Serveur d'exploration sur Mozart

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

Gender and the performance of music

Identifieur interne : 000130 ( Pmc/Checkpoint ); précédent : 000129; suivant : 000131

Gender and the performance of music

Auteurs : Desmond C. Sergeant ; Evangelos Himonides

Source :

RBID : PMC:3997045

Abstract

This study evaluates propositions that have appeared in the literature that music phenomena are gendered. Were they present in the musical “message,” gendered qualities might be imparted at any of three stages of the music–communication interchange: the process of composition, its realization into sound by the performer, or imposed by the listener in the process of perception. The research was designed to obtain empirical evidence to enable evaluation of claims of the presence of gendering at these three stages. Three research hypotheses were identified and relevant literature of music behaviors and perception reviewed. New instruments of measurement were constructed to test the three hypotheses: (i) two listening sequences each containing 35 extracts from published recordings of compositions of the classical music repertoire, (ii) four “music characteristics” scales, with polarities defined by verbal descriptors designed to assess the dynamic and emotional valence of the musical extracts featured in the listening sequences. 69 musically-trained listeners listened to the two sequences and were asked to identify the sex of the performing artist of each musical extract; a second group of 23 listeners evaluated the extracts applying the four music characteristics scales. Results did not support claims that music structures are inherently gendered, nor proposals that performers impart their own-sex-specific qualities to the music. It is concluded that gendered properties are imposed subjectively by the listener, and these are primarily related to the tempo of the music.


Url:
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00276
PubMed: 24795663
PubMed Central: 3997045


Affiliations:


Links toward previous steps (curation, corpus...)


