
.
<source>Behav. Res. Methods</source>
<volume>39</volume>
,
<fpage>283</fpage>
<lpage>290</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3758/BF03193159</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17695356</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B107">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Novembre</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ticini</surname>
<given-names>L. F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schütz-Bosbach</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Keller</surname>
<given-names>P. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Distinguishing self and other in joint action. Evidence from a musical paradigm</article-title>
.
<source>Cereb. Cortex</source>
<volume>22</volume>
,
<fpage>2894</fpage>
<lpage>2903</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1093/cercor/bhr364</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22235034</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B108">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Oatley</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>A taxonomy of the emotions of literary response and a theory of identification in fictional narrative</article-title>
.
<source>Poetics</source>
<volume>23</volume>
,
<fpage>53</fpage>
<lpage>74</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0304-422X(94)P4296-S</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B109">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Panagiotidi</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Samartzi</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Time estimation: musical training and emotional content of stimuli</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>41</volume>
,
<fpage>620</fpage>
<lpage>629</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735612441737</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B110">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Panksepp</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>The emotional sources of “chills” induced by music</article-title>
.
<source>Music Percept</source>
.
<volume>13</volume>
,
<fpage>171</fpage>
<lpage>207</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/40285693</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B111">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Payne</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1980</year>
).
<article-title>Towards an understanding of music appreciation</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>8</volume>
,
<fpage>31</fpage>
<lpage>41</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/030573568082004</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B112">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Peretz</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Towards a neurobiology of musical emotions</article-title>
, in
<source>Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, Applications</source>
, eds.
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sloboda</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford, UK</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>99</fpage>
<lpage>126</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B113">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Peretz</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gagnon</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hebert</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Macoir</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Singing in the brain: insights from cognitive neuropsychology</article-title>
.
<source>Music Percept</source>
.
<volume>21</volume>
,
<fpage>373</fpage>
<lpage>390</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1525/mp.2004.21.3.373</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B114">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Perlovsky</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Musical emotions: functions, origins, evolution</article-title>
.
<source>Phys. Life Rev</source>
.
<volume>7</volume>
,
<fpage>2</fpage>
<lpage>27</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.plrev.2009.11.001</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20374916</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B115">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Pouivet</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>On the cognitive functioning of aesthetic emotions</article-title>
.
<source>Leonardo</source>
<volume>33</volume>
,
<fpage>49</fpage>
<lpage>53</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1162/002409400552234</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B116">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Preston</surname>
<given-names>S. D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>De Waal</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases</article-title>
.
<source>Behav. Brain Sci</source>
.
<volume>25</volume>
,
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>20</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1017/S0140525X02000018</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12625087</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B117">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Pugh</surname>
<given-names>S. D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Service with a smile: emotional contagion in the service encounter</article-title>
.
<source>Acad. Manag. J</source>
.
<volume>44</volume>
,
<fpage>1018</fpage>
<lpage>1027</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/3069445</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B118">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Radford</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1989</year>
).
<article-title>Emotions and music: a reply to the cognitivists</article-title>
.
<source>J. Aesthet. Art Crit</source>
.
<volume>47</volume>
,
<fpage>69</fpage>
<lpage>76</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/431994</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B119">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Raghunathan</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Corfman</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Is happiness shared doubled and sadness shared halved? Social influence on enjoyment of hedonic experiences</article-title>
.
<source>J. Mark. Res</source>
.
<volume>43</volume>
,
<fpage>386</fpage>
<lpage>394</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1509/jmkr.43.3.386</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B120">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Reid</surname>
<given-names>S. C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kauer</surname>
<given-names>S. D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dudgeon</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sanci</surname>
<given-names>L. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Shrier</surname>
<given-names>L. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Patton</surname>
<given-names>G. C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>A mobile phone program to track young people's experiences of mood, stress and coping</article-title>
.
<source>Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol</source>
.
