Serveur d'exploration Debussy

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

Mechanisms of Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings: A Study of Perception on Sequenza VI for Viola Solo by Luciano Berio

Identifieur interne : 002758 ( Istex/Corpus ); précédent : 002757; suivant : 002759

Mechanisms of Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings: A Study of Perception on Sequenza VI for Viola Solo by Luciano Berio

Auteurs : Irene Deliege ; Abdessadek El Ahnmadi

Source :

RBID : ISTEX:6510B7F256B8D453C34D907F0678D969B2490CA4

English descriptors

Abstract

Eighteen musicians and 18 non-musicians were asked to indicate perceived segmentations in a performance of Berio's Sequenza VI. Segmentations were marked during active listening by depressing a key oil a compLitCie console. The segmentations of both musicians and non-musicians were i general agreement with a reference analysis pi-ovided bv two compposers. These segmentations had a high degree of stability over two 1istenings. J'he results are discussed in terms of a theory of cue extraction allowing identification of the beginning and end of major compositioned units

Url:
DOI: 10.1177/0305735690181003

Links to Exploration step

ISTEX:6510B7F256B8D453C34D907F0678D969B2490CA4

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI wicri:istexFullTextTei="biblStruct">
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">Mechanisms of Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings: A Study of Perception on Sequenza VI for Viola Solo by Luciano Berio</title>
<author wicri:is="90%">
<name sortKey="Deliege, Irene" sort="Deliege, Irene" uniqKey="Deliege I" first="Irene" last="Deliege">Irene Deliege</name>
</author>
<author wicri:is="90%">
<name sortKey="El Ahnmadi, Abdessadek" sort="El Ahnmadi, Abdessadek" uniqKey="El Ahnmadi A" first="Abdessadek" last="El Ahnmadi">Abdessadek El Ahnmadi</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>Unite de Recherche en Psychologie (tie Ia Miqsltue, Laboratoire de Psychologie Experinientale. Universite de Liege, Sart Tilman, B132 4000 Liege, Belgium</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">ISTEX</idno>
<idno type="RBID">ISTEX:6510B7F256B8D453C34D907F0678D969B2490CA4</idno>
<date when="1990" year="1990">1990</date>
<idno type="doi">10.1177/0305735690181003</idno>
<idno type="url">https://api.istex.fr/document/6510B7F256B8D453C34D907F0678D969B2490CA4/fulltext/pdf</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Istex/Corpus">002758</idno>
<idno type="wicri:explorRef" wicri:stream="Istex" wicri:step="Corpus" wicri:corpus="ISTEX">002758</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">Mechanisms of Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings: A Study of Perception on Sequenza VI for Viola Solo by Luciano Berio</title>
<author wicri:is="90%">
<name sortKey="Deliege, Irene" sort="Deliege, Irene" uniqKey="Deliege I" first="Irene" last="Deliege">Irene Deliege</name>
</author>
<author wicri:is="90%">
<name sortKey="El Ahnmadi, Abdessadek" sort="El Ahnmadi, Abdessadek" uniqKey="El Ahnmadi A" first="Abdessadek" last="El Ahnmadi">Abdessadek El Ahnmadi</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>Unite de Recherche en Psychologie (tie Ia Miqsltue, Laboratoire de Psychologie Experinientale. Universite de Liege, Sart Tilman, B132 4000 Liege, Belgium</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<monogr></monogr>
<series>
<title level="j">Psychology of music</title>
<idno type="ISSN">0305-7356</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1741-3087</idno>
<imprint>
<publisher>Sage Publications</publisher>
<pubPlace>Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</pubPlace>
<date type="published" when="1990-04">1990-04</date>
<biblScope unit="volume">18</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="issue">1</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" from="18">18</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" to="44">44</biblScope>
</imprint>
<idno type="ISSN">0305-7356</idno>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
<seriesStmt>
<idno type="ISSN">0305-7356</idno>
</seriesStmt>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass>
<keywords scheme="Teeft" xml:lang="en">
<term>Science collections</term>
</keywords>
</textClass>
<langUsage>
<language ident="en">en</language>
</langUsage>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">Eighteen musicians and 18 non-musicians were asked to indicate perceived segmentations in a performance of Berio's Sequenza VI. Segmentations were marked during active listening by depressing a key oil a compLitCie console. The segmentations of both musicians and non-musicians were i general agreement with a reference analysis pi-ovided bv two compposers. These segmentations had a high degree of stability over two 1istenings. J'he results are discussed in terms of a theory of cue extraction allowing identification of the beginning and end of major compositioned units</div>