Links to Exploration step

PMC:3997045

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI>
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">Gender and the performance of music</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sergeant, Desmond C" sort="Sergeant, Desmond C" uniqKey="Sergeant D" first="Desmond C." last="Sergeant">Desmond C. Sergeant</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Himonides, Evangelos" sort="Himonides, Evangelos" uniqKey="Himonides E" first="Evangelos" last="Himonides">Evangelos Himonides</name>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">PMC</idno>
<idno type="pmid">24795663</idno>
<idno type="pmc">3997045</idno>
<idno type="url">http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3997045</idno>
<idno type="RBID">PMC:3997045</idno>
<idno type="doi">10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00276</idno>
<date when="2014">2014</date>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Corpus">000720</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Curation">000720</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Checkpoint">000130</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title xml:lang="en" level="a" type="main">Gender and the performance of music</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sergeant, Desmond C" sort="Sergeant, Desmond C" uniqKey="Sergeant D" first="Desmond C." last="Sergeant">Desmond C. Sergeant</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Himonides, Evangelos" sort="Himonides, Evangelos" uniqKey="Himonides E" first="Evangelos" last="Himonides">Evangelos Himonides</name>
</author>
</analytic>
<series>
<title level="j">Frontiers in Psychology</title>
<idno type="e-ISSN">1664-1078</idno>
<imprint>
<date when="2014">2014</date>
</imprint>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">
<p>This study evaluates propositions that have appeared in the literature that music phenomena are gendered. Were they present in the musical “message,” gendered qualities might be imparted at any of three stages of the music–communication interchange: the process of composition, its realization into sound by the performer, or imposed by the listener in the process of perception. The research was designed to obtain empirical evidence to enable evaluation of claims of the presence of gendering at these three stages. Three research hypotheses were identified and relevant literature of music behaviors and perception reviewed. New instruments of measurement were constructed to test the three hypotheses: (i) two listening sequences each containing 35 extracts from published recordings of compositions of the classical music repertoire, (ii) four “music characteristics” scales, with polarities defined by verbal descriptors designed to assess the dynamic and emotional valence of the musical extracts featured in the listening sequences. 69 musically-trained listeners listened to the two sequences and were asked to identify the sex of the performing artist of each musical extract; a second group of 23 listeners evaluated the extracts applying the four music characteristics scales. Results did not support claims that music structures are inherently gendered, nor proposals that performers impart their own-sex-specific qualities to the music. It is concluded that gendered properties are imposed subjectively by the listener, and these are primarily related to the tempo of the music.</p>
</div>
</front>
<back>
<div1 type="bibliography">
<listBibl>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Asmus, E P" uniqKey="Asmus E">E. P. Asmus</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Balkwill, L L" uniqKey="Balkwill L">L.-L. Balkwill</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thompson, W F" uniqKey="Thompson W">W. F. Thompson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ballantine, C" uniqKey="Ballantine C">C. Ballantine</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Banaji, M R" uniqKey="Banaji M">M. R. Banaji</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Greenwald, A G" uniqKey="Greenwald A">A. G. Greenwald</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Banse, R" uniqKey="Banse R">R. Banse</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Scherer, K R" uniqKey="Scherer K">K. R. Scherer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Baron Cohen, S" uniqKey="Baron Cohen S">S. Baron-Cohen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Knickmeyer, R" uniqKey="Knickmeyer R">R. Knickmeyer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Belmote, M" uniqKey="Belmote M">M. Belmote</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Behrens, S" uniqKey="Behrens S">S. Behrens</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Besson, M" uniqKey="Besson M">M. Besson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Magne, C" uniqKey="Magne C">C. Magne</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schon, D" uniqKey="Schon D">D. Schön</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bhatara, A" uniqKey="Bhatara A">A. Bhatara</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tirovolas, A K" uniqKey="Tirovolas A">A. K. Tirovolas</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Duan, L M" uniqKey="Duan L">L. M. Duan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Levy, B" uniqKey="Levy B">B. Levy</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Levitin, D" uniqKey="Levitin D">D. Levitin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Biddleconbe, G" uniqKey="Biddleconbe G">G. Biddleconbe</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bolinger, D L" uniqKey="Bolinger D">D. L. Bolinger</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brattico, E" uniqKey="Brattico E">E. Brattico</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="N T Nen, R" uniqKey="N T Nen R">R. Näätänen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tervaniemi, M" uniqKey="Tervaniemi M">M. Tervaniemi</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brett, P" uniqKey="Brett P">P. Brett</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wood, E" uniqKey="Wood E">E. Wood</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thomas, G" uniqKey="Thomas G">G. Thomas</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brown, C P" uniqKey="Brown C">C. P. Brown</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brown, D" uniqKey="Brown D">D. Brown</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brown, P" uniqKey="Brown P">P. Brown</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Budd, M" uniqKey="Budd M">M. Budd</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Campbell, G" uniqKey="Campbell G">G. Campbell</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Canli, T" uniqKey="Canli T">T. Canli</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Desmond, J E" uniqKey="Desmond J">J. E. Desmond</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zhao, Z" uniqKey="Zhao Z">Z. Zhao</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gabrieli, J D E" uniqKey="Gabrieli J">J. D. E. Gabrieli</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Christensen, O M" uniqKey="Christensen O">O. M. Christensen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Citron, M J" uniqKey="Citron M">M. J. Citron</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Coker, W" uniqKey="Coker W">W. Coker</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cusick, S G" uniqKey="Cusick S">S. G. Cusick</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cutietta, R A" uniqKey="Cutietta R">R. A. Cutietta</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcallister, P A" uniqKey="Mcallister P">P. A. McAllister</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="D Indy, V" uniqKey="D Indy V">V. D'Indy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Daly, N" uniqKey="Daly N">N. Daly</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Warren, P" uniqKey="Warren P">P. Warren</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Deaux, K" uniqKey="Deaux K">K. Deaux</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lewis, L" uniqKey="Lewis L">L. Lewis</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dellacherie, D" uniqKey="Dellacherie D">D. Dellacherie</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Roy, M" uniqKey="Roy M">M. Roy</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hugueville, L" uniqKey="Hugueville L">L. Hugueville</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Peretz, L" uniqKey="Peretz L">L Peretz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Samson, S" uniqKey="Samson S">S. Samson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Delsing, M J" uniqKey="Delsing M">M. J. Delsing</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ter Bogt, T F M" uniqKey="Ter Bogt T">T. F. M. Ter Bogt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Engels, R C M E" uniqKey="Engels R">R. C. M. E. Engels</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meeus, W H J" uniqKey="Meeus W">W. H. J. Meeus</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Denora, T" uniqKey="Denora T">T. DeNora</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Descartes, R" uniqKey="Descartes R">R. Descartes</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dollinger, S J" uniqKey="Dollinger S">S. J. Dollinger</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Duerr, W" uniqKey="Duerr W">W. Duerr</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dunn, P G" uniqKey="Dunn P">P. G. Dunn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Ruyter, D" uniqKey="De Ruyter D">D. de Ruyter</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bouwhuis, D G" uniqKey="Bouwhuis D">D. G. Bouwhuis</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Eisenberg, N" uniqKey="Eisenberg N">N. Eisenberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lennon, R" uniqKey="Lennon R">R. Lennon</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Evers, S" uniqKey="Evers S">S. Evers</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dannert, J" uniqKey="Dannert J">J. Dannert</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rodding, D" uniqKey="Rodding D">D. Rödding</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rotter, G" uniqKey="Rotter G">G. Rötter</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Feingold, A" uniqKey="Feingold A">A. Feingold</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Feld, S" uniqKey="Feld S">S. Feld</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fernald, A" uniqKey="Fernald A">A. Fernald</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Frick, R W" uniqKey="Frick R">R. W. Frick</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Frith, S" uniqKey="Frith S">S. Frith</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fuller, C D" uniqKey="Fuller C">C. D. Fuller</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Galvin, J J" uniqKey="Galvin J">J. J. Galvin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Free, R H" uniqKey="Free R">R. H. Free</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ba Kent, D" uniqKey="Ba Kent D">D. Başkent</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gaab, N" uniqKey="Gaab N">N. Gaab</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Keenan, J P" uniqKey="Keenan J">J. P. Keenan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schlaug, G" uniqKey="Schlaug G">G. Schlaug</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gabrielsson, A" uniqKey="Gabrielsson A">A. Gabrielsson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gabrielsson, A" uniqKey="Gabrielsson A">A. Gabrielsson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gabrielsson, A" uniqKey="Gabrielsson A">A. Gabrielsson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lindstrom, E" uniqKey="Lindstrom E">E. Lindström</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gagnon, L" uniqKey="Gagnon L">L. Gagnon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Peretz, I" uniqKey="Peretz I">I. Peretz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Geringer, J M" uniqKey="Geringer J">J. M. Geringer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Madsen, C K" uniqKey="Madsen C">C. K. Madsen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Geringer, J M" uniqKey="Geringer J">J. M. Geringer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Madsen, C K" uniqKey="Madsen C">C. K. Madsen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Goebl, W" uniqKey="Goebl W">W. Goebl</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dixon, S" uniqKey="Dixon S">S. Dixon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Poli, G" uniqKey="De Poli G">G. De Poli</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Friberg, A" uniqKey="Friberg A">A. Friberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bresu, R" uniqKey="Bresu R">R. Bresu</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Widmer, G" uniqKey="Widmer G">G. Widmer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grahn, J A" uniqKey="Grahn J">J. A. Grahn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rowe, J B" uniqKey="Rowe J">J. B. Rowe</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Green, L" uniqKey="Green L">L. Green</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hanna, J L" uniqKey="Hanna J">J. L. Hanna</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hargreaves, D J" uniqKey="Hargreaves D">D. J. Hargreaves</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Macdonald, R" uniqKey="Macdonald R">R. MacDonald</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Miell, D" uniqKey="Miell D">D. Miell</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Heiman, M E" uniqKey="Heiman M">M. E. Heiman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hevner, K" uniqKey="Hevner K">K. Hevner</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hevner, K" uniqKey="Hevner K">K. Hevner</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hoffman, M L" uniqKey="Hoffman M">M. L. Hoffman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Holbrook, M B" uniqKey="Holbrook M">M. B. Holbrook</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Arnand, P" uniqKey="Arnand P">P. Arnand</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Holsinger, B W" uniqKey="Holsinger B">B. W. Holsinger</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ilie, G" uniqKey="Ilie G">G. Ilie</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thompson, W F" uniqKey="Thompson W">W. F. Thompson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Imaizumi, S" uniqKey="Imaizumi S">S. Imaizumi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Homma, M" uniqKey="Homma M">M. Homma</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ozawa, Y" uniqKey="Ozawa Y">Y. Ozawa</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Maruishi, M" uniqKey="Maruishi M">M. Maruishi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Muranaka, H" uniqKey="Muranaka H">H. Muranaka</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnson, C M" uniqKey="Johnson C">C. M. Johnson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Laukka, P" uniqKey="Laukka P">P. Laukka</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Laukka, P" uniqKey="Laukka P">P. Laukka</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Katz, S L" uniqKey="Katz S">S. L. Katz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kelly, J" uniqKey="Kelly J">J. Kelly</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kemp, A E" uniqKey="Kemp A">A. E. Kemp</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Keyser, D" uniqKey="Keyser D">D. Keyser</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Khatoon M Nighute, S" uniqKey="Khatoon M Nighute S">S. Khatoon, M, Nighute</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ishaque, M" uniqKey="Ishaque M">M. Ishaque</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kimura, D" uniqKey="Kimura D">D. Kimura</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kitayama, S" uniqKey="Kitayama S">S. Kitayama</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ishii, K" uniqKey="Ishii K">K. Ishii</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Koelsch, S" uniqKey="Koelsch S">S. Koelsch</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grossman, T" uniqKey="Grossman T">T. Grossman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gunter, T C" uniqKey="Gunter T">T. C. Gunter</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hahne, A" uniqKey="Hahne A">A. Hahne</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schroger, E" uniqKey="Schroger E">E. Schröger</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Friederici, A D" uniqKey="Friederici A">A. D. Friederici</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Koelsch, S" uniqKey="Koelsch S">S. Koelsch</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gunter, T C" uniqKey="Gunter T">T. C. Gunter</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wittfroth, M" uniqKey="Wittfroth M">M. Wittfroth</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Koelsch, S" uniqKey="Koelsch S">S. Koelsch</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Maess, B" uniqKey="Maess B">B. Maess</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grossman, T" uniqKey="Grossman T">T. Grossman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Friederici, A D" uniqKey="Friederici A">A. D. Friederici</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Krumhansl, C L" uniqKey="Krumhansl C">C. L. Krumhansl</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Krumhansl, C L" uniqKey="Krumhansl C">C. L. Krumhansl</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kuhl, O" uniqKey="Kuhl O">O. Kuhl</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Langmeyer, A" uniqKey="Langmeyer A">A. Langmeyer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Guglhor Rudan, A" uniqKey="Guglhor Rudan A">A. Guglhör-Rudan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tarna, C" uniqKey="Tarna C">C. Tarna</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lattner, S" uniqKey="Lattner S">S. Lattner</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meyer, M E" uniqKey="Meyer M">M. E. Meyer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Friederici, A D" uniqKey="Friederici A">A. D. Friederici</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lehmann, A" uniqKey="Lehmann A">A. Lehmann</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Liljestrom, S" uniqKey="Liljestrom S">S. Liljeström</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Juslin, P N" uniqKey="Juslin P">P. N. Juslin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="V Stfj Ll, D" uniqKey="V Stfj Ll D">D. Västfjäll</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lin, Y P" uniqKey="Lin Y">Y-P. Lin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wang, C H" uniqKey="Wang C">C-H. Wang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jung, T P" uniqKey="Jung T">T-P. Jung</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wu, T L" uniqKey="Wu T">T-L. Wu</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jeng, S K" uniqKey="Jeng S">S-K. Jeng</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Duann, J R" uniqKey="Duann J">J. R. Duann</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lowery, H" uniqKey="Lowery H">H. Lowery</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lu, Y" uniqKey="Lu Y">Y. Lu</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wu, H" uniqKey="Wu H">H. Wu</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Qian, J" uniqKey="Qian J">J. Qian</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Xie, F" uniqKey="Xie F">F. Xie</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Luck, G" uniqKey="Luck G">G. Luck</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Saarikallio, S" uniqKey="Saarikallio S">S. Saarikallio</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Burger, B" uniqKey="Burger B">B. Burger</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thompson, M R" uniqKey="Thompson M">M. R. Thompson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tolvainen, P" uniqKey="Tolvainen P">P. Tolvainen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Marques, C" uniqKey="Marques C">C. Marques</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Moreno, S" uniqKey="Moreno S">S. Moreno</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Castro, S L" uniqKey="Castro S">S. L. Castro</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Besson, M" uniqKey="Besson M">M. Besson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Marx, A B" uniqKey="Marx A">A. B. Marx</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Maus, F E" uniqKey="Maus F">F. E. Maus</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcclary, S" uniqKey="Mcclary S">S. McClary</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meyer, L B" uniqKey="Meyer L">L. B. Meyer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mitchell, R L C" uniqKey="Mitchell R">R. L. C. Mitchell</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Elliott, R" uniqKey="Elliott R">R. Elliott</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barry, M" uniqKey="Barry M">M. Barry</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cruttenden, A" uniqKey="Cruttenden A">A. Cruttenden</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Woodruff, P W R" uniqKey="Woodruff P">P. W. R. Woodruff</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Monelle, R" uniqKey="Monelle R">R. Monelle</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Monelle, R" uniqKey="Monelle R">R. Monelle</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nater, U M" uniqKey="Nater U">U. M. Nater</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Abbruzzese, E" uniqKey="Abbruzzese E">E. Abbruzzese</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Krebs, M" uniqKey="Krebs M">M. Krebs</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ehlert, U" uniqKey="Ehlert U">U. Ehlert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nettl, B" uniqKey="Nettl B">B. Nettl</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nygaard, L C" uniqKey="Nygaard L">L. C. Nygaard</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Queen, J S" uniqKey="Queen J">J. S. Queen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Obleser, J" uniqKey="Obleser J">J. Obleser</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rockstroh, B" uniqKey="Rockstroh B">B. Rockstroh</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Eulitz, C" uniqKey="Eulitz C">C. Eulitz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Olsson, B" uniqKey="Olsson B">B. Olsson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Onishi, T" uniqKey="Onishi T">T. Onishi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Matsuda, H" uniqKey="Matsuda H">H. Matsuda</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Asada, T" uniqKey="Asada T">T. Asada</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aruga, M" uniqKey="Aruga M">M. Aruga</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hirakata, M" uniqKey="Hirakata M">M. Hirakata</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nishakawa, M" uniqKey="Nishakawa M">M. Nishakawa</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Panksepp, J" uniqKey="Panksepp J">J. Panksepp</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pearsall, E" uniqKey="Pearsall E">E. Pearsall</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Almen, B" uniqKey="Almen B">B. Almen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Peraino, J A" uniqKey="Peraino J">J. A. Peraino</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rawlings, D" uniqKey="Rawlings D">D. Rawlings</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ciancarelli, V" uniqKey="Ciancarelli V">V. Ciancarelli</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Reich, N B" uniqKey="Reich N">N. B. Reich</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rentfrow, P J" uniqKey="Rentfrow P">P. J. Rentfrow</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gosling, S D" uniqKey="Gosling S">S. D. Gosling</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Riemann, H" uniqKey="Riemann H">H. Riemann</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rigg, M" uniqKey="Rigg M">M. Rigg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Russell, B" uniqKey="Russell B">B. Russell</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ruytjens, L" uniqKey="Ruytjens L">L. Ruytjens</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Georgiadis, J R" uniqKey="Georgiadis J">J. R. Georgiadis</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Holstege, G" uniqKey="Holstege G">G. Holstege</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wit, H P" uniqKey="Wit H">H. P. Wit</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Albers, W J" uniqKey="Albers W">W. J. Albers</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Willemsen, A T M" uniqKey="Willemsen A">A. T. M. Willemsen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rycenga, J" uniqKey="Rycenga J">J. Rycenga</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schirmer, A" uniqKey="Schirmer A">A. Schirmer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kotz, S A" uniqKey="Kotz S">S. A. Kotz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Friederici, A D" uniqKey="Friederici A">A. D. Friederici</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Seeger, C" uniqKey="Seeger C">C. Seeger</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sergeant, D C" uniqKey="Sergeant D">D. C. Sergeant</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vraka, M" uniqKey="Vraka M">M. Vraka</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sergeant, D C" uniqKey="Sergeant D">D. C. Sergeant</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Welch, G F" uniqKey="Welch G">G. F. Welch</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sheldon, D A" uniqKey="Sheldon D">D. A. Sheldon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gregory, D" uniqKey="Gregory D">D. Gregory</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shepherd, J" uniqKey="Shepherd J">J. Shepherd</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sloboda, J A" uniqKey="Sloboda J">J. A. Sloboda</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Standke, R" uniqKey="Standke R">R. Standke</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Strunk, O" uniqKey="Strunk O">O. Strunk</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sundberg, J" uniqKey="Sundberg J">J. Sundberg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Taruskin, R" uniqKey="Taruskin R">R. Taruskin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Taylor, J" uniqKey="Taylor J">J. Taylor</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tekman, H G" uniqKey="Tekman H">H. G. Tekman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tervaniemi, M" uniqKey="Tervaniemi M">M. Tervaniemi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Just, V" uniqKey="Just V">V. Just</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Koelsch, S" uniqKey="Koelsch S">S. Koelsch</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Widman, A" uniqKey="Widman A">A. Widman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schroger, E" uniqKey="Schroger E">E. Schroger</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct></biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thompson, W F" uniqKey="Thompson W">W. F. Thompson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Balkwill, L L" uniqKey="Balkwill L">L.