<volume>44</volume>
,
<fpage>501</fpage>
<lpage>507</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s00127-008-0455-5</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19011723</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B121">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rentfrow</surname>
<given-names>P. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Goldberg</surname>
<given-names>L. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Levitin</surname>
<given-names>D. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>The structure of musical preferences: a five-factor model</article-title>
.
<source>J. Person. Soc. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>100</volume>
,
<fpage>1139</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0022406</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21299309</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B122">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rentfrow</surname>
<given-names>P. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gosling</surname>
<given-names>S. D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>The do re mi's of everyday life: the structure and personality correlates of music preferences</article-title>
.
<source>J. Pers. Soc. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>84</volume>
,
<fpage>1236</fpage>
<lpage>1256</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.84.6.1236</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12793587</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B123">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rickard</surname>
<given-names>N. S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Intense emotional responses to music: a test of the physiological arousal hypothesis</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>32</volume>
,
<fpage>371</fpage>
<lpage>388</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735604046096</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B124">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Robinson</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>The expression and arousal of emotion in music</article-title>
.
<source>J. Aesthet. Art Crit</source>
.
<volume>52</volume>
,
<fpage>13</fpage>
<lpage>22</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/431581</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B125">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<collab>Romantic(ism).</collab>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<source>The Oxford Dictionary of Music, 2nd Ed. rev. Oxford Music Online</source>
.
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
Available online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.oxfordmusiconline.com/subscriber/article/opr/t237/e8648">http://www.oxfordmusiconline.com/subscriber/article/opr/t237/e8648</ext-link>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B126">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Roy</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mailhot</surname>
<given-names>J.-P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gosselin</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Paquette</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Peretz</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Modulation of the startle reflex by pleasant and unpleasant music</article-title>
.
<source>Int. J. Psychophysiol</source>
.
<volume>71</volume>
,
<fpage>37</fpage>
<lpage>42</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.07.010</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18725255</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B127">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Roy</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Peretz</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rainville</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Emotional valence contributes to music-induced analgesia</article-title>
.
<source>Pain</source>
<volume>134</volume>
,
<fpage>140</fpage>
<lpage>147</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.pain.2007.04.003</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17532141</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B128">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Russell</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1979</year>
).
<article-title>Affective space is bipolar</article-title>
.
<source>J. Person. Soc. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>37</volume>
,
<fpage>345</fpage>
<lpage>356</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.37.3.345</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B129">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Russell</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1980</year>
).
<article-title>A circumplex model of affect</article-title>
.
<source>J. Soc. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>39</volume>
,
<fpage>1161</fpage>
<lpage>1178</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/h007774</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B130">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Russell</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lewicka</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Niit</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1989a</year>
).
<article-title>A cross-cultural study of a circumplex model of affect</article-title>
.
<source>J. Person. Soc. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>57</volume>
,
<fpage>848</fpage>
<lpage>856</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.57.5.848</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B131">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Russell</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Weiss</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mendelsohn</surname>
<given-names>G. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1989b</year>
).
<article-title>Affect grid: a single-item scale of pleasure and arousal</article-title>
.
<source>J. Person. Soc. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>57</volume>
,
<fpage>493</fpage>
<lpage>502</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.57.3.493</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">9819938</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B132">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Saarikallio</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Erkkilä</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>The role of music in adolescents' mood regulation</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>35</volume>
,
<fpage>88</fpage>
<lpage>109</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735607068889</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20671333</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B133">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Salimpoor</surname>
<given-names>V. N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Benovoy</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Longo</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cooperstock</surname>
<given-names>J. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zatorre</surname>
<given-names>R. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>The rewarding aspects of music listening are related to degree of emotional arousal</article-title>
.
<source>PLoS ONE</source>
<volume>4</volume>
:
<fpage>e7487</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0007487</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19834599</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B134">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sato</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fujimura</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kochiyama</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Suzuki</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Relationships among Facial Mimicry, Emotional Experience, and Emotion Recognition</article-title>
.