</front>
</TEI>
<istex>
<corpusName>sage</corpusName>
<keywords>
<teeft>
<json:string>science collections</json:string>
</teeft>
</keywords>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>Irene Deliege</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>Abdessadek El Ahnmadi</name>
<affiliations>
<json:string>Unite de Recherche en Psychologie (tie Ia Miqsltue, Laboratoire de Psychologie Experinientale. Universite de Liege, Sart Tilman, B132 4000 Liege, Belgium</json:string>
</affiliations>
</json:item>
</author>
<articleId>
<json:string>10.1177_0305735690181003</json:string>
</articleId>
<arkIstex>ark:/67375/M70-MWBZPQRV-V</arkIstex>
<language>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</language>
<originalGenre>
<json:string>research-article</json:string>
</originalGenre>
<abstract>Eighteen musicians and 18 non-musicians were asked to indicate perceived segmentations in a performance of Berio's Sequenza VI. Segmentations were marked during active listening by depressing a key oil a compLitCie console. The segmentations of both musicians and non-musicians were i general agreement with a reference analysis pi-ovided bv two compposers. These segmentations had a high degree of stability over two 1istenings. J'he results are discussed in terms of a theory of cue extraction allowing identification of the beginning and end of major compositioned units</abstract>
<qualityIndicators>
<score>3.029</score>
<pdfWordCount>9</pdfWordCount>
<pdfCharCount>54</pdfCharCount>
<pdfVersion>1.3</pdfVersion>
<pdfPageCount>27</pdfPageCount>
<pdfPageSize>411 x 651.96 pts</pdfPageSize>
<refBibsNative>true</refBibsNative>
<abstractWordCount>85</abstractWordCount>
<abstractCharCount>573</abstractCharCount>
<keywordCount>0</keywordCount>
</qualityIndicators>
<title>Mechanisms of Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings: A Study of Perception on Sequenza VI for Viola Solo by Luciano Berio</title>
<genre>
<json:string>research-article</json:string>
</genre>
<host>
<title>Psychology of music</title>
<language>
<json:string>unknown</json:string>
</language>
<issn>
<json:string>0305-7356</json:string>
</issn>
<eissn>
<json:string>1741-3087</json:string>
</eissn>
<publisherId>
<json:string>POM</json:string>
</publisherId>
<volume>18</volume>
<issue>1</issue>
<pages>
<first>18</first>
<last>44</last>
</pages>
<genre>
<json:string>journal</json:string>
</genre>
</host>
<namedEntities>
<unitex>
<date>
<json:string>1990</json:string>
</date>
<geogName></geogName>
<orgName></orgName>
<orgName_funder></orgName_funder>
<orgName_provider></orgName_provider>
<persName></persName>
<placeName></placeName>
<ref_url></ref_url>
<ref_bibl></ref_bibl>
<bibl></bibl>
</unitex>
</namedEntities>
<ark>
<json:string>ark:/67375/M70-MWBZPQRV-V</json:string>
</ark>
<categories>
<wos>
<json:string>1 - social science</json:string>
<json:string>2 - psychology, experimental</json:string>
<json:string>2 - psychology, educational</json:string>
<json:string>2 - psychology, applied</json:string>
</wos>
<scienceMetrix>
<json:string>1 - health sciences</json:string>
<json:string>2 - psychology & cognitive sciences</json:string>
<json:string>3 - experimental psychology</json:string>
</scienceMetrix>
<scopus>
<json:string>1 - Social Sciences</json:string>
<json:string>2 - Psychology</json:string>
<json:string>3 - Psychology (miscellaneous)</json:string>
<json:string>1 - Social Sciences</json:string>
<json:string>2 - Arts and Humanities</json:string>
<json:string>3 - Music</json:string>
</scopus>
<inist>
<json:string>1 - sciences humaines et sociales</json:string>
</inist>
</categories>
<publicationDate>1990</publicationDate>
<copyrightDate>1990</copyrightDate>
<doi>
<json:string>10.1177/0305735690181003</json:string>
</doi>
<id>6510B7F256B8D453C34D907F0678D969B2490CA4</id>
<score>1</score>
<fulltext>
<json:item>
<extension>pdf</extension>
<original>true</original>
<mimetype>application/pdf</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/6510B7F256B8D453C34D907F0678D969B2490CA4/fulltext/pdf</uri>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<extension>ocr</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>text/ocr</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/6510B7F256B8D453C34D907F0678D969B2490CA4/fulltext/ocr</uri>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<extension>zip</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>application/zip</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/6510B7F256B8D453C34D907F0678D969B2490CA4/fulltext/zip</uri>
</json:item>