-L. Balkwill</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thompson, W F" uniqKey="Thompson W">W. F. Thompson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Robitalle, B" uniqKey="Robitalle B">B. Robitalle</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thompson, W F" uniqKey="Thompson W">W. F. Thompson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schellenberg, E G" uniqKey="Schellenberg E">E. G. Schellenberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Glenn, E" uniqKey="Glenn E">E. Glenn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Husain, G" uniqKey="Husain G">G. Husain</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thompson, W F" uniqKey="Thompson W">W. F. Thompson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schellenberg, E G" uniqKey="Schellenberg E">E. G. Schellenberg</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Husain, G" uniqKey="Husain G">G. Husain</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Thorpe, M" uniqKey="Thorpe M">M. Thorpe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ockelford, A" uniqKey="Ockelford A">A. Ockelford</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aksentrijevic, A" uniqKey="Aksentrijevic A">A. Aksentrijevic</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Treitler, L" uniqKey="Treitler L">L. Treitler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Trevarthen, C" uniqKey="Trevarthen C">C. Trevarthen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Delafield, R J" uniqKey="Delafield R">R. J. Delafield</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Butt, J" uniqKey="Butt J">J. Butt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schogler, B" uniqKey="Schogler B">B. Schogler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Trimmer, M" uniqKey="Trimmer M">M. Trimmer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fischer, A H" uniqKey="Fischer A">A. H. Fischer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Manstead, A S R" uniqKey="Manstead A">A. S. R. Manstead</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Webster, G D" uniqKey="Webster G">G. D. Webster</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weir, C G" uniqKey="Weir C">C. G. Weir</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wheeler, B L" uniqKey="Wheeler B">B. L. Wheeler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wurm, L H" uniqKey="Wurm L">L. H. Wurm</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vakoch, D A" uniqKey="Vakoch D">D. A. Vakoch</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Strasser, M R" uniqKey="Strasser M">M. R. Strasser</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Calm Jagerlman, R" uniqKey="Calm Jagerlman R">R. Calm-Jagerlman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shannon, E R" uniqKey="Shannon E">E. R. Shannon</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yamasaki, T" uniqKey="Yamasaki T">T. Yamasaki</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zweigenghaft, R L" uniqKey="Zweigenghaft R">R. L. Zweigenghaft</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
</listBibl>
</div1>
</back>
</TEI>
<pmc article-type="research-article">
<pmc-dir>properties open_access</pmc-dir>
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="nlm-ta">Front Psychol</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="iso-abbrev">Front Psychol</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="publisher-id">Front. Psychol.</journal-id>
<journal-title-group>
<journal-title>Frontiers in Psychology</journal-title>
</journal-title-group>
<issn pub-type="epub">1664-1078</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Frontiers Media S.A.</publisher-name>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmid">24795663</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmc">3997045</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00276</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Original Research Article</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>Gender and the performance of music</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sergeant</surname>
<given-names>Desmond C.</given-names>
</name>
<uri xlink:type="simple" xlink:href="http://community.frontiersin.org/people/u/126396"></uri>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Himonides</surname>
<given-names>Evangelos</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="author-notes" rid="fn001">
<sup>*</sup>
</xref>
<uri xlink:type="simple" xlink:href="http://community.frontiersin.org/people/u/102582"></uri>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<aff>
<institution>Department of Culture, Communication and Media, International Music Education Research Centre, Institute of Education, University of London</institution>
<country>London, UK</country>
</aff>
<author-notes>
<fn fn-type="edited-by">
<p>Edited by: Aaron Williamon, Royal College of Music, UK</p>
</fn>
<fn fn-type="edited-by">
<p>Reviewed by: Adam Ockelford, University of Roehampton, UK; Stephanie Pitts, University of Sheffield, UK</p>
</fn>
<corresp id="fn001">*Correspondence: Evangelos Himonides, Department of Culture, Communication and Media, International Music Education Research Centre (iMerc), Institute of Education, University of London, 20 Bedford Way, London, WC1H 0AL, UK e-mail:
<email xlink:type="simple">e.himonides@ioe.ac.uk</email>
</corresp>
<fn fn-type="other" id="fn002">
<p>This article was submitted to Cognitive Science, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology.</p>
</fn>
</author-notes>
<pub-date pub-type="epub">
<day>16</day>
<month>4</month>
<year>2014</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="collection">
<year>2014</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>5</volume>
<elocation-id>276</elocation-id>
<history>
<date date-type="received">
<day>04</day>
<month>12</month>
<year>2013</year>
</date>
<date date-type="accepted">
<day>14</day>
<month>3</month>
<year>2014</year>
</date>
</history>
<permissions>
<copyright-statement>Copyright © 2014 Sergeant and Himonides.</copyright-statement>
<copyright-year>2014</copyright-year>
<license license-type="open-access" xlink:href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/">
<license-p>This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.</license-p>
</license>
</permissions>
<abstract>
<p>This study evaluates propositions that have appeared in the literature that music phenomena are gendered. Were they present in the musical “message,” gendered qualities might be imparted at any of three stages of the music–communication interchange: the process of composition, its realization into sound by the performer, or imposed by the listener in the process of perception. The research was designed to obtain empirical evidence to enable evaluation of claims of the presence of gendering at these three stages. Three research hypotheses were identified and relevant literature of music behaviors and perception reviewed. New instruments of measurement were constructed to test the three hypotheses: (i) two listening sequences each containing 35 extracts from published recordings of compositions of the classical music repertoire, (ii) four “music characteristics” scales, with polarities defined by verbal descriptors designed to assess the dynamic and emotional valence of the musical extracts featured in the listening sequences. 69 musically-trained listeners listened to the two sequences and were asked to identify the sex of the performing artist of each musical extract; a second group of 23 listeners evaluated the extracts applying the four music characteristics scales. Results did not support claims that music structures are inherently gendered, nor proposals that performers impart their own-sex-specific qualities to the music. It is concluded that gendered properties are imposed subjectively by the listener, and these are primarily related to the tempo of the music.</p>
</abstract>
<kwd-group>
<kwd>music</kwd>
<kwd>performance</kwd>
<kwd>musical meaning</kwd>
<kwd>gender</kwd>
<kwd>sex differences</kwd>
</kwd-group>
<counts>
<fig-count count="5"></fig-count>
<table-count count="2"></table-count>
<equation-count count="0"></equation-count>
<ref-count count="143"></ref-count>
<page-count count="13"></page-count>
<word-count count="11706"></word-count>
</counts>
</article-meta>
</front>
<body>
<sec>
<title>Three levels of music communication</title>
<p>Bertrand Russell said of language that “it expresses the state of the speaker and alters the state of the hearer” (1941, p. 204). He might well have made a similar observation about music, whose nature is “a form of communication in which all humans participate, analogous to language or speech” (Nettl,
<xref rid="B104" ref-type="bibr">2014</xref>
) and whose purpose is “to please, and the pleasure consists in different emotions being activated in the listener” (Descartes,
<xref rid="B33" ref-type="bibr">1618</xref>
).</p>
<p>Despite the comparability of their intentions, there are essential differences between speech and music in the nature of their significations. In speech, meanings attaching to words are relatively specific, at least to the extent of being capable of listing in a dictionary (Coker,
<xref rid="B24" ref-type="bibr">1972</xref>
, p. 7) whereas those of music are less precise, though they cannot be regarded as being arbitrary (Monelle,
<xref rid="B102" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
, p. 11). Seeger (
<xref rid="B126" ref-type="bibr">1977</xref>
) suggests that speech is communication of “the intellectualization of reality” whereas music is communication of “the feeling of reality.”</p>
<p>A second difference that distinguishes speech from music is that in the case of speech, the expression and consequent alteration of states take place through communication within a dyad—the speaker and the hearer—but in the Western “classical” art music praxis, the interchange of information passes through three levels. This triadic transfer can be modeled thus Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F1">1</xref>
.</p>
<fig id="F1" position="float">
<label>Figure 1</label>
<caption>
<p>
<bold>The triadic transfer of music information</bold>
.</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="fpsyg-05-00276-g0001"></graphic>
</fig>
<p>The first generative level comprises the infrastructure of pitch, temporal and timbral events determined by the composer, intentional gestures organized in groups and sequences to form a musical narrative reflective of an inner mental and emotional state and, at least in the case of Western music, set out in the score as the givens of a composition (Kuhl,
<xref rid="B86" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
; Katz,
<xref rid="B73" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
, p. 171). The successful expression of meaning at this stage is reliant on the goodness of fit between the musical content of the gestural signs and the musical and emotional experiences of the composer at the moment of their conception and the accuracy of their notation in the score, i.e., their validity as expressions of thought (Thompson and Robitalle,
<xref rid="B143" ref-type="bibr">1992</xref>
; Trevarthen et al.,
<xref rid="B148" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
).</p>
<p>Superimposed onto this structural data is a second level: that of the performer who actualizes the score into sound and “interprets” the compositional data, giving it expression
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="fn0001">
<sup>1</sup>
</xref>
<sup>,</sup>
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="fn0002">
<sup>2</sup>
</xref>
. The motivation in this is probably not so much a deliberate intention to make the music “expressive,” but more a desire to enable the listener to share the way the performer intrinsically feels “the music should go,” whilst being “truthful” to what are believed were the composer's intentions
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="fn0003">
<sup>3</sup>
</xref>
.</p>
<p>The performer contributes to a “change of state” in the listener by means of small adjustments to parameters of tempo, phrasing, duration, articulation (staccato and legato), loudness, spectral envelope (timbre), dynamics and pitch (where the instrument of performance allows the player control of this), i.e., the
<italic>prosody</italic>
of the performance. Juslin (
<xref rid="B67" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
,
<xref rid="B68" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
,
<xref rid="B70" ref-type="bibr">2009</xref>
) provides detailed accounts and argues that no single dimension can encapsulate the expressivity of a musical performance: rather, the variables are highly interactive and operate variously in complex combinations. The degrees of freedom open to a performer to manipulate these parameters are not limitless: they are subject to stylistically accepted norms and median values, and to optimum values relative to each particular musical work (Holbrook and Arnand,
<xref rid="B62" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
). Adjustments that are very small may be imperceptible to a listener: too great, and they are likely to be perceived as unacceptable exaggeration or waywardness, and departure from the “truth” of the composition.</p>
<p>The final level in the strata of communication is embodied in the listener, in reception of the sonic information and its neural transcription in processes of cognition and transformation into meaning. These essential processes are dependent upon the musical experience, awareness, and responsiveness of the listener and familiarity with the musical idiom of the composer. For the communication to be functional, the “meaning” imparted to a musical gesture by the performer must be recognized by the listener. Composer, performer, and listener must therefore understand a mutually shared code. Citron (
<xref rid="B22" ref-type="bibr">1993</xref>
) cautions that during a performance “each listener will be creating another ontology of the work. This newly constructed version probably does not coincide in all respects with the performer's understood version or with the composer's.” Sundberg (
<xref rid="B135" ref-type="bibr">1993</xref>
) similarly comments that “expressive details in music performance become meaningful because of the listener's extramusical experiences.” Although there is no certainty that the meaning intended by the composer is the same as that realized by the performer or understood by the listener at the final stage, there is good evidence that there will be at least positive correspondence (Krumhansl,
<xref rid="B84" ref-type="bibr">1997</xref>
,
<xref rid="B85" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
; Balkwill and Thompson,
<xref rid="B4" ref-type="bibr">1999</xref>
; Juslin and Laukka,
<xref rid="B71" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
,
<xref rid="B72" ref-type="bibr">2004</xref>
; Juslin,
<xref rid="B69" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
; Bhatara et al.,
<xref rid="B11" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
). Meaning in music is therefore not an absolute quality, and Pearsall and Almen (
<xref rid="B112" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
, p. 1) describe the term as having a degree of “semantic slipperiness.”</p>
<p>With three levels of information transfer in the musical communication process, it follows that three levels of musical meaning are operative. The first is, the “meaning” of the compositional events and structures, variously termed “embodied” or “intramusical” meaning (Meyer,
<xref rid="B99" ref-type="bibr">1961</xref>
) and “inherent” meaning (Green,
<xref rid="B55" ref-type="bibr">1997</xref>
), contained within the music itself, a view predicated on the concept of predictive meaning whereby a gesture may lead to expectancy of future gestures having similar or related characteristics (Coker,
<xref rid="B24" ref-type="bibr">1972</xref>
, p. 5). The second level is the expressive meaning introduced at the performance level, and lastly that invoked in the listener. The apprehension and interpretation of meaning by the listener is subject to constraints of listener mind-set, social perceptions, availability of appropriate cognitive abilities, and experience of the musico-linguistic idiom employed (Dellacherie et al.,
<xref rid="B30" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
), a condition that accords with Lowery's definition of music as “… a subjective phenomenon that only begins when the sounds heard are understood” (1952, p. 18).</p>
<p>The problem of meaning in music has been the subject of extended discussion in the literature, and lies within the province of
<italic>semiotics</italic>
, the theory of signs and their significations. The process of semiosis is one by which a sign becomes a signifier for something else: its signification. “The communicator constructs an internal representation of some aspect of the world and then carries out some symbolic behavior intended to convey the content of that representation. The recipient must perceive the symbolic behavior and recover from it the content it signifies” (Juslin,
<xref rid="B69" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
, p. 86). The adjustive response of the listener to a gesture therefore represents the meaning of that gesture, but the affective value of its signification is dependent on the contribution the perceiver brings to the event, and is therefore not inherent in it. Thus a knock at the door might constitute an objective sign, its signification being that a caller is outside, and the adaptive behavior that the door is opened. The affective value of the signification, however, would depend on the anticipation of the door's owner as to the probable identity of the caller: the postman, a lover, a debt-collector, the police!</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Gender and musical meaning</title>
<p>Given the human intentions and interactions intrinsic to this triad of stages of communication, it is not surprising that music has been argued to be a social phenomenon and that “musical meanings are socially and culturally constructed” (Olsson,
<xref rid="B107" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
, p. 989), having “fundamentally a social life” (Feld,
<xref rid="B40" ref-type="bibr">1984</xref>
), “growing out of specific social context, and expressing the assumptions of that context” (Citron,
<xref rid="B22" ref-type="bibr">1993</xref>
, p. 120). Music is part of the constructs of our sense of identity within society (Frith,
<xref rid="B43" ref-type="bibr">1996</xref>
, p. 124).</p>
<p>It has consequently been postulated that as social meanings are almost invariably gendered, so also are musical meanings. McClary (
<xref rid="B98" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
) for example, describes music as essentially “a gendered discourse” and the history of musical form as “a heavily gendered legacy” (p. 17), and considers that “classical music—no less than pop—is bound up with issues of gender” (p. 54). Treitler (
<xref rid="B147" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
) states that “… gender, race and ethnicity has the meaning,” and Taylor (
<xref rid="B138" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
) that “music is a dynamic mode of gender” and “a stylistic marker of sexual identity.” Hargreaves et al. (
<xref rid="B57" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
) list gender as a principal variable at each of the three stages of transfer of information: composer–performer–listener (pp. 8–15); but none of these writers provide an account of the modifications in musical infrastructure or output or perception by which gendering would be evidenced, nor whether these would be the same at each stage of information transfer.</p>
<p>Gender has been held to be inherent in basic music structures, and “reflected in perceptual and music phenomena” (Brett et al.,
<xref rid="B15" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
). Maus (
<xref rid="B97" ref-type="bibr">1993</xref>
) considers gender to be related to music theory, and by McClary as being present in resolution of chromaticism to the triad, thereby “taking on the cultural cast of femininity” (p. 124). Shepherd (
<xref rid="B130" ref-type="bibr">1991</xref>
) also interprets the inherent hierarchy of tonality as “an image of a gendered hierarchy of political and social hegemony,” and Green (
<xref rid="B55" ref-type="bibr">1997</xref>
) refers to “… the gender-related characteristics of the music itself” (p. 139).</p>
<p>Sexual dissonance is seen as embodied in the contrasting characteristics of first and second themes in sonata form (McClary,
<xref rid="B98" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
, p. 124). This idea appears to have originated in A.B.Marx's music-theoretic text “
<italic>Die Lehre von musikalischen compositionen</italic>
” (1845), in the course of which he described second theme as being “of more tender nature, flexibly rather than emphatically constructed … as it were the
<italic>feminine</italic>
to the preceding
<italic>masculine</italic>
.” The analogy was repeated by Riemann (
<xref rid="B118" ref-type="bibr">1888</xref>
) and again by D'Indy (
<xref rid="B27" ref-type="bibr">1909</xref>
) and has subsequently become a common allusion, as has the application of the adjective
<italic>feminine</italic>
to a cadence that reaches closure on an unaccented beat
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="fn0004">
<sup>4</sup>
</xref>
. Monelle (
<xref rid="B101" ref-type="bibr">1995</xref>
) describes the opening of the Tristan prelude as proceeding by “feminine quavers and dotted rhythm,” and Clément sees the chromaticism of the same passage as reflecting “seductive, deadly feminine sexuality.” Shepherd writes of “the role of timbre in articulating and reproducing gender identities,” “male timbres” and “female timbres” (1991, p. 170)</p>
<p>The association of music and gendering extends beyond gender to sexuality and sexual identity since “musicality is next-door to sexuality” (Cusick,
<xref rid="B25" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
, p. 74) and “the history of Western music is a history of sexual anxiety, ambivalence and negotiation” (Peraino,
<xref rid="B114" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
). For McClary, music is tied with the “channeling of desire … and competing images of sexuality,” and tonality to be “strongly informed by erotic imagery” (p. 54). Holsinger (
<xref rid="B63" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
) even sees female sexual desire and pleasure as inseparable from musical and devotional experience in the convent of Hildegard of Bingen.</p>
<p>The imputed gendering of musical signs becomes more complex when the connection moves beyond the boundaries of male/female heterosexuality: Cusick (
<xref rid="B25" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
, p. 67 et seq.) discusses the effects of “a Lesbian relationship with music” and Rycenga (
<xref rid="B122" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
) discusses “lesbian compositional processes” (p. 277) stating that being a lesbian “transforms the thought/action process that is composition” and Cusick speaks of a “Lesbian reception of music's message” (2006, p. 70). Brown (
<xref rid="B17" ref-type="bibr">1986</xref>
, pp. 100–187) sees strong associations between Tchaikowsky's fourth symphony op.36 and the composer's reported homosexuality.</p>
<p>Gender is repeatedly argued to be present at every stage of the communication triad: Green (
<xref rid="B55" ref-type="bibr">1997</xref>
, p. 71) cites the composer Ethel Smyth that “… she expected women to distinguish themselves from men not only in their approach and behavior, but also in their playing …” and argues that the listener receives a gendered message as “a cultural artifact within societal and historical context” (p. 6). A listener's sex, she argues, will “influence their overall response to music, and perception of masculinity or femininity, so that “… men and women must have a slightly different type of musical experience resulting from their gender” (p. 139). Citron (p. 120) also lists gender and sexuality among crucial factors for receptor.</p>
<p>Juslin (
<xref rid="B69" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
, p. 93) however, suggests that though present, their effects may be small “listener judgments are only marginally affected by musical training, age and gender of the listener,” and this view is shared by others (Gabrielsson and Juslin,