<source>PLoS ONE</source>
<volume>8</volume>
:
<fpage>e57889</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0057889</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23536774</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B135">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schenker</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gentleman</surname>
<given-names>J. F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>On judging the significance of differences by examining the overlap between confidence intervals</article-title>
.
<source>Am. Stat</source>
.
<volume>55</volume>
,
<fpage>182</fpage>
<lpage>186</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1198/000313001317097960</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B136">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Scherer</surname>
<given-names>K. R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Which emotions can be induced by music? What are the underlying mechanisms? And how can we measure them?</article-title>
<source>J. New Music Res</source>
.
<volume>33</volume>
,
<fpage>239</fpage>
<lpage>251</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/0929821042000317822</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B137">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Scherer</surname>
<given-names>K. R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>What are emotions? And how can they be measured?</article-title>
<source>Soc. Sci. Inform</source>
.
<volume>44</volume>
,
<fpage>695</fpage>
<lpage>729</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0539018405058216</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B138">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Scherer</surname>
<given-names>K. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zentner</surname>
<given-names>M. R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Emotional effects of music: production rules</article-title>
, in
<source>Music and Emotion: Theory and Research</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sloboda</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>361</fpage>
<lpage>392</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B138a">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schimmack</surname>
<given-names>U.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rainer</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Experiencing activation: energetic arousal and tense arousal are not mixtures of valence and activation</article-title>
.
<source>Emotion</source>
<volume>2</volume>
,
<fpage>412</fpage>
<lpage>417</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037//1528-3542.2.4.412</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12899373</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B139">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1996</year>
).
<article-title>Enjoyment of negative emotions in music: an associative network explanation</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>24</volume>
,
<fpage>18</fpage>
<lpage>28</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735696241003</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B140">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<article-title>Measuring emotion continuously: validity and reliability of the two-dimensional emotion-space</article-title>
.
<source>Austr. J. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>51</volume>
,
<fpage>154</fpage>
<lpage>165</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/00049539908255353</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B141">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007a</year>
).
<article-title>The influence of emotion, locus of emotion and familiarity upon preference in music</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>35</volume>
,
<fpage>499</fpage>
<lpage>515</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735607072657</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B142">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007b</year>
).
<article-title>Locus of emotion: the effect of task order and age on emotion perceived and emotion felt in response to music</article-title>
.
<source>J. Music Ther</source>
.
<volume>44</volume>
,
<fpage>344</fpage>
<lpage>368</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17997625</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B143">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009–2010</year>
).
<article-title>The fundamental function of music</article-title>
.
<source>Musicae Sci</source>
.
<volume>13</volume>
,
<fpage>63</fpage>
<lpage>81</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/1029864909013002051</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B144">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Affective, evaluative and collative responses to hated and loved music</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts</source>
<volume>4</volume>
,
<fpage>36</fpage>
<lpage>46</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0016316</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B145">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Using affect valence and emotion valence to understand musical experience and response: the case of hated music</article-title>
, in
<source>Proceedings of the 30th ISME World Conference on Music Education Music Pædeia: From Ancient Greek Philosophers Toward Global Music Communities</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Sims</surname>
<given-names>W. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Nedlands, WA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>International Society for Music Education</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>317</fpage>
<lpage>324</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B146">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Loved music can make a listener feel negative emotions</article-title>
.
<source>Musicae Sci</source>
.
<volume>17</volume>
,
<fpage>11</fpage>
<lpage>26</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/1029864912461321</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B147">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McPherson</surname>
<given-names>G. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>The perception of emotion in music</article-title>
, in
<source>The Child as Musician: A Handbook of Musical Development</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>McPherson</surname>
<given-names>G. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>193</fpage>
<lpage>212</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B148">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schwarz</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Strack</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<article-title>Reports of subjective well-being: judgmental processes and their methodological implications</article-title>
, in
<source>Well-being: The foundations of Hedonic psychology</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Kahneman</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Diener</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schwarz</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Russell Sage Foundation</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>61</fpage>
<lpage>84</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B149">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Scruton</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1983</year>
).