<istex:fulltextTEI uri="https://api.istex.fr/document/6510B7F256B8D453C34D907F0678D969B2490CA4/fulltext/tei">
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">Mechanisms of Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings: A Study of Perception on Sequenza VI for Viola Solo by Luciano Berio</title>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<authority>ISTEX</authority>
<publisher scheme="https://publisher-list.data.istex.fr">Sage Publications</publisher>
<pubPlace>Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</pubPlace>
<availability>
<licence>
<p>sage</p>
</licence>
</availability>
<p scheme="https://loaded-corpus.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/XBH-0J1N7DQT-B"></p>
<date>1990</date>
</publicationStmt>
<notesStmt>
<note type="research-article" scheme="https://content-type.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/XTP-1JC4F85T-7">research-article</note>
<note type="journal" scheme="https://publication-type.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/JMC-0GLKJH51-B">journal</note>
</notesStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct type="inbook">
<analytic>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">Mechanisms of Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings: A Study of Perception on Sequenza VI for Viola Solo by Luciano Berio</title>
<author xml:id="author-0000">
<persName>
<forename type="first">Irene</forename>
<surname>Deliege</surname>
</persName>
</author>
<author xml:id="author-0001">
<persName>
<forename type="first">Abdessadek</forename>
<surname>El Ahnmadi</surname>
</persName>
<affiliation>Unite de Recherche en Psychologie (tie Ia Miqsltue, Laboratoire de Psychologie Experinientale. Universite de Liege, Sart Tilman, B132 4000 Liege, Belgium</affiliation>
</author>
<idno type="istex">6510B7F256B8D453C34D907F0678D969B2490CA4</idno>
<idno type="ark">ark:/67375/M70-MWBZPQRV-V</idno>
<idno type="DOI">10.1177/0305735690181003</idno>
<idno type="article-id">10.1177_0305735690181003</idno>
</analytic>
<monogr>
<title level="j">Psychology of music</title>
<idno type="pISSN">0305-7356</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1741-3087</idno>
<idno type="publisher-id">POM</idno>
<idno type="PublisherID-hwp">sppom</idno>
<imprint>
<publisher>Sage Publications</publisher>
<pubPlace>Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</pubPlace>
<date type="published" when="1990-04"></date>
<biblScope unit="volume">18</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="issue">1</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" from="18">18</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" to="44">44</biblScope>
</imprint>
</monogr>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<creation>
<date>1990</date>
</creation>
<langUsage>
<language ident="en">en</language>
</langUsage>
<abstract xml:lang="en">
<p>Eighteen musicians and 18 non-musicians were asked to indicate perceived segmentations in a performance of Berio's Sequenza VI. Segmentations were marked during active listening by depressing a key oil a compLitCie console. The segmentations of both musicians and non-musicians were i general agreement with a reference analysis pi-ovided bv two compposers. These segmentations had a high degree of stability over two 1istenings. J'he results are discussed in terms of a theory of cue extraction allowing identification of the beginning and end of major compositioned units</p>
</abstract>
</profileDesc>
<revisionDesc>
<change when="1990-04">Published</change>
</revisionDesc>
</teiHeader>
</istex:fulltextTEI>
<json:item>
<extension>txt</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>text/plain</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/6510B7F256B8D453C34D907F0678D969B2490CA4/fulltext/txt</uri>
</json:item>
</fulltext>
<metadata>
<istex:metadataXml wicri:clean="corpus sage not found" wicri:toSee="no header">
<istex:xmlDeclaration>version="1.0" encoding="UTF-8"</istex:xmlDeclaration>
<istex:docType PUBLIC="-//NLM//DTD Journal Publishing DTD v2.3 20070202//EN" URI="journalpublishing.dtd" name="istex:docType"></istex:docType>
<istex:document>
<article article-type="research-article" dtd-version="2.3" xml:lang="EN">
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="hwp">sppom</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="publisher-id">POM</journal-id>
<journal-title>Psychology of Music</journal-title>
<issn pub-type="ppub">0305-7356</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Sage Publications</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</publisher-loc>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735690181003</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="publisher-id">10.1177_0305735690181003</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Articles</subject>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>Mechanisms of Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings: A Study of Perception on Sequenza VI for Viola Solo by Luciano Berio</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Deliege</surname>