<xref rid="B46" ref-type="bibr">1996</xref>
,
<xref rid="B47" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
; Yamasaki,
<xref rid="B152" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
). Biddleconbe (
<xref rid="B12" ref-type="bibr">1992</xref>
) has criticized McClary on grounds that she fails to explain how the codes of gendering might operate, and as Taruskin (
<xref rid="B137" ref-type="bibr">1997</xref>
) points out “if one is going to talk about a sign, one must also specify its referents.” Without hard evidence of the modus operandi by which composers and performers might imbue their musical output with gender implication, and that this is demonstrably received by listeners, linking signs with outcomes, these claims remain at the level of metaphor. Reference to
<italic>masculine</italic>
and
<italic>feminine</italic>
as descriptors of thematic characteristics or to
<italic>feminine endings</italic>
for cadence types may be helpful for illustrative purposes, but it requires a syllogistic leap to posit that they are therefore gendered in real musical experience. The diminished seventh chord has sometimes been described as the “Clapham Junction of tonality” because of its capability of resolution in multiple tonalities, but it has not been argued that at a point of its occurrence in a modulation a listener might therefore experience the feeling of the humdrum of a busy railway junction, nor in the field of language has the word
<italic>manhood</italic>
been argued to have feminine connotations because it is an example of a feminine ending.</p>
<p>Propositions of gendering therefore raise questions about the nature and processes of music communication, and of semiosis in music, and these can be rationalized into the form of three propositional hypotheses:
<list list-type="order">
<list-item>
<p>Compositional structures and the gestures they represent are inherently gendered, irrespective of era and style of composition;</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>Musical performances are conditioned by the sex of the performer in ways that are evident to appropriately skilled listeners;</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>Perception of musical meaning is qualitatively affected by the sex of the listener.</p>
</list-item>
</list>
</p>
<p>The extent to which these three propositions are supported by evidence of research is examined below. Areas of study in which relevant sex differences have been observed range across brain topography and neural functioning, language and communication, personality, emotionality, and expressiveness, musicality, and musical performance.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Sex differences in communication and processing</title>
<sec>
<title>Sex differences in neural processing of music and music-related stimuli</title>
<p>Men and women do not differ only in physical attributes and functions: recent evidence from neurological research indicates that perceptual and cognitive processes are also sex-dimorphic, both in strength of activity and topographical deployment of brain resources. Kimura (
<xref rid="B78" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
) attributes this to differential effects of hormones which influence development of brain organization from very early stages of life, to the extent that the environment can be considered to be acting on differently wired brains.</p>
<p>Sex-specific contrasts in neural functioning have been shown in functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) studies to be evident in differential lateralization of brain behavior, with a greater level of bilateral functioning in females compared to more asymmetrical laterality in males (Koelsch et al.,
<xref rid="B80" ref-type="bibr">2003a</xref>
,
<xref rid="B82" ref-type="bibr">b</xref>
,
<xref rid="B81" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
). Importantly, processing of auditory signals of both speech and music are affected by these differences (Brown,
<xref rid="B16" ref-type="bibr">1999</xref>
; Gaab et al.,
<xref rid="B45" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
; Lattner et al.,
<xref rid="B88" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
; Ruytjens et al.,
<xref rid="B121" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
; Sergeant and Vraka,
<xref rid="B127" ref-type="bibr">2014</xref>
). Recent studies have shown that auditory evoked potentials (AEPs) may differ in latency between sexes, with females showing significantly shorter inter-peak values measured at brainstem levels than males (Khatoon M Nighute and Ishaque,
<xref rid="B77" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
). Auditory tympanometry indicators also show sex differences.</p>
<p>Inter-sex contrasts in electrophysiological measures indicating significant differences in brain reactivity have been observed by Nater et al. (
<xref rid="B103" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
): females showed greater sensitivity to aversive musical stimuli such as “heavy metal” and their data accord. Recent evidence from a study by Thorpe et al. (
<xref rid="B146" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
) suggest that neural sex differences may be reflected in differences in the ways that males and females process and predict musical structure (p. 459).</p>
<p>Notwithstanding this accumulating evidence of sex-related disparities, as Kimura (
<xref rid="B78" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
) remarks, unless differences in neural processing can be demonstrated to affect behavior in detectable sex-dimorphic ways, they are not meaningful in terms of human outcomes.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Evidence of sex differences in comparable modes of communication: spoken language</title>
<p>The meaning of speech is not communicated solely by its semantic content: prosody (which includes the variables of pitch contour, average pitch level, intensity, tempo, duration, accentuation, average loudness level, and voice timbre) contributes importantly to communication of meaning by expressing emotional qualification of the semantic content, identifying important words and qualifying the category of an utterance as declarative or interrogative (Behrens,
<xref rid="B9" ref-type="bibr">1985</xref>
; Besson et al.,
<xref rid="B10" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
; Mitchell et al.,
<xref rid="B100" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
; Sergeant and Welch,
<xref rid="B128" ref-type="bibr">2009</xref>
). These complexes have been shown to be cross-cultural (Bolinger,
<xref rid="B13" ref-type="bibr">1978</xref>
; Thompson and Balkwill,
<xref rid="B142" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
) and represent a fundamental principle of human brain organization affecting both speech and music (Thompson et al.,
<xref rid="B144" ref-type="bibr">2004</xref>
,
<xref rid="B145" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
; Nygaard and Queen,
<xref rid="B105" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
).</p>
<p>Prosody can be considered to function independently of the semantic content (Frick,
<xref rid="B42" ref-type="bibr">1985</xref>
; Standke,
<xref rid="B133" ref-type="bibr">1992</xref>
; Banse and Scherer,
<xref rid="B7" ref-type="bibr">1996</xref>
; Kitayama and Ishii,
<xref rid="B79" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
; Ilie and Thompson,
<xref rid="B64" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
). Schirmer et al. (
<xref rid="B124" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
) found behavioral and electrophysiological responses to prosody to be earlier in females than in males, for whom longer intervals between prime and target were required for processing. Similar findings are reported by Obleser et al. (
<xref rid="B106" ref-type="bibr">2004</xref>
); Koelsch et al. (
<xref rid="B81" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
) and Wurm et al. (
<xref rid="B154" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
), and are confirmed by an fMRI study by Imaizumi et al. (
<xref rid="B65" ref-type="bibr">2004</xref>
). Cortical areas for both words and prosody have been found to be activated significantly more strongly in females than in males, bringing the conclusion that there are sex-differentials in comprehension of emotional signals.</p>
<p>It is self-evident that men and women differ in the pitch range of speech, but the two sexes also differ in habitual speech styles and intonation patterns. Women's speech moves over a wider pitch range, and has greater dynamic flexibility, with more rapid pitch excursion, whereas men's speech is characterized by less dynamism (Daly and Warren,
<xref rid="B28" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
). These differences have been interpreted as showing greater felt emotion and empathy in women (Fernald,
<xref rid="B41" ref-type="bibr">1989</xref>
).</p>
<p>These studies indicate the presence of differential levels and styles of activity and responsiveness between males and females when engaging in communicative behaviors.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Sex differences in personality characteristics, emotionality and expressiveness</title>
<p>It is a common-place postulation of gender stereotyping that the sexes differ in personality characteristics (Deaux and Lewis,
<xref rid="B29" ref-type="bibr">1984</xref>
; Feingold,
<xref rid="B39" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
; Banaji and Greenwald,
<xref rid="B6" ref-type="bibr">1995</xref>
; Heiman,
<xref rid="B58" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
). Women are commonly believed to be more emotional, more moody, more trusting, sympathetic, tender-hearted, community-spirited, more conservative in their use of language, and to show greater tendency to neuroticism (Hoffman,
<xref rid="B61" ref-type="bibr">1997</xref>
). Men are viewed as more rational, assertive, agentic, less trusting.</p>
<p>In a qualified way, research evidence offers some support for these anecdotal wisdoms, but by no means unreservedly. The common stereotyping of women as more empathetic than men, is supported in the work of Trimmer et al. (
<xref rid="B149" ref-type="bibr">1998</xref>
), though this has been attributed to a greater social acceptability for women to display their emotions more overtly (Eisenberg and Lennon,
<xref rid="B37" ref-type="bibr">1983</xref>
; Kelly,
<xref rid="B74" ref-type="bibr">1999</xref>
). Feingold (
<xref rid="B39" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
) reports from a meta-analysis of research data that women are characterized by higher levels of extraversion and anxiety, are more trusting and especially are more tender-minded and nurturing, and this is supported by evidence of Baron-Cohen et al. (
<xref rid="B8" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
) of structural differences in the brain. Women are also observed to be more expressive of their emotions. Men are reported as higher on scales of assertiveness and self-esteem and lower on those characteristics observed in women. These sex differences are found to be constant across ages, years of data collection, and education.</p>
<p>Music preferences and responsiveness have been found to be reliably predicted by personality characteristics (Wheeler,
<xref rid="B151" ref-type="bibr">1985</xref>
; Dollinger,
<xref rid="B34" ref-type="bibr">1993</xref>
; Rentfrow and Gosling,
<xref rid="B117" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
; Delsing et al.,
<xref rid="B31" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
; Zweigenghaft,
<xref rid="B153" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
; Tekman,
<xref rid="B139" ref-type="bibr">2009</xref>
; Luck et al.,
<xref rid="B94" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
; Langmeyer et al.,
<xref rid="B87" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
). Neuroticism has been shown to have a robust association with preference for classical music (Dunn et al.,
<xref rid="B36" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
) and “openness to experience” to be related to both a liking for jazz and a wider range of musical styles (Rawlings and Ciancarelli,
<xref rid="B115" ref-type="bibr">1997</xref>
; Dunn et al.,
<xref rid="B36" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
) and to more intense experiencing of music-inspired emotions (Liljeström et al.,
<xref rid="B90" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
). A relevant constraint here, however, is that these findings have been drawn from broad general populations, and are unlikely to be consistent with evidence from musically trained subjects; Kemp's (
<xref rid="B75" ref-type="bibr">1996</xref>
) extensive study indicates that musicians are untypical in terms of measured personality, and are not conformant to patterns reported from general populations; a view that is supported by the work of Cutietta and McAllister (
<xref rid="B26" ref-type="bibr">1997</xref>
).</p>
<p>Sex differences have been noted in neural functioning in responses to music (Delsing et al.,
<xref rid="B31" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
; Lu et al.,
<xref rid="B93" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
), evidenced by increased activation in brain areas involved in auditory processing. This is consonant with findings of an fMRI study by Canli et al. (
<xref rid="B20" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
) that significantly more brain areas with faster response times were operative in women, leading to the conclusion that emotions were evoked more powerfully in females than males (Lin et al.,
<xref rid="B91" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
). Females are also reported to experience “chills” in response to music more than males (Panksepp,
<xref rid="B109" ref-type="bibr">1995</xref>
).</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Gender and the performance of music</title>
<p>Although the studies reviewed above give some support to propositions that the sex of a performing artist might have some qualitative effect on the sonic qualities of that artist's performance. Evidence from analysis of actual musical performances is currently confined to a single study: Lehmann (
<xref rid="B89" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
) has reported analysis of 54 performances of the opening bars of “
<italic>Arlequin</italic>
” from Schumann's “
<italic>Carnaval</italic>
” op.9 extracted from commercially available recordings by renowned pianists, ten of whom were women. Weighting cases by sex of artist, female performers were found to perform the passage marginally, but statistically significantly, slower than the males (265 beats per min vs. 280 for males). However, as this study samples only the first eight bars of a single work its scale is too small to allow generalization to all musical literature or performance.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Research design</title>
<p>The study reported below was designed to acquire empirical evidence to test the validity of claims of gendered properties of music and music performance, and to test empirically the three hypotheses. Musically experienced listeners, recruited via an internet facility, heard a sequence of extracts from recorded performances of music from the “art/classical” repertoire. At each extract they were asked to judge the sex of the artist performing—the “performer-sex discrimination task.” A second group of musically cognizant listeners listened to the same sequence of extracts, and rated the emotional valence of each item using four rating scales—the “music characteristics scales” and these provided data of the perceived emotional valence of each musical extract.</p>
<p>A sensitive measure—the MASCFEM scale—was constructed from data generated in the performer-sex task by combining “
<italic>male/female</italic>
” performer-sex decisions with their associated confidence ratings. This provided a measure of perceived masculinity/femininity of the musical extracts, and facilitated examination of possible interactions between sex of performer and sex of listener. A decision that a performer was “
<italic>male</italic>
,” made with maximum confidence rating of 7 was taken as the optimal masculine polarity of the scale (low MASCFEM score = 1); similarly, a response of “
<italic>female</italic>
” made with a confidence rating of 7 was taken as the optimal feminine point (high MASCFEM score = 14). The mid-point of the scale (score 7–8) thus represented gender-neutrality (Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F2">2</xref>
).</p>
<fig id="F2" position="float">
<label>Figure 2</label>
<caption>
<p>
<bold>Structure of the MASCFEM scale: perceived masculinity/ femininity of extracts and performances</bold>
.</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="fpsyg-05-00276-g0002"></graphic>
</fig>
<p>Should sex-specific information be present at micro-levels of musical articulation of performances of male and female artists, they would be most likely to be detected by listeners who have themselves experienced sustained musical training and have acquired performance skills. Musically experienced listeners have, unsurprisingly, been shown to be superior to non-musicians in detection of small changes in pitch changes and pitch discrimination (Brattico et al.,
<xref rid="B14" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
; Tervaniemi et al.,
<xref rid="B140" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
; Fuller et al.,
<xref rid="B44" ref-type="bibr">2014</xref>
) pitch violations (Marques et al.,
<xref rid="B95" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
), beat perception (Grahn and Rowe,
<xref rid="B54" ref-type="bibr">2009</xref>
), sensitivity to rubato (Johnson,
<xref rid="B66" ref-type="bibr">1996</xref>
), tempo of orchestral excerpts (Geringer and Madsen,
<xref rid="B51" ref-type="bibr">1984</xref>
,
<xref rid="B52" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
), etc. These differences have been observed to reflect differences in patterns of brain behavior (Brattico et al.,
<xref rid="B14" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
; Onishi et al.,
<xref rid="B108" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
), and in times required for neural activity to reach peak responsiveness (Evers et al.,
<xref rid="B38" ref-type="bibr">1999</xref>
). In the event that musically skilled listeners were shown not to be able to differentiate performances of male and female artists at a statistically reliable level, it would be difficult to argue that sex-specific qualities were present in the musical information at a meaningful level.</p>
</sec>
<sec sec-type="materials|methods" id="s1">
<title>Materials and methods</title>
<sec>
<title>Performer-sex discrimination task</title>
<p>Two data-collection instruments (listening sequences) were constructed, each comprising 35 extracts taken from published recordings of music of the Western art/classical music repertoire. Extracts, each of approximately 1.5 min duration, were taken from either the opening bars of a composition or starting at an appropriate point later in its progress. In the case of ensemble works such as chamber or concerto movements (e.g., Mozart Clarinet Quintet), a passage where the solo instrument was prominent was selected.</p>
<p>The two sequences comprised extracts from the same musical works, starting and fading at identical points, the difference between them being that when the performer of an extract in one sequence was male, the performer of the matching extract in the other sequence was female, and vice-versa. With 35 extracts in each of the two listening sequences, male and female performances were sampled equally across 70 presentations.</p>
<p>In order to test the first hypothesis (gendered information is present in the structures irrespective of era and style of composition), works sampled (listed at Supplementary material) were selected from the standard repertoire so as to include a range of eras and genres of musical composition, ranging from smaller-scale pieces, and chamber works to solo concerti, such as might regularly be encountered by a music-listening audience. Instruments sampled in the performances were piano, harpsichord, violin, viola, cello, harp, trumpet, recorder, flute, oboe, clarinet, and bassoon. It would have been desirable that a greater range of brass and percussion instruments be included, but this was prevented by the limited availability of recordings of female performers of these instruments
<xref ref-type="fn" rid="fn0005">
<sup>5</sup>
</xref>
and therefore difficulty in finding matching female/male recordings of the same works. For obvious reasons, no vocal music was included. Works in major and minor modes and atonal examples were included.</p>
<p>Bearing in mind that era of composition may affect musical structure, meaning and interpretation (Christensen,
<xref rid="B21" ref-type="bibr">1995</xref>
) and in order to test the second part of the first hypothesis, compositions sampled in the sequences ranged from early 18th century (earliest date of composition 1720, Handel suite no 5 HWV430) to mid-20th century (latest date 1911, Schoenberg, op.19). Hearing and responding to an entire Listening Sequence required approximately 56 min.</p>
<p>The great majority of composers have been male, and it would not have been possible to find sufficient recorded works by women composers to enable matching pairs of works in the sequences. None of the writers proposing gendering (cited above) however has suggested that gendering influences might be restricted to either sex, and almost all of the works to which gendering has been attributed by these writers have been works of male composers.</p>
<p>All digital audio files were scrutinized using a professional digital audio editing suite to ensure that no systematic differences of tempi (mean beats per min), pitch range, overall sound recording levels (means and peaks of loudness levels), or other acoustic factors were present between matched recordings of male and female performances. Any suspect files were replaced with other performances. Factors such as pitch range, timbre, would, of course, be common to both male and female performances of an extract.</p>
<sec>
<title>Procedure</title>
<p>Listeners were advised in a preliminary “Guidance to listeners” paragraph that during the playing of an extract the name of the instrument to be judged would be highlighted on the response page, and that this was the instrument of which the sex of the performer was to be judged.</p>
<p>At each extract in the listening sequences, listeners were asked to judge whether the artist performing that extract was female or male. They were then asked to qualify their “
<italic>male/female</italic>
” response by rating their confidence in their decision using a 7-point scale where 1 = “
<italic>I'm not at all confident</italic>
” and 7 = “
<italic>I am very confident</italic>
.” After listening and responding, listeners were able to trigger the following extract when ready. It was possible to suspend the session for resumption at a later time, at which point the system restored them to the extract reached at the point of suspension.</p>
<p>The two sequences were made available to listeners via an online facility on the web server of the International Music Education Research Centre (iMerc) at the Institute of Education, University of London. When a potential listener logged on to the dedicated website, they were presented with an introductory page of information explaining the background, motives, and procedures of the research. When they had read to the end of the introduction, they were asked whether they wished to proceed to the Listening Sequence. On affirmative response they were randomly assigned by the automated system to either Sequence A or Sequence B. After completion of the sequence, the listener was asked to provide personal data of sex, age-group, instruments played and musical work-role. All responses were stored in the system for analysis.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Participants</title>
<p>A total of 138 listeners responded to the performer–sex identification task. 69 response sets were discarded, either because the listener had failed to listen to all 35 extracts and their response data were therefore incomplete, or in three cases, they had failed to provide details enabling them to be classified by musical experience, sex or age-group. The valid data sets therefore comprised 69 cases, 34 for Sequence A listeners, 35 for Sequence B.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Music characteristics scaling</title>
<sec>
<title>Materials and procedure</title>
<p>The two listening sequences used in the performer–sex discrimination task were evaluated further by a second group of listeners who had not participated as listeners in the performer-sex measures.</p>
<p>These four additional measures were created in order to provide measures of perceived emotional valence of the musical extracts used in the listening sequences. Listeners for these measures were asked to rate each extract using 9-point semantic differential scales whose verbal polarities were designed to characterize the temporal density, emotional valence and mood of each extract (Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F3">3</xref>
).</p>
<fig id="F3" position="float">
<label>Figure 3</label>
<caption>
<p>
<bold>Four “Music Characteristics” scales</bold>
.</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="fpsyg-05-00276-g0003"></graphic>
</fig>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Participants</title>
<p>All participants (
<italic>n</italic>
= 23) were graduate students following a master's degree course in music at a state university in U.S.A. All participants had extended advanced musical training. The task was completed using a bespoke designed online listening form/instrument.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec sec-type="results" id="s2">