<source>The Aesthetic Understanding: Essays in the Philosophy of Art and Culture</source>
.
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Methuen</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B150">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sevdalis</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Keller</surname>
<given-names>P. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Self−recognition in the Perception of Actions Performed in Synchrony with Music</article-title>
.
<source>Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci</source>
.
<volume>1169</volume>
,
<fpage>499</fpage>
<lpage>502</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04773.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19673830</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B151">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Silvia</surname>
<given-names>P. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Looking past pleasure: anger, confusion, disgust, pride, surprise, and other unusual aesthetic emotions</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts</source>
<volume>3</volume>
,
<fpage>48</fpage>
<lpage>51</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0014632</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B152">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Silvia</surname>
<given-names>P. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brown</surname>
<given-names>E. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Anger, disgust, and the negative aesthetic emotions: expanding an appraisal model of aesthetic experience</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts</source>
<volume>1</volume>
,
<fpage>100</fpage>
<lpage>106</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/1931-3896.1.2.100</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B153">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sloboda</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1992</year>
).
<article-title>Empirical studies of emotional response to music</article-title>
, in
<source>Cognitive Bases of Musical Communication</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Jones</surname>
<given-names>M. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Holleran</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Washington, DC</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>American Psychological Association</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>33</fpage>
<lpage>46</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/10104-003</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B154">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sloboda</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1996</year>
).
<article-title>Emotional responses to music: a review</article-title>
, in
<source>Proceedings of the Nordic Acoustical Meeting</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Riederer</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lahti</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Helsinki</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>The Acoustical Society of Finland</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>385</fpage>
<lpage>392</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B155">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sloboda</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>At the interface between the inner and outer world: psychological perspectives</article-title>
, in
<source>Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, Applications</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Sloboda</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford, UK</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>OUP</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>73</fpage>
<lpage>97</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B156">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Swanwick</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1975</year>
).
<article-title>Can there be objectivity in listening to music?</article-title>
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>3</volume>
,
<fpage>17</fpage>
<lpage>23</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/030573567532003</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B157">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Thaut</surname>
<given-names>M. H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>De L'etoile</surname>
<given-names>S. K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1993</year>
).
<article-title>The effects of music on mood state-dependent recall</article-title>
.
<source>J. Music Ther</source>
.
<volume>30</volume>
,
<fpage>70</fpage>
<lpage>80</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B158">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Thayer</surname>
<given-names>J. F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1986</year>
).
<source>Multiple Indicators of Affective Response to Music</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Doctoral Dissertation, New York University</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B159">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Townsend</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1987</year>
).
<article-title>From Shaftesbury to Kant: the development of the concept of aesthetic experience</article-title>
.
<source>J. Hist. Ideas</source>
<volume>48</volume>
,
<fpage>287</fpage>
<lpage>305</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/2709559</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B160">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Valentine</surname>
<given-names>C. W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1962</year>
).
<source>The Experimental Psychology of Beauty</source>
.
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Methuen London</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B161">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Van Zijl</surname>
<given-names>A. G. W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sloboda</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Performers' experienced emotions in the construction of expressive musical performance: an exploratory investigation</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>39</volume>
,
<fpage>196</fpage>
<lpage>219</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735610373563</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B162">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Vastfjall</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Emotion induction through music: a review of the musical mood induction procedure</article-title>
.
<source>Musicae Sci</source>
.
<volume>5</volume>
,
<fpage>173</fpage>
<lpage>211</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/10298649020050S107</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B163">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Vieillard</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Peretz</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gosselin</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Khalfa</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gagnon</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bouchard</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Happy, sad, scary and peaceful musical excerpts for research on emotions</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Emot</source>
.
<volume>22</volume>
,
<fpage>720</fpage>
<lpage>752</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/02699930701503567</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B164a">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Viswanathan</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sudman</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Johnson</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Maximum versus meaningful discrimination in scale response: implications for validity of measurement of consumer perceptions about products</article-title>
.