<given-names>Irene</given-names>
</name>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>El Ahnmadi</surname>
<given-names>Abdessadek</given-names>
</name>
<aff>Unite de Recherche en Psychologie (tie Ia Miqsltue, Laboratoire de Psychologie Experinientale. Universite de Liege, Sart Tilman, B132 4000 Liege, Belgium</aff>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<pub-date pub-type="ppub">
<month>04</month>
<year>1990</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>18</volume>
<issue>1</issue>
<fpage>18</fpage>
<lpage>44</lpage>
<abstract>
<p>Eighteen musicians and 18 non-musicians were asked to indicate perceived segmentations in a performance of Berio's
<italic>Sequenza VI.</italic>
Segmentations were marked during active listening by depressing a key oil a compLitCie console. The segmentations of both musicians and non-musicians were i general agreement with a reference analysis pi-ovided bv two compposers. These segmentations had a high degree of stability over two 1istenings. J'he results are discussed in terms of a theory of cue extraction allowing identification of the beginning and end of major compositioned units</p>
</abstract>
<custom-meta-wrap>
<custom-meta xlink:type="simple">
<meta-name>sagemeta-type</meta-name>
<meta-value>Journal Article</meta-value>
</custom-meta>
<custom-meta xlink:type="simple">
<meta-name>search-text</meta-name>
<meta-value> R.vycholo,gy of Music, & 199() 1 the Suoc IOr o Rcs. lchr?! 1990( 18, 18-44 .s s'(zology (l' .Wuics t2tis d V1losic I f l11ch I J (1 Mechanisms of Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings: A Study of Perception on Sequenza VI for Viola Solo by Luciano Berio IRENE DELIEGE and ABDESSADEK Ef AHN/AI)I L nihW de Recherche en Psychologie (tie Ia Miqsltue, Laboralolrc +c Psvchologie Experinientale. Univ,ersit e 1(it LieJ Sart Tilman, B132 4000 Liege, Be! giufi; Eighteen musicians and 18 non-musicians were asked to indicate perceived segmentations in a performance of Berio's Sequenza VI. Segmentations were marked during active listening by depressing a key oil a compLitCie console. The segmentations of both musicians and non-musicians were i general agreement with a reference analysis pi-ovided bv two compposers. These segmentations had a high degree of stability over two 1istenings. J'he results are discussed in terms of a theory of cue extractioi lallo)w[in identification of the beginning and end of major compositio:ncld ulnitsh Introduction Preliminary Remarks Some psychologists consider listening to music a creative act. It could evein be considered a "composition" in so far as the listener injects elemenits into it that derive from his or her personal preferences and imagination. In this sense, listening-or apprehending any work of art-could never have tsxwo identical versions, since the contributions of the subject are essentiallv variable and related to his condition and state of mind at the precise moment that he is in contact with the work. It is not the "icreative' listening that takes its departure from the work that is considered here, but rather listening that presumes a deliberate predisposition towards its reconstitution: one which seeks to encounter the composer's project and coincide with his orientations Can this listening, by which the listener tends towards maximum coincidence with the work's structures, be used to reconstitute and analyse them'? ilt seems that we are not generally capable of consciously attending to every aspect of the auditory input (or indeed of other sensory inputs)" (Moore 1982, p. 202); except for cases of exceptional memory (Sloboda. Hermelil O'Connor, 1985), no one will be surprised if aural analysis is considered a simplification, or even an impoverishment, in comparison with the objective content of the material to be perceived. Aural analysis-the discovery of points of perceptive condensation of the macro-structure, according to Imberty (1985)-is in continuous formation up until the moment at which the temporal organisation of the musical object becomes clear to the listener. Does this mean that at the end of listening, the listener will have understood, without other recourse, what particular type ol form is concerned? Recognising the formal characteristics of a work on 18 Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings listening to it is typically an acquired skill: such competence is inconceivable without the help of previous information and is clearly