<title>Results</title>
<sec>
<title>Performer-sex identifications</title>
<p>With 35 extracts in each sequence, and a two-alternative performer–sex response of “male” or “female,” the inherent probability of the measures was
<italic>p</italic>
= 0.5, giving an expected score at the binomial mean of 17.5. The range of scores that could be regarded as non-significant variants of the binomial mean would notionally extend between ±1.96σ (i.e., the point where
<italic>p</italic>
= 0.05 probability will commence), here ranging from a minimum score of 11.7 to a maximum at 23.3. Scores significantly removed from the mean would therefore lie below 12 or above 24.</p>
<p>The mean score for the combined A and B sequences obtained by the 69 listeners was 17.57, (17.53 for listeners to sequence A, 17.97 for listeners to sequence B, difference = 0.44,
<italic>t</italic>
= 0.60,
<italic>df</italic>
67,
<italic>p</italic>
= 0.55, not significant), i.e., both located almost exactly at the binomial mean. It must therefore be concluded that, as a group, our listeners were not able to determine the sex of a performer, and this outcome applied to respondents to each of the “A” and “B” sequences.</p>
<p>The distribution of scores was slightly positively skewed about the mean. Two listeners recorded performer-sex decisions slightly above the upper expected upper limit of chance scores, both making 26/35 correct decisions. Given a total of 69 listeners, this would be within a number of borderline significant scores possibly gained by chance, and their performance is therefore not interpreted as indicating presence of reliable ability to identify the sex of a performer.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Effects of sex of listener</title>
<p>Sex of listeners did not affect performer–sex decisions: the mean of correct decisions by male listeners was 17.45 and for female listeners 18.16,
<italic>t</italic>
= 0.91,
<italic>df</italic>
63,
<italic>p</italic>
= 0.55, not significant. Responses to performances by male and female artists were examined by means of multiple regression analyses using the “backward entry” method, in order to identify possible effects of age-group, work-role and sex of listeners on judgments made for both male and female artists. None of these variables was found to have significant relationships with the dependent variables, and no tendency was found for female listeners to make superior judgments in respect of female performers, nor for male listeners to greater accuracy in respect of male performers (Table
<xref ref-type="table" rid="T1">1</xref>
).</p>
<table-wrap id="T1" position="float">
<label>Table 1</label>
<caption>
<p>
<bold>Effects of sex, work-role and age-group of listeners on performer-sex identifications</bold>
.</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th rowspan="1" colspan="1"></th>
<th align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>Male performers (F = 1.462,
<italic>df</italic>
3, p = 0.234, n.s.)</bold>
</th>
<th align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>Female performers (F = 0.363,
<italic>df</italic>
3, p = 0.780, n.s.)</bold>
</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Sex of listener</td>
<td align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<italic>t</italic>
= 0.813,
<italic>p</italic>
= 0.420, n.s.</td>
<td align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<italic>t</italic>
= 0.846,
<italic>p</italic>
= 0.401, n.s.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Age-group of listener</td>
<td align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<italic>t</italic>
= 1.385,
<italic>p</italic>
= 0.171, n.s.</td>
<td align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<italic>t</italic>
= 0.727,
<italic>p</italic>
= 0.470, n.s.</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Work-role of listener</td>
<td align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<italic>t</italic>
= 1.447,
<italic>p</italic>
= 0.153, n.s.</td>
<td align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<italic>t</italic>
= 0.165,
<italic>p</italic>
= 0.869, n.s.</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</table-wrap>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Music characteristic ratings</title>
<p>Ratings for the 35 musical extracts on the four music characteristics scales showed remarkable consistency among the 23 listeners, with a mean inter-listener correlation of
<italic>r</italic>
= 0.545,
<italic>p</italic>
< 0.001. Of the 506 inter-listener correlations, only 12 failed to reach significance at
<italic>p</italic>
= 0.05 level, and most were significant
<italic>p</italic>
< 0.001. Between-sex differences in the application of the four scales by male and female listeners yielded non-significant
<italic>t</italic>
values. These results were taken as validation of the four scales as effective measures of musical affect.</p>
<p>The mean range of ratings was narrow for all four of the 9-point scales, extending over only 1.85 (
<italic>SD</italic>
= 0.427) for the “Tempo” scale, 2.04 (
<italic>SD</italic>
= 0.695) for the “controlled–objective / sensitive–emotional” scale, 2.37 (0.660) for the “calm–reflective / stressed–dramatic” scale and 2.23 (0.635) for “mild–submissive / assertive–masterful” scale. This reflected the high degree of agreement between these musician listeners. This was again taken as a validation of the scales as being meaningful verbal descriptors of real musical characteristics.</p>
<p>Ratings for three of the music characteristics scales were highly correlated (Table
<xref ref-type="table" rid="T2">2</xref>
).</p>
<table-wrap id="T2" position="float">
<label>Table 2</label>
<caption>
<p>
<bold>Correlations evident among ratings for the four music characteristics' scales</bold>
.</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<thead>
<tr>
<th rowspan="1" colspan="1"></th>
<th align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>Controlled-objective / sensitive-emotional</bold>
</th>
<th align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>Calm-reflective / stressed-dramatic</bold>
</th>
<th align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<bold>Mild-submissive / assertive-masterful</bold>
</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Tempo: adagio-prestissimo</td>
<td align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<italic>r</italic>
= −0.659,
<italic>p</italic>
< 0.001</td>
<td align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<italic>r</italic>
= 0.702,
<italic>p</italic>
< 0.001</td>
<td align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<italic>r</italic>
= 0.737,
<italic>p</italic>
< 0.001</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Controlled-objective / sensitive – emotional</td>
<td rowspan="1" colspan="1"></td>
<td align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<italic>r</italic>
= 0.304 not sig.</td>
<td align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<italic>r</italic>
= −0.336,
<italic>p</italic>
< 0.05</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Calm-reflective / stressed-dramatic</td>
<td rowspan="1" colspan="1"></td>
<td rowspan="1" colspan="1"></td>
<td align="center" valign="top" rowspan="1" colspan="1">
<italic>r</italic>
= 0.941,
<italic>p</italic>
< 0.001</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</table-wrap>
<p>These intercorrelations reveal a somewhat complex relationship among the four dimensions (Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F4">4</xref>
), centered primarily on tempo: faster tempi were associated with perception of the music as more stressed and dramatic, more assertive and masterful, more controlled and objective: slower tempi were associated with music perceived as more calm and reflective, mild and submissive, sensitive and emotional.</p>
<fig id="F4" position="float">
<label>Figure 4</label>
<caption>
<p>
<bold>Relationships of music characteristics to tempi</bold>
.</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="fpsyg-05-00276-g0004"></graphic>
</fig>
<p>When the inter-relationships of the four scales were subjected to multiple regression analyses independently of the dominant effects of fast or slow tempi, statistically significant associations were observed between
<list list-type="bullet">
<list-item>
<p>“Tempo” and “mild–submissive / assertive–masterful” scales (
<italic>F</italic>
= 6.84,
<italic>df</italic>
1.21,
<italic>p</italic>
< 0.02)</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>“calm–reflective / stressed–dramatic” and “mild–submissive / assertive–masterful” (
<italic>F</italic>
= 27.14,
<italic>df</italic>
2.21,
<italic>p</italic>
< 0.001),</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>“controlled–objective / Sensitive–dramatic” and “mild–submissive / assertive–masterful” (
<italic>F</italic>
= 6.52,
<italic>df</italic>
1.21,
<italic>p</italic>
< 0.02).</p>
</list-item>
</list>
</p>
<p>MASCFEM scores for the combined A and B sequences were found to be significantly higher (i.e., more feminine) for extracts rated lower on the “controlled–objective / sensitive–emotional scale” (
<italic>t</italic>
= 2.69,
<italic>df</italic>
68,
<italic>p</italic>
0.009), the “calm–reflective/stressed–dramatic” scale (
<italic>t</italic>
= 1.98,
<italic>df</italic>
68,
<italic>p</italic>
0.05) and the “mild–submissive / assertive–masterful” scales (
<italic>t</italic>
= 2.00,
<italic>df</italic>
68,
<italic>p</italic>
0.05). The masculine / feminine directional polarities that emerge are shown at Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F5">5</xref>
.</p>
<fig id="F5" position="float">
<label>Figure 5</label>
<caption>
<p>
<bold>Perceived Gendered propensities of music characteristics</bold>
.</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="fpsyg-05-00276-g0005"></graphic>
</fig>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Effects of era of composition</title>
<p>Possible effects of era of composition on affect ratings of the musical extracts were examined by applying weightings to the extracts: extracts composed between 1650 and 1800 (era 1: baroque-classical) were assigned a weighting of 1, extracts composed between 1801 and 1900 (era 2: romantic) were weighted 2 and those composed between 1901 and 2000 (era 3: post-romantic-modern) were weighted 3. Ratings for the “controlled–objective / sensitive–emotional” scale were found to be related to the eras of composition of the musical extracts. Unsurprisingly, compositions of era 2, the so-called “era of Romanticism” were rated significantly higher on this scale than were compositions of the preceding and following compositional eras. Rating contrasts between compositional eras were: era 1 vs. era 2,
<italic>t</italic>
= 4.927,
<italic>df</italic>
22,
<italic>p</italic>
< 0.001; Era 1 vs. era 3,
<italic>t</italic>
= 2.656,
<italic>df</italic>
. 20,
<italic>p</italic>
= 0.015, Era 2 (mean = 6.45) vs. era 3 (mean = 5.55) not significant.</p>
<p>Although these results indicate that differences in affect levels were recognized by the music-characteristic listeners, (music of era 1 judged to differ qualitatively from that of eras 2 and 3) neither accuracy of performer-sex decisions nor scores on the MASCFEM scales reached significance for any era. Our results therefore provide no evidence of qualitative differences between male and female performers; female artists are not identifiable by reason of any tendencies to emotionality stereotypically attributed to women, or empathy with affective characteristics of the music performed.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Summary of findings</title>
<list list-type="simple">
<list-item>
<p>(1) Performer-sex discrimination: mean scores for the two performer-sex discrimination sequences were very close to their binomial means, indicating that as a group these musically experienced listeners were not able to determine the sex of a performer when restricted to auditory information.</p>
</list-item>
</list>
<p>Neither Sex, age-group nor musical work-role of listeners were found to be related to accuracy of performer-sex decisions. No superiority of decisions in respect of performances by own-sex artists was found.</p>
<list list-type="simple">
<list-item>
<p>(2) Music characteristic ratings by 23 musically trained listeners on the four scales showed a high level of consistency, with most inter-listener correlations reaching significance p<.001. Analysis of between-scale correlations revealed complex but systematic relationships among variables, with tempo as the controlling parameter.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>(3) The MASCFEM scale enabled a more refined analysis of potentially gendered polarities in the judgments of listeners in the performer-sex task and possible relationships to music characteristics ratings. Gendered tendencies were noted but these were strongly related to tempo of the musical extract, and no relationship to the sex of performer was evident.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>(4) Eras of composition: although ratings on the music-characteristic scales showed that listeners consistently identified differences in affect characteristics of music of the three eras of musical composition, this was not found to be related to performer-sex identifications, female artists were not found to reveal their sex by a detectably greater empathy for music rated as having greater emotional valence.</p>
</list-item>
</list>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Discussion and conclusions</title>
<p>Perception of gendered qualities by the listener at the final stage of the musical information transfer would necessarily lead to one of two conclusion: either the gendering originated at one of the preceding stages—composition, or performance—and was therefore present in the message as it reached the listener, or that it was a product of the listener's own cognitive constructs. In the latter case gendering would be a subjective property subscribed by the listener and could not be considered to be an inherent quality of the message.</p>
<p>In this context, the validity of the three propositions can each be evaluated from the results of the study.</p>
<p>Proposition 1: that musical structures inherently possesses characteristics that are gendered, irrespective of era and style of the music's composition.</p>
<p>Imposition of gendered information at the compositional stage would presumably require a deliberate intention on the part of the composer, but no indicative criteria have yet been proposed against which a composer could determine which sounds would be perceived as masculine and which would be feminine.</p>
<p>Of course, as McClary (
<xref rid="B98" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
, pp. 56–65) points out, male composers do sometimes write passages that are intentionally descriptive of feminine qualities, witness an example which she discusses at some length—the
<italic>habañera</italic>
sung by Carmen in the first act of the opera of that name by Georges Bizet, in which she taunts her would-be lover with promises of gratification “… this year, next year, sometime, never …”! Here the composer sets out to portray the sexual allure of the gipsy factory girl, as she flaunts her womanly attractions. Ballantine (
<xref rid="B5" ref-type="bibr">1984</xref>
, p. 52) similarly draws attention to the sexual ambiguity of the music at the point of the “double-blind” of cross-dressing and a soprano-voiced male character during the interaction between Susanna, Cherubino, and Countess Rosina in Act II of Mozart's opera
<italic>Le Nozze di Figaro</italic>
(see Keyser,
<xref rid="B76" ref-type="bibr">1987</xref>
for a commentary). Gendered musical intentions such as these, however, are particular to the genres of music-drama and dance music which according to Hanna “is embedded in divine sanction of sex and erotic fantasy” 1988, p. xvii), and there are no grounds for an assumption that because they exist in these specific situations they may therefore be considered to be equally present in the wider genre of instrumental music.</p>
<p>Proposition 1: results of the performer-sex discrimination task show that listeners do not recover gendered information from the musical signal as passed on by the performer. Unless it is argued to have been removed during the performance, it cannot be held to have been present in the compositional gestures set out by the composer. Proposition 1 therefore fails, and gendered information, if present in the signal, could only have been imparted at the stage of performance.</p>
<p>Proposition 2: that male and female performers imbue the overall musical message with qualities or characteristics unique to their own-performer-sex.</p>
<p>A minimum criterial requirement for validation of this proposition would be that appropriately musically experienced listeners should demonstrate a statistically reliable ability to determine which of the
<italic>male/female</italic>
polarities of the gendered information was represented in musical works they hear. Our 69 musically experienced listeners signally failed to demonstrate such ability: mean scores for identifications were very close to the binomial mean, and well within the range of chance. Male and female listeners were equally unsuccessful, and between-sex
<italic>t</italic>
tests proved non-significant.</p>
<p>We therefore conclude that any “change of state” experienced by our listeners was not conditioned by information relating to the sex of performer. The second proposition is therefore not substantive.</p>
<p>Proposition 3: that male and female listeners take away different features and “delineated gendered meanings” from the musical experience.</p>
<p>In so far as an ability to identify the sex of a performer can be considered an essential condition for presence of such gendered information, our listeners failed to provide evidence to support this third proposition. Performer-sex identifications made by male and female listeners in respect of both male and female artists all failed to rise above chance level. Regression analyses failed to yield significant
<italic>F</italic>
values for the sex, age-group or music work-role of listeners, and even the summed variances for these three variables made negligible contributions to the total variances. On prima facie evidence therefore the third proposition appears not to find support in our data.</p>
<p>Verbal descriptors can at best provide approximations to actual musical experience; nevertheless their use has long-enduring provenance as a means of measurement, (see Asmus,
<xref rid="B3" ref-type="bibr">2009</xref>
, for an extended review). The 23 musically trained listeners rated the 35 extracts of the listening sequences against four “Music Characteristics” scales whose qualifiers were descriptors of qualities inherent in the music itself, not of the performances. The listeners were assigned by the online system randomly to sequences A and B, in the manner previously described, so that differences and correlations observed among the data therefore derive from performances by artists of both sexes. The four scales yielded very concise data that showed remarkable consistency. This persuades us that the semantic differentials employed to define the polarities of the scales were valid as qualities that musicians recognized as present in the musical extracts they heard.</p>
<p>Three of the scales were found to be highly correlated, creating a triadic relationship, with tempo as its pivotal quality. This is conformant with well-rehearsed evidence: early studies (Hevner,
<xref rid="B59" ref-type="bibr">1936</xref>
,
<xref rid="B60" ref-type="bibr">1937</xref>
, p. 625; Rigg,
<xref rid="B119" ref-type="bibr">1940</xref>
) had observed that speed was a primary determinant of musical mood and listener response, and that variations in tempo invoked strong effects on affective responses. Budd (
<xref rid="B19" ref-type="bibr">1985</xref>
), similarly describes tempo as an “essential musical device for expression of emotion,” and a critical factor of musical interpretation. Other studies have yielded similar findings (e.g., Brown,
<xref rid="B18" ref-type="bibr">1979</xref>
, p. 32; Duerr,
<xref rid="B35" ref-type="bibr">1981</xref>
; Geringer and Madsen,
<xref rid="B51" ref-type="bibr">1984</xref>
,
<xref rid="B52" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
; Holbrook and Arnand,
<xref rid="B62" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
; Gabrielsson and Lindström,
<xref rid="B48" ref-type="bibr">1995</xref>
; Sheldon and Gregory,
<xref rid="B129" ref-type="bibr">1997</xref>
; Gagnon and Peretz,
<xref rid="B49" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
; Webster and Weir,
<xref rid="B150" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
).</p>
<p>Ratings on the “controlled–objective vs. sensitive-emotional” scale were found to differ between musical works composed during the three compositional eras, 1650–1800, 1801–1900, and 1901–2000.</p>
<p>The MASCFEM scale enabled measurement of masculinity/femininity tendencies in the “Music Characteristics” ratings. Since the Performer–sex task listeners were a separate group from those generating the “Music Characteristics” ratings, data from the two sets of judgments were independent, and comparisons of the two scales are free from cross-contamination.</p>
<p>The results of this analysis run counter to stereotypical expectations: on the basis of much-repeated wisdoms, partly supported by research evidence, it might have been anticipated that greater emotionality and “sensitive-dramatic” polarity adjudged to be present in some music extracts would have been rated more highly toward the feminine polarity of the “MASCFEM” scale. The opposite was the case: MASCFEM scores were higher (i.e., more feminine) for extracts that were judged to be more “controlled-objective,” “calm-reflective” and “mild-submissive.” This effect may possibly be related to Kemp's (
<xref rid="B75" ref-type="bibr">1996</xref>
) observations that personality traits of musicians tend to run counter to trends observed for normal population samples.</p>
<p>Our conclusions from this study are that (i) except in special situations such as those discussed by McClary and Ballantine, music structures, at the point of composition, are innocent of gender implication, and that gendering is not an inherent quality of music structure,
<italic>per se</italic>
; (ii) our data do not support claims that performers impart their own sex-specific information into the sonic soundscape of their musical performances. The evidence of the inter-correlations between ratings of the four music characteristics scales (Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F4">4</xref>
) and the polarities of emotional valence indicated by application of the MASCFEM scale (Figure
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="F5">5</xref>
), however, suggest that gender-related perceptions may be understood by listeners. We conclude that if gendered perception of music is a reality, the perceived properties are subjectively imposed on the musical message by the listener,” through the listener's appropriations” (DeNora,
<xref rid="B32" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
) and are primarily related to the tempo of the music.</p>
<sec>
<title>Conflict of interest statement</title>
<p>The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
</body>
<back>
<fn-group>
<fn id="fn0001">
<p>
<sup>1</sup>
At an early stage in the development of music computing, when reliable sound synthesis first became a reality, a forthcoming radio programme was announced which was to include a computer-generated performance of a work by J. S. Bach. The news was greeted with keen anticipation by a sector of the press; it was seen by press music critics as heralding the dawn of a new age for music performance. Here was a new means of flawless performance: not only would it be wholly error-free, it would be liberated from those personal vagaries and idiosyncrasies of a human performer that were held to mask the composers true intentions. A computer-based performance could thus bring direct and definitive insights into the composer's mind.</p>
<p>Retrospective commentary after the broadcast, whilst acknowledging the event to have been a significant engineering achievement, generally recognized that, as a musical experience, the performance had disappointed. It had seemed lifeless, dull and mechanical, and the anticipated insights had not been realized. The event served as a useful reminder that it is those very human vagaries and idiosyncrasies that release a musical work from the constraints of the otherwise ‘dead’ printed score (Goebl et al.,
<xref rid="B53" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
) and give it expressive meaning, that “the letter killeth, but the spirit giveth life” (St. Paul; Campbell,
<xref rid="B123" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
). As Sloboda (
<xref rid="B131" ref-type="bibr">1986</xref>
) comments, if technical mastery and accuracy of tone and timing are all there are to music performance, we may just as well pass over the task to computer programmers and close our conservatoires.</p>
</fn>
<fn id="fn0002">
<p>
<sup>2</sup>
“Even if I am not a creative artist, still I am recreating” (Clara Schumann, as cited in Reich,
<xref rid="B116" ref-type="bibr">1985</xref>
).</p>
</fn>
<fn id="fn0003">
<p>
<sup>3</sup>
The performer “must play everything in such a way that he himself will be moved by it.” Leopold Mozart, cited by (Strunk,
<xref rid="B134" ref-type="bibr">1950</xref>
).</p>
</fn>
<fn id="fn0004">
<p>
<sup>4</sup>
The expression “feminine ending” is not unique to music, it is also used in the fields of linguistics and literature to describe a two-phoneme rhyme of which the penultimate is stressed but the final unstressed, (“
<italic>comment</italic>
,” “
<italic>gravel</italic>
” “
<italic>torrent</italic>