<source>J. Bus. Res</source>
.
<volume>57</volume>
,
<fpage>108</fpage>
<lpage>124</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0148-2963(01)00296-X</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B165">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Vuoskoski</surname>
<given-names>J. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Eerola</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Can sad music really make you sad? Indirect measures of affective states induced by music and autobiographical memories</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts</source>
<volume>6</volume>
,
<fpage>204</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0026937</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B166">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Vuoskoski</surname>
<given-names>J. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thompson</surname>
<given-names>W. F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mcilwain</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Eerola</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Who enjoys listening to sad music and why?</article-title>
<source>Music Percept</source>
.
<volume>29</volume>
,
<fpage>311</fpage>
<lpage>317</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1525/mp.2012.29.3.311</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B167">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wang</surname>
<given-names>H. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cheong</surname>
<given-names>L. F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Affective understanding in film</article-title>
.
<source>IEEE Trans. Circ. Syst. Video Technol</source>
.
<volume>16</volume>
,
<fpage>689</fpage>
<lpage>704</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1109/TCSVT.2006.873781</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B168">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Weld</surname>
<given-names>H. P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1912</year>
).
<article-title>An experimental study of musical enjoyment</article-title>
.
<source>Am. J. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>23</volume>
,
<fpage>245</fpage>
<lpage>308</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/1412844</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B169">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Werner</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Roberts</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rosen</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dean</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kramer</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Weiner</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Emotional reactivity and emotion recognition in frontotemporal lobar degeneration</article-title>
.
<source>Neurology</source>
<volume>69</volume>
,
<fpage>148</fpage>
<lpage>155</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1212/01.wnl.0000265589.32060.d3</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17620547</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B170">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wilhelm</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gillis</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schubert</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Whittle</surname>
<given-names>E. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>On a blue note: depressed peoples' reasons for listening to music</article-title>
.
<source>Mus. Med</source>
.
<volume>5</volume>
,
<fpage>76</fpage>
<lpage>83</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/1943862113482143</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B171">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Williamson</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gamble</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Altman</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hutton</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Outcome selection bias in meta-analysis</article-title>
.
<source>Stat. Methods Med. Res</source>
.
<volume>14</volume>
,
<fpage>515</fpage>
<lpage>524</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1191/0962280205sm415oa</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16248351</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B172">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Yorke</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Bipolarity… or not? Some conceptual problems relating to bipolar rating scales</article-title>
.
<source>Br. Educ. Res. J</source>
.
<volume>27</volume>
,
<fpage>171</fpage>
<lpage>186</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/01411920120037126</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B173a">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Zentner</surname>
<given-names>M. R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>The nature of musical emotions</article-title>
, in
<source>Sixth International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Woods</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Luck</surname>
<given-names>G. B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brochard</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Seddon</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sloboda</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-name>Department of Psychology</publisher-name>
).</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B173">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Zentner</surname>
<given-names>M. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Grandjean</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Scherer</surname>
<given-names>K. R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Emotions evoked by the sound of music: characterization, classification, and measurement</article-title>
.
<source>Emotion</source>
<volume>8</volume>
,
<fpage>494</fpage>
<lpage>521</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.494</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18729581</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</pmc>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Wicri/Sarre/explor/MusicSarreV3/Data/Pmc/Corpus
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 000190 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Corpus/biblio.hfd -nk 000190 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Wicri/Sarre
   |area=    MusicSarreV3
   |flux=    Pmc
   |étape=   Corpus
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     PMC:3865445
   |texte=   Emotion felt by the listener and expressed by the music: literature review and theoretical perspectives
}}

Pour générer des pages wiki

HfdIndexSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Corpus/RBID.i   -Sk "pubmed:24381565" \
       | HfdSelect -Kh $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Corpus/biblio.hfd   \
       | NlmPubMed2Wicri -a MusicSarreV3 

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.33.
Data generation: Sun Jul 15 18:16:09 2018. Site generation: Tue Mar 5 19:21:25 2024