”), whereas in a masculine rhyme the stress is on the final syllable of the words (
<italic>restore</italic>
,
<italic>sublime</italic>
).</p>
</fn>
<fn id="fn0005">
<p>
<sup>5</sup>
The “Red Catalogue” listing all published recordings in CD format for the year in question listed no solo recordings of women players of French horn, trombone, tuba or percussion (Red Catalogue,
<xref rid="B141" ref-type="bibr">2004</xref>
).</p>
</fn>
</fn-group>
<sec sec-type="supplementary material" id="s3">
<title>Supplementary material</title>
<p>The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.frontiersin.org/journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00276/abstract">http://www.frontiersin.org/journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00276/abstract</ext-link>
</p>
<supplementary-material content-type="local-data">
<media xlink:href="Presentation1.PDF">
<caption>
<p>Click here for additional data file.</p>
</caption>
</media>
</supplementary-material>
</sec>
<ref-list>
<title>References</title>
<ref id="B3">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Asmus</surname>
<given-names>E. P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>The measurement of Musical expression</article-title>
, in
<source>Paper to Suncoast Music Education Symposium</source>
(
<publisher-loc>Tampa, FL</publisher-loc>
).</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B4">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Balkwill</surname>
<given-names>L.-L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thompson</surname>
<given-names>W. F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<article-title>A cross-cultural investigation of perception of emotion in music: psychological and cultural cues</article-title>
.
<source>Music Percept</source>
.
<volume>17</volume>
,
<fpage>43</fpage>
<lpage>69</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/40285811</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B5">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ballantine</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1984</year>
).
<source>Music and its Social Meanings</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Gordon and Breach</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B6">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Banaji</surname>
<given-names>M. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Greenwald</surname>
<given-names>A. G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>Implicit gender stereotyping in judgments of fame</article-title>
.
<source>J. Pers. Soc. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>68</volume>
,
<fpage>181</fpage>
<lpage>198</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.68.2.181</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">7877095</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B7">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Banse</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Scherer</surname>
<given-names>K. R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1996</year>
).
<article-title>Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression</article-title>
.
<source>J. Pers. Soc. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>70</volume>
,
<fpage>614</fpage>
<lpage>636</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.614</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8851745</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B8">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Baron-Cohen</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Knickmeyer</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Belmote</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Sex differences in the brain: implications for explaining autism</article-title>
.
<source>Science</source>
<volume>310</volume>
,
<fpage>819</fpage>
<lpage>823</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1126/science.1115455</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16272115</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B9">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Behrens</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1985</year>
).
<article-title>The perception of stress and lateralization of prosody</article-title>
.
<source>Brain Lang</source>
.
<volume>26</volume>
,
<fpage>332</fpage>
<lpage>348</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0093-934X(85)90047-1</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">4084769</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B10">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Besson</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Magne</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schön</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Emotional prosody: sex differences in sensitivity to speech melody</article-title>
.
<source>Trends Cogn. Sci</source>
.
<volume>10</volume>
,
<fpage>405</fpage>
<lpage>407</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01975-7</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12413566</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B11">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bhatara</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tirovolas</surname>
<given-names>A. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Duan</surname>
<given-names>L. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Levy</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Levitin</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Perception of emotional expression in musical performance</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>37</volume>
,
<fpage>921</fpage>
<lpage>934</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/a0021922</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21261418</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B12">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Biddleconbe</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1992</year>
).
<article-title>Review of ‘Feminine endings: music, gender and sexuality’ by Susan McClary</article-title>
.
<source>Pop. Music</source>
<volume>11</volume>
,
<fpage>373</fpage>
<lpage>376</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1017/S0261143000005225</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B13">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bolinger</surname>
<given-names>D. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1978</year>
).
<article-title>Intonation across languages</article-title>
, in
<source>Universals of Human Language</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Greenberg</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Stanford, CA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Stanford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>471</fpage>
<lpage>524</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B14">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Brattico</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Näätänen</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tervaniemi</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Context effects on pitch perception in Musicians and nonMusicians: evidence from event-related-potential recordings</article-title>
.
<source>Music Percept</source>
.
<volume>19</volume>
,
<fpage>199</fpage>
<lpage>227</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1525/mp.2001.19.2.199</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B15">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Brett</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wood</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thomas</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(eds.). (
<year>2006</year>
).
<source>Queering the pitch: The New Gay and Lesbian Musicology</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Abingdon</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Taylor Francis</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B16">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Brown</surname>
<given-names>C. P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<article-title>Sex and hemisphere differences for rapid auditory processing in normal adults</article-title>
.
<source>Laterality</source>
<volume>4</volume>
,
<fpage>39</fpage>
<lpage>50</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/713754320</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15513103</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B17">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Brown</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1986</year>
).
<source>New Grove Russian Masters</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Norton</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B18">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Brown</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1979</year>
).
<article-title>An enquiry into the origins and nature of tempo behaviour</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>7</volume>
,
<fpage>19</fpage>
<lpage>35</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/030573567971003</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B19">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Budd</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1985</year>
).
<source>Music and Emotions: The philosophical Theories</source>
.
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Routledge Keegan Paul</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B123">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Campbell</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<source>King James Bible. 400th Anniversary Edition</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B20">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Canli</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Desmond</surname>
<given-names>J. E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zhao</surname>
<given-names>Z.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gabrieli</surname>
<given-names>J. D. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Sex differences in the neural bases of emotional memories</article-title>
.
<source>Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A</source>
.
<volume>99</volume>
,
<fpage>10789</fpage>
<lpage>10794</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1073/pnas.162356599</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12145327</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B21">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Christensen</surname>
<given-names>O. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>Interpretation and meaning in music</article-title>
, in
<source>Musical Singnifications: Essays in the Semiotic Theory and Musical Analysis</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Tarasti</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>The Hague</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Mouton de Gruyter</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>81</fpage>
<lpage>108</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1515/9783110885187.81</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B22">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Citron</surname>
<given-names>M. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1993</year>
).
<source>Gender and the Musical Canon</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Illinois University Press and CUP Cambridge</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B24">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Coker</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1972</year>
).
<source>Music and Meaning: a Theoretical Introduction to Musical Aesthetics</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>The Free Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B25">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cusick</surname>
<given-names>S. G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>On a lesbian relationship with music: a serious effort not to think straight</article-title>
, in
<source>Queering the Pitch</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Brett</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wood</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thomas</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Abingdon</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Taylor Francis</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>67</fpage>
<lpage>84</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B26">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cutietta</surname>
<given-names>R. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>McAllister</surname>
<given-names>P. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1997</year>
).
<article-title>Student personality and instrumental participation, continuation, and choice</article-title>
.
<source>J. Res. Music Educ</source>
.
<volume>45</volume>
,
<fpage>282</fpage>
<lpage>294</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/3345587</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B27">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>D'Indy</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1909</year>
).
<source>Cours de composition nusicale</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Paris</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Durand et fils</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B28">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Daly</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Warren</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Pitching it differently in New Zealand English: speaker sex and intonation patterns</article-title>
.
<source>J. Sociolinguist</source>
.
<volume>5</volume>
,
<fpage>85</fpage>
<lpage>96</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/1467-9481.00139</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B29">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Deaux</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lewis</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1984</year>
).
<article-title>Structure of gender stereotypes: interrelationships among components and gender label</article-title>
.
<source>J. Pers. Soc. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>46</volume>
,
<fpage>991</fpage>
<lpage>1004</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0042-3524.46.991</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B30">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Dellacherie</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Roy</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hugueville</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Peretz</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Samson</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>The effect of musical experience on emotional self-reports and psychophysiological responses to dissonance</article-title>
.
<source>Psychophysiology</source>
<volume>48</volume>
,
<fpage>337</fpage>
<lpage>349</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01075.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20701708</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B31">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Delsing</surname>
<given-names>M. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ter Bogt</surname>
<given-names>T. F. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Engels</surname>
<given-names>R. C. M. E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Meeus</surname>
<given-names>W. H. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Adolescents' Music preferences and personality characteristics</article-title>
.
<source>Eur. J. Pers</source>
.
<volume>22</volume>
,
<fpage>109</fpage>
<lpage>130</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1002/per.665</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B32">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>DeNora</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<source>Music in Everyday Life</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1017/CBO9780511489433</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B33">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Descartes</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1618</year>
).
<source>Abrege de Musique</source>
. (
<person-group person-group-type="translator">
<name>
<surname>de Buzon</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
, Trans.).
<publisher-loc>Paris</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Presses Universitaire de France</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B34">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Dollinger</surname>
<given-names>S. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1993</year>
).
<article-title>Research note: personality and music preference: extraversion and excitement seeking or openness to experience</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>21</volume>
,
<fpage>73</fpage>
<lpage>77</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/030573569302100105</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B35">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Duerr</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1981</year>
).
<article-title>Rhythm in music: a formal scaffolding of time</article-title>
, in
<source>The Voices of Time</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Fraser</surname>
<given-names>J. T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Amherst, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Massachusetts Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>180</fpage>
<lpage>201</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B36">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Dunn</surname>
<given-names>P. G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>de Ruyter</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bouwhuis</surname>
<given-names>D. G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Toward a better understanding of the relation between music preference, listening behaviour, and personality</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>40</volume>
,
<fpage>411</fpage>
<lpage>427</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735610388897</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B37">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Eisenberg</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lennon</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1983</year>
).
<article-title>Sex differences in empathy and related capacities</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Bull</source>
.
<volume>94</volume>
,
<fpage>100</fpage>
<lpage>131</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-2909.94.1.100</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B38">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Evers</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dannert</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rödding</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rötter</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<article-title>The cerebral dynamics of music perception</article-title>
.
<source>Brain</source>
<volume>122</volume>
,
<fpage>75</fpage>
<lpage>85</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1093/brain/122.1.75</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10050896</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B39">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Feingold</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>Gender differences in personality: a meta-analysis</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Bull</source>
.
<volume>116</volume>
,
<fpage>429</fpage>
<lpage>256</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-2909.116.3.429</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">7809307</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B40">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Feld</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1984</year>
).
<article-title>Communication, music and speech about music</article-title>
.
<source>Yearb. Tradit. Music</source>
<volume>16</volume>
,
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>18</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/768199</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B41">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fernald</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1989</year>
).
<article-title>Intonation and communicative intent in mother's speech to infants: is the melody the message?</article-title>
<source>Child Dev</source>
.
<volume>60</volume>
,
<fpage>1947</fpage>
<lpage>1510</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/1130938</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">2612255</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B42">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Frick</surname>
<given-names>R. W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1985</year>
).
<article-title>Communicating emotion: the role of prosodic features</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Bull</source>
.
<volume>97</volume>
,
<fpage>412</fpage>
<lpage>429</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-2909.97.3.412</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B43">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Frith</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1996</year>
).
<article-title>Music and identity</article-title>
, in
<source>Questions of Cultural Identity</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Hall</surname>
<given-names>J. W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>DuGay</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Sage</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>108</fpage>
<lpage>117</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B44">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fuller</surname>
<given-names>C. D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Galvin</surname>
<given-names>J. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Free</surname>
<given-names>R. H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Başkent</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2014</year>
).
<article-title>Musician effect in cochlear implant simulated gender categorization</article-title>
.
<source>J. Acoust. Soc. Am</source>
.
<volume>135</volume>
,
<fpage>EL159</fpage>
<lpage>EL165</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1121/1.4865263</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">24606310</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B45">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gaab</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Keenan</surname>
<given-names>J. P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schlaug</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>The effect of gender on the neural substrates of pitch memory</article-title>
.
<source>J. Cogn. Neurosci</source>
.
<volume>15</volume>
,
<fpage>810</fpage>
<lpage>820</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1162/089892903322370735</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14511534</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B46">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gabrielsson</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1996</year>
).
<article-title>Emotional expression in Music performance between the performer's intention and the listener's experience</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>24</volume>
,
<fpage>68</fpage>
<lpage>91</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735696241007</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B47">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gabrielsson</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Emotional expression in Music</article-title>
, in
<source>Handbook of the Affective Sciences</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Davidson</surname>
<given-names>R. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sherer</surname>
<given-names>K. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Goldsmith</surname>
<given-names>H. H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>503</fpage>
<lpage>534</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B48">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gabrielsson</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lindström</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>Emotional expression in synthesizer and sentograph performance</article-title>
.
<source>Psychomusicology</source>
<volume>14</volume>
,
<fpage>94</fpage>
<lpage>116</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/h0094089</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B49">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gagnon</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Peretz</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Mode and tempo relative contributions to “happy-sad” judgements of equitone music</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Emot</source>
.
<volume>17</volume>
,
<fpage>25</fpage>
<lpage>40</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/02699930302279</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B51">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Geringer</surname>
<given-names>J. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Madsen</surname>
<given-names>C. K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1984</year>
).
<article-title>Pitch and tempo discrimination in recorded orchestral music among musicians and nonmusicians</article-title>
.
<source>J. Res. Music Educ</source>
.
<volume>32</volume>
,
<fpage>185</fpage>
<lpage>204</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/3344838</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B52">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Geringer</surname>
<given-names>J. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Madsen</surname>
<given-names>C. K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Gradual tempo change and aesthetic responses of music majors</article-title>
.
<source>Int. J. Music Educ</source>
.
<volume>40</volume>
,
<fpage>3</fpage>
<lpage>15</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/025576140304000102</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B53">
<mixed-citation publication-type="webpage">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Goebl</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dixon</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>De Poli</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Friberg</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bresu</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Widmer</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<source>‘Sense’ in Expressive Music performance: Data Acquisition, Comparative Computational Studies and Models</source>
. Available online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.cp.jku.at/research/papers/Goebl_etal_S2S2_2007.pdf">http://www.cp.jku.at/research/papers/Goebl_etal_S2S2_2007.pdf</ext-link>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B54">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Grahn</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rowe</surname>
<given-names>J. B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Feeling the beat: premotor and striatal interactions in musicians and nonmusicians during beat perception</article-title>
.
<source>J. Neurosci</source>
.
<volume>29</volume>
,
<fpage>7450</fpage>
<lpage>7548</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2018-08.2009</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19515913</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B55">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Green</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1997</year>
).
<source>Music, Gender, Education</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1017/CBO9780511585456</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B56">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hanna</surname>
<given-names>J. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1988</year>
).
<source>Dance, Sex and Gender: Signs of Identity, Dominance, Defiance and Desire</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Chicago, IL</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Chicago Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B57">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hargreaves</surname>
<given-names>D. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>MacDonald</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Miell</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>How do people communicate using music?</article-title>
in
<source>Musical Communication</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Miell</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hargreaves</surname>
<given-names>D. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>MacDonald</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
).
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198529361.003.0001</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B58">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Heiman</surname>
<given-names>M. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Description and prescription: how gender stereotypes prevent women's ascent up the organizational ladder</article-title>
.
<source>J. Soc. Issues</source>
<volume>57</volume>
,
<fpage>657</fpage>
<lpage>674</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/0022-4537.00234</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B59">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hevner</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1936</year>
).
<article-title>Experimental studies of the elements of expression in Music</article-title>
.
<source>Am. J. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>49</volume>
,
<fpage>246</fpage>
<lpage>268</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/1415746</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">19884142</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B60">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hevner</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1937</year>
).
<article-title>The affective value of pitch and tempo in music</article-title>
.
<source>Am. J. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>49</volume>
,
<fpage>621</fpage>
<lpage>630</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/1416385</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B61">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hoffman</surname>
<given-names>M. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1997</year>
).
<article-title>Sex differences in empathy and related behaviors</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Bull</source>
.
<volume>1977</volume>
,
<fpage>712</fpage>
<lpage>722</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-2909.84.4.712</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B62">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Holbrook</surname>
<given-names>M. B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Arnand</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<article-title>Effects of tempo and situational arousal in the listener's perceptual and affective responses to music</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>18</volume>
,
<fpage>150</fpage>
<lpage>162</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735690182004</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B63">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Holsinger</surname>
<given-names>B. W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<source>Music, Body and Desire in Mediavel Culture</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Stanford, CA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Stanford University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B64">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ilie</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thompson</surname>
<given-names>W. F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>A comparison of acoustic cues in music and speech for three dimensions of affect</article-title>
.
<source>Music Percept</source>
.
<volume>23</volume>
,
<fpage>319</fpage>
<lpage>330</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1525/mp.2006.23.4.319</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B65">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Imaizumi</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Homma</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ozawa</surname>
<given-names>Y.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Maruishi</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Muranaka</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Gender differences in emotional prosody processing – an fMRI study</article-title>
.
<source>Psychologia</source>
<volume>47</volume>
,
<fpage>113</fpage>
<lpage>124</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2117/psysoc.2004.113</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B66">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Johnson</surname>
<given-names>C. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1996</year>
).
<article-title>Musicians and nonmusicians assessment of perceived rubato in musical performance</article-title>
.
<source>J. Res. Music Educ</source>
.
<volume>44</volume>
,
<fpage>84</fpage>
<lpage>96</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/3345415</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B67">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Communicating emotion in music performance: a review and a theoretical framework</article-title>
, in
<source>Music and Emotion: Theory and Research</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sloboda</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>309</fpage>
<lpage>337</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B68">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Five facets of musical expression: a psychologist's perspective on music performance</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>31</volume>
,
<fpage>273</fpage>
<lpage>302</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/03057356030313003</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B69">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>From mimesis to catharsis: expression, perception and induction of emotion in music</article-title>
, in
<source>Music Communication</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Miell</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>MacDonald</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hargreaves</surname>
<given-names>D. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>85</fpage>
<lpage>116</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198529361.003.0005</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B70">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Emotion in music performance</article-title>
, in
<source>The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology</source>
eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Hallam</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cross</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thaut</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
).</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B71">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Laukka</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Communication of emotions in vocal and music performance: relation of performance to perception</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Bull</source>
.
<volume>129</volume>
,
<fpage>770</fpage>
<lpage>814</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.770</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12956543</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B72">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Laukka</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Expression, perception, and induction of Musical emotions: a review and a questionnaire study of everyday listening</article-title>
.
<source>J. New Music Res</source>
.
<volume>33</volume>
,
<fpage>217</fpage>
<lpage>101</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/0929821042000317813</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B73">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Katz</surname>
<given-names>S. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>The influence of the extra-musical on the composing process</article-title>
, in
<source>The Act of Music XComposition: Studies in the Creative Process</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Colllins</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Farnham</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Ashgate</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>171</fpage>
<lpage>186</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B74">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kelly</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<article-title>Gender-emotion stereotypes are context-specific</article-title>
.
<source>Sex Roles</source>
<volume>2040</volume>
,
<fpage>107</fpage>
<lpage>120</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1023/A:1018834501996</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B75">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kemp</surname>
<given-names>A. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1996</year>
).
<source>The Musical Temperament</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198523628.001.0001</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B76">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Keyser</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1987</year>
).
<article-title>Cross-sexual casting in baroque opera musical and theatrical conventions</article-title>
.
<source>Opera Q</source>
.
<volume>5</volume>
,
<fpage>46</fpage>
<lpage>57</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1093/oq/5.4.46</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B77">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Khatoon, M, Nighute</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ishaque</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Effect of gender on brainstem auditory evoked potential</article-title>
.
<source>J. Recent Adv. Appl. Sci</source>
.
<volume>28</volume>
,
<fpage>113</fpage>
<lpage>117</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B78">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kimura</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<source>Sex Differences in the Brain, Special Editions</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Publ. Nature America, New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Scientific American</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B79">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kitayama</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ishii</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Word and voice: spontaneous attention to emotional utterances in two languages</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Emot</source>
.
<volume>16</volume>
,
<fpage>29</fpage>
<lpage>59</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/0269993943000121</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B80">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Koelsch</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Grossman</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gunter</surname>
<given-names>T. C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hahne</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schröger</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Friederici</surname>
<given-names>A. D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003a</year>
).
<article-title>Children processing music: electric brain responses reveal musical competence and gender differences</article-title>
.
<source>J. Cogn. Neurosci</source>
.
<volume>15</volume>
,
<fpage>683</fpage>
<lpage>693</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1162/jocn.2003.15.5.683</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12965042</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B81">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Koelsch</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gunter</surname>
<given-names>T. C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wittfroth</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Interaction between syntax processing and language</article-title>
.
<source>J. Cogn. Neurosci</source>
.
<volume>17</volume>
,
<fpage>1565</fpage>
<lpage>1577</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1162/089892905774597290</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16269097</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B82">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Koelsch</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Maess</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Grossman</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Friederici</surname>
<given-names>A. D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003b</year>
).
<article-title>Electric brain responses reveal gender differences in music processing</article-title>
.
<source>Neuroreport</source>
<volume>14</volume>
,
<fpage>709</fpage>
<lpage>713</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1097/00001756-200304150-00010</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12692468</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B84">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Krumhansl</surname>
<given-names>C. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1997</year>
).
<article-title>An exploratory study of musical emotion and psychophysiology</article-title>
.
<source>Can. J. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>51</volume>
,
<fpage>336</fpage>
<lpage>365</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/1196-1961.51.4.336</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B85">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Krumhansl</surname>
<given-names>C. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Rhythm and pitch in music cognition</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Bull</source>
.
<volume>126</volume>
,
<fpage>159</fpage>
<lpage>179</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.159</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10668354</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B86">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kuhl</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>The semiotic gesture</article-title>
, in
<source>New Perspectives on Music and Gesture</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Gritten</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>King</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Farnham</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Ashgate</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>123</fpage>
<lpage>130</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B87">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Langmeyer</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Guglhör-Rudan</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tarna</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>What do music preferences reveal about personality? A cross-cultural replication using self-ratings and ratings of music samples</article-title>
.
<source>J. Individ. Diff</source>
.
<volume>33</volume>
,
<fpage>119</fpage>
<lpage>130</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1027/1614-0001/a000082</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B88">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lattner</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Meyer</surname>
<given-names>M. E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Friederici</surname>
<given-names>A. D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Voice perception, sex, pitch and the right hemisphere</article-title>
.
<source>Hum. Brain Mapp</source>
.
<volume>24</volume>
,
<fpage>11</fpage>
<lpage>20</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1002/hbm.20065</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15593269</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B89">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lehmann</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Expressive variants in the opening of Robert Schumann's Arlequin (from Carnaval, op 9): 54 pianists' interpretations of a metrical ambiguity. Cited by Kopiez</article-title>
, in
<source>Music and the Mind, Essays in Honour of John Sloboda</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Deliège</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Davidson</surname>
<given-names>J. W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
).</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B90">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Liljeström</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Juslin</surname>
<given-names>P. N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Västfjäll</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Experimental evidence of the roles of choice, social context, and listener personality in emotional reactions to Music</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
.
<volume>41</volume>
,
<fpage>579</fpage>
<lpage>599</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735612440615</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B91">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lin</surname>
<given-names>Y-P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wang</surname>
<given-names>C-H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jung</surname>
<given-names>T-P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wu</surname>
<given-names>T-L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jeng</surname>
<given-names>S-K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Duann</surname>
<given-names>J. R.</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>EEG-based emotion recognition in music listening</article-title>
.
<source>IEEE Trans. Biomed. Sci. TBME</source>
<volume>57</volume>
,
<fpage>1798</fpage>
<lpage>1806</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1109/TBME.2010.2048568</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20442037</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B92">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lowery</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1952</year>
).
<source>The Background of Music</source>
.
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Hutchinson's University Library</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B93">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lu</surname>
<given-names>Y.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wu</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Qian</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Xie</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Reserach on the influence of Mozart sonata K.448 on EEG power spectrum and gravity frequency</article-title>
.
<source>Acta Biophysica</source>
<volume>27</volume>
,
<fpage>154</fpage>
<lpage>166</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3724/SP.J.1260.2011.00154</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B94">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Luck</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Saarikallio</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Burger</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thompson</surname>
<given-names>M. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tolvainen</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Effets of the big five and musical genre on music-induced movement</article-title>
.
<source>J. Res. Pers</source>
.
<volume>44</volume>
,
<fpage>714</fpage>
<lpage>720</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jrp.2010.10.001</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B95">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Marques</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Moreno</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Castro</surname>
<given-names>S. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Besson</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Musicians detect pitch violations in a foreign language better than nonmusicians: behavioural and electrophysiological evidence</article-title>
.
<source>J. Cogn. Neurosci</source>
.
<volume>19</volume>
,
<fpage>1453</fpage>
<lpage>1463</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1453</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17714007</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B96">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Marx</surname>
<given-names>A. B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1845</year>
).
<source>Die Lehre von der musikalischen Kompositionen</source>
. Vol. Part 3.
<publisher-loc>Leipzig</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Breitkopf and Hertel</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B97">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Maus</surname>
<given-names>F. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1993</year>
).
<article-title>Masculine discourse in music theory</article-title>
.
<source>Perspect. New Music</source>
<volume>31</volume>
,
<fpage>264</fpage>
<lpage>293</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/833390</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B98">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>McClary</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<source>Feminine Endings in Retrospect</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Minneapolis, MN</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Minnesota Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B99">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Meyer</surname>
<given-names>L. B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1961</year>
).
<source>Emotion and Meaning in Music</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Chicago, IL</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Chicago Press</publisher-name>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.7208/chicago/9780226521374.001.0001</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B100">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Mitchell</surname>
<given-names>R. L. C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Elliott</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Barry</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cruttenden</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Woodruff</surname>
<given-names>P. W. R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>The neural response to emotional prosody, as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging</article-title>
.
<source>Neurpsychologia</source>
<volume>14</volume>
,
<fpage>1410</fpage>
<lpage>1421</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00017-4</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B101">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Monelle</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>Music and semantics</article-title>
, in
<source>Musical Signification:
<italic>Approaches</italic>
to
<italic>Semiotics</italic>
</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Tarasti</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Mouton de Gruyter</publisher-loc>
),
<fpage>91</fpage>
<lpage>108</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1515/9783110885187.91</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B102">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Monelle</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<source>The Sense of Music: Semiotic Essays</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Princeton, NJ</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Princeton University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B103">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Nater</surname>
<given-names>U. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Abbruzzese</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Krebs</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ehlert</surname>
<given-names>U.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Sex differences in emotional and psychophysiological responses to musical stimuli</article-title>
.
<source>Int. J. Psychophysiol</source>
.
<volume>62</volume>
,
<fpage>300</fpage>
<lpage>308</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.05.011</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16828911</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B104">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Nettl</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2014</year>
).
<source>Music</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B105">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Nygaard</surname>
<given-names>L. C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Queen</surname>
<given-names>J. S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Communicating emotion: linking affective prosody and word meaning</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform</source>
.
<volume>34</volume>
,
<fpage>1017</fpage>
<lpage>1030</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0096-1523.34.4.1017</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18665742</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B106">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Obleser</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rockstroh</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Eulitz</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Gender differences in hemispheric asymmetry of syllable processing: left-lateralized magnetic N100 varies with syllable categorization in females</article-title>
.
<source>Psychophysiology</source>
<volume>41</volume>
,
<fpage>783</fpage>
<lpage>388</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00204.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15318884</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B107">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Olsson</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Social issues in music education</article-title>
, in
<source>International Handbook of Research in Arts Education</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Brewer</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Springer</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>989</fpage>
<lpage>1002</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/978-1-4020-3052-9_68</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B108">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Onishi</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Matsuda</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Asada</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Aruga</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hirakata</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nishakawa</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Functional anatomy of Musical perception in Musicians</article-title>
.
<source>Cereb. Cortex</source>
<volume>11</volume>
,
<fpage>754</fpage>
<lpage>760</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1093/cercor/11.8.754</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11459765</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B109">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Panksepp</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>The emotional sources of “chills” induced by music</article-title>
.
<source>Music Percept</source>
.
<volume>13</volume>
,
<fpage>171</fpage>
<lpage>207</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/40285693</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B112">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Pearsall</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Almen</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>The divining rod: on imagination, interpretation and analysis</article-title>
, in
<source>Approaches to Meaning in Music</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Amen</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pearsall</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Bloomington, IN</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Indiana University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>10</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B114">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Peraino</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Listening to the sirens: music as queer ethical practice</article-title>
.
<source>GLQ J. Lesbian Gay Stud</source>
.
<volume>9</volume>
,
<fpage>433</fpage>
<lpage>470</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1215/10642684-9-4-433</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B115">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rawlings</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ciancarelli</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1997</year>
).
<article-title>Music preference and the five-factor model of the NEO personality inventory</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
.
<volume>25</volume>
,
<fpage>120</fpage>
<lpage>132</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735697252003</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B116">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Reich</surname>
<given-names>N. B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1985</year>
).
<source>Clara Schumann the Artist and the Woman</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Ithaca, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cornell University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B117">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rentfrow</surname>
<given-names>P. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gosling</surname>
<given-names>S. D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>The do re mi's of everyday life: the structure and personality correlates of music preferences</article-title>
.
<source>J. Pers. Soc. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>84</volume>
,
<fpage>1236</fpage>
<lpage>1256</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/0022-3514.84.6.1236</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12793587</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B118">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Riemann</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1888</year>
).
<source>Katechismus der Musik</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Leipzig</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Breitkopf and Hertel</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B119">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rigg</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1940</year>
).
<article-title>Speed as a determiner of musical mood</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>27</volume>
,
<fpage>566</fpage>
<lpage>571</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/h0058652</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18509502</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B120">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Russell</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1941</year>
).
<source>An Inquiry into Meaning and Truth</source>
.
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>George Allen and Unwin</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B121">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ruytjens</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Georgiadis</surname>
<given-names>J. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Holstege</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wit</surname>
<given-names>H. P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Albers</surname>
<given-names>W. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Willemsen</surname>
<given-names>A. T. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Functional sex differences in human primary auditory cortex</article-title>
.
<source>Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging</source>
<volume>34</volume>
,
<fpage>2073</fpage>
<lpage>2081</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s00259-007-0517-z</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17703299</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B122">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rycenga</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Lesbian compositional process</article-title>
, in
<source>Queering the Pitch</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Brett</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wood</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thomas</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Abingdon</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Taylor Francis</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>275</fpage>
<lpage>296</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B124">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schirmer</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kotz</surname>
<given-names>S. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Friederici</surname>
<given-names>A. D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Sex differentiates the role of emotional prosody during word processing</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Brain Res</source>
.
<volume>14</volume>
,
<fpage>228</fpage>
<lpage>233</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00108-8</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12067695</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B126">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Seeger</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1977</year>
).
<article-title>The musicological juncture: 1976</article-title>
.
<source>Ethnomusicolocy</source>
<volume>21</volume>
,
<fpage>170</fpage>
<lpage>188</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/850942</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B127">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sergeant</surname>
<given-names>D. C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Vraka</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2014</year>
).
<article-title>Pitch perception and absolute pitch</article-title>
, in
<source>Advanced Musical Performance in Higher Education Learning. SEMPRE Studies in the Psychology of Music</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Papageorgi</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Welch</surname>
<given-names>G. F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Aldershot</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Ashgate Press</publisher-name>
). ISBN 978-1-4094-3689-8.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B128">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sergeant</surname>
<given-names>D. C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Welch</surname>
<given-names>G. F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Gender differences in long-term average spectra of children's singing voices</article-title>
.
<source>J. Voice</source>
<volume>23</volume>
,
<fpage>319</fpage>
<lpage>336</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.jvoice.2007.10.010</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18468845</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B129">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sheldon</surname>
<given-names>D. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gregory</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1997</year>
).
<article-title>Perception of tempo modulation by listeners of different educational experience</article-title>
.
<source>J. Res. Music Educ</source>
.
<volume>45</volume>
,
<fpage>367</fpage>
<lpage>379</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2307/3345532</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B130">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Shepherd</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1991</year>
).
<source>Music as Social Text</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Polity Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B131">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sloboda</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1986</year>
).
<source>The Musical Mind: The Cognitive Psychology of Music</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Clarendon Press</publisher-name>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198521280.001.0001</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B133">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Standke</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1992</year>
).
<source>Methods of Digital Speech Analysis in Research on Vocal Communication</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Frankfurt</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Peter Lang</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B134">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Strunk</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1950</year>
).
<source>Source Readings in Music History</source>
. N.Y. Norton, Inc. (citing Leopold Mozart:
<italic>Versuch einer gründlichen Violinschule)</italic>
(
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
).</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B135">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sundberg</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1993</year>
).
<article-title>How can music be expressive</article-title>
.
<source>Speech Commun</source>
.
<volume>13</volume>
,
<fpage>239</fpage>
<lpage>253</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0167-6393(93)90075-V</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B137">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Taruskin</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1997</year>
).
<source>Defining Russia Musically: Historical and Hermeneutical Essays</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Princeton, NJ</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Princeton University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B138">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Taylor</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Taking it in the ear: on musico-sexual synergies and the (queer) possibility that music is sex</article-title>
.
<source>Continuum</source>
<volume>26</volume>
,
<fpage>603</fpage>
<lpage>614</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/10304312.2012.698039</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B139">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tekman</surname>
<given-names>H. G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Music preferences as signs of who we are: personality and social factors</article-title>
,
<source>Proceedings of 7th Trienniel Conf. ESCOM</source>
(
<publisher-loc>Jyvaskyla</publisher-loc>
),
<fpage>592</fpage>
<lpage>595</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B140">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tervaniemi</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Just</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Koelsch</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Widman</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schroger</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Pitch discrimination accuracy in musicians and nonmusicians: an event-related potential and behavioural study</article-title>
.
<source>Exp. Brain Res</source>
.
<volume>161</volume>
,
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>10</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s00221-004-2044-5</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15551089</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B141">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<collab>Red Catalogue.</collab>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<source>The RED Classical Catalogue, London, RED Published in association with Gramophone</source>
.
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Red Publishing Co. Uk. N1</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B142">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Thompson</surname>
<given-names>W. F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Balkwill</surname>
<given-names>L.-L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Decoding speech prosody in five languages</article-title>
.
<source>Semiotica</source>
<volume>178</volume>
,
<fpage>407</fpage>
<lpage>424</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1515/SEM.2006.017</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B143">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Thompson</surname>
<given-names>W. F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Robitalle</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1992</year>
).
<article-title>Can composers express emotion through music?</article-title>
<source>Empir. Stud. Arts</source>
<volume>10</volume>
,
<fpage>79</fpage>
<lpage>89</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.2190/NBNY-AKDK-GW58-MTEL</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B144">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Thompson</surname>
<given-names>W. F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schellenberg</surname>
<given-names>E. G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Glenn</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Husain</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Decoding speech prosody: Do music lessons help?</article-title>
<source>Emotion</source>
<volume>4</volume>
,
<fpage>46</fpage>
<lpage>64</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1037/1528-3542.4.1.46</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15053726</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B145">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Thompson</surname>
<given-names>W. F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schellenberg</surname>
<given-names>E. G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Husain</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Perceiving prosody in speech</article-title>
.
<source>Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci</source>
.
<volume>999</volume>
,
<fpage>530</fpage>
<lpage>532</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1196/annals.1284.067</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">14681180</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B146">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Thorpe</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ockelford</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Aksentrijevic</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>An empirical exploration of the zygonic model of expectation in music</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>40</volume>
,
<fpage>428</fpage>
<lpage>470</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735610392103</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B147">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Treitler</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<source>Reflections on Musical Meaning and Its Representations</source>
.
<publisher-name>Indiana University Press</publisher-name>
,
<publisher-loc>Bloomington, IN</publisher-loc>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B148">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Trevarthen</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Delafield</surname>
<given-names>R. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Butt</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schogler</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Psychobiology of music gesture: innate rhythm, harmony and melody in movements of narration</article-title>
, in
<source>New Perspect. Music Gesture</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Gritten</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>King</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Farnham</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Ashgate</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>11</fpage>
<lpage>44</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B149">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Trimmer</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fischer</surname>
<given-names>A. H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Manstead</surname>
<given-names>A. S. R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1998</year>
).
<article-title>Gender differences in motives for regulating emotions</article-title>
.
<source>Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull</source>
.
<volume>24</volume>
,
<fpage>974</fpage>
<lpage>985</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0146167298249005</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21534673</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B150">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Webster</surname>
<given-names>G. D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Weir</surname>
<given-names>C. G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Emotional responses to music: interactive effects of mode, texture and tempo</article-title>
.
<source>Motiv. Emot</source>
.
<volume>29</volume>
,
<fpage>19</fpage>
<lpage>39</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s11031-005-4414-0</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B151">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wheeler</surname>
<given-names>B. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1985</year>
).
<article-title>Relationships of personality characteristics to mood and enjoyment after hearing live and recorded music andto musical taste</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Music</source>
<volume>13</volume>
,
<fpage>81</fpage>
<lpage>92</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735685132002</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B154">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wurm</surname>
<given-names>L. H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Vakoch</surname>
<given-names>D. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Strasser</surname>
<given-names>M. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Calm-Jagerlman</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Shannon</surname>
<given-names>E. R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Speech perception and vocal expression of emotion</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Emotion</source>
<volume>15</volume>
,
<fpage>831</fpage>
<lpage>852</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/02699930143000086</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B152">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Yamasaki</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Emotional communication in improvised performance by musically untrained players</article-title>
, in
<source>Proceedings of the 17th International Congress of the International Association of Empirical Aesthetics</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Kato</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Osaka</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>IAEA</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>521</fpage>
<lpage>524</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B153">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Zweigenghaft</surname>
<given-names>R. L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>A do re mi encore: a closer look at the personality correlates of music preferences</article-title>
.
<source>J. Individ. Diff</source>
.
<volume>29</volume>
,
<fpage>45</fpage>
<lpage>55</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1027/1614-0001.29.1.45</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</pmc>
<affiliations>
<list></list>
<tree>
<noCountry>
<name sortKey="Himonides, Evangelos" sort="Himonides, Evangelos" uniqKey="Himonides E" first="Evangelos" last="Himonides">Evangelos Himonides</name>
<name sortKey="Sergeant, Desmond C" sort="Sergeant, Desmond C" uniqKey="Sergeant D" first="Desmond C." last="Sergeant">Desmond C. Sergeant</name>
</noCountry>
</tree>
</affiliations>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Wicri/Musique/explor/MozartV1/Data/Pmc/Checkpoint
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 000130 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Checkpoint/biblio.hfd -nk 000130 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Wicri/Musique
   |area=    MozartV1
   |flux=    Pmc
   |étape=   Checkpoint
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     PMC:3997045
   |texte=   Gender and the performance of music
}}

Pour générer des pages wiki

HfdIndexSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Checkpoint/RBID.i   -Sk "pubmed:24795663" \
       | HfdSelect -Kh $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Checkpoint/biblio.hfd   \
       | NlmPubMed2Wicri -a MozartV1 

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.20.
Data generation: Sun Apr 10 15:06:14 2016. Site generation: Tue Feb 7 15:40:35 2023