related to learned listening techniques. This point will not be disputed here. On the other hand, we will be concerned with the development of the mental representation of the object, that is, those mechanisms involved in the perception of its overall architecture. In this sense, the demonstration will indirectly consider any music in terms of form and will be concerned with what Apel (1972, p. 326-27) calls "form in music", i.e. the establishment of a network of relations according to internal principles and rules of organisation, as opposed to "form of music" which is concerned with pre-established designs on which the composition is based. The Cue Extraction Mechanism Up to now, in the approach to the formation of groups on a surface level, the impact of acoustic or temporal differences has been emphasised (I. Deliege, 1987 b): parts of the musical discourse are marked off by contrast- moments that signal and impose the segmentations (Fraisse, 1956, p. 11; 1982). The stage of the forming of surface groups provide the material for immediate retention, but cannot, alone, lead to analysis. Broadening the groups or the constitution of regroupings of groups is put in place on a wider and wider basis until the entire work is included: "A piece constitutes a group" (Lerdahl and Jackendoff, 1983, p. 38). Access to this hierarchic process of grouping presupposes the presence of elements that can be used to demarcate longer passages and to facilitate their recognition. In a previous study (I. Deliege, 1987a), the existence of a cue extraction mechanism created through attentive listening was postulated; the cues would be used as labels for the retention of groups, just like a library file (Lieury, 1980, p. 16). At the time of retrieval, it is the cue passage alone that gives access to the whole group. Its "signal" function is a signpost of recognition. Since it orients attention in the progressive reconstitution of the plan of the work, the degree of pertinence and persistence of the cues.provided by superficial characteristics is important (Gibson, 1982). They permit active identification of structures temporarily stored in memory. The subject listens and psychologically brings back to the present (Fraisse, 1974, p. 75) structures that have already disappeared. Information is never preserved as an absolute entity, but in relative terms, ready to be "rediscussed" and re-evaluated in function of more recent entries (Palmer, 1982). The structures are constantly recuperated and reactualised, thus establishing new relations. In some cases, the subject can even develop connections between what he perceives and the fruit of his learning and previous experience. One may wonder what constitutes a cue and its degree of pertinence. Specific studies on this point have yet to be carried out. But on a first approach, one can say it consists of a rather brief marker whose impact is very clear in sound and rhythm, sufficiently significant to be able to automatically characterise a whole sequence. However, an intuitive aspect persists in such a definition. In the same way, it is impossible to specify the conditions for a final order of pertinence of cues, since the number of combinations of sounds is infinite. 19 Irene Deliege and Abdessadek El Ahmadi Is the organisation of the cue selection process conceivable in the course of listening? The abbreviations of groups allow for efficient storage but their accumulation in memory can soon result in overloading. We must understand here that this organisation of the cues is not limited to the formationl of surface groups, but on the contrary is part of all stages of grouping. As broader and broader regroupings of groups are formed, a dynamic evaluatiol of the cues is created. The more the listener circumscribes large portions ot the music, the more the selection of cues extracted on first hearing becomes functional. The formation of groupings of groups at different hierarchic levels is not stored on the basis of adding together the entrance labels, but by the elimination, in order of pertinence, of those traits that were not strong enough to be retained, replaced by reinforcement of the most significant markers which emerge and signal the stages of the piece A mental representation of the form gradually develops from this point on. Attentive listening generates a sort of "same-different" test which not only allows one to assign a trait to a given group, but this or that group to the "paragraph" of the work that is most appropriate because of ties of similaritv shared with the other structures. So, on the sole basis of the organisation of groupings, a mental image of the form can be conceived: the interplay of the relations between cues rendering the main articulations perceptible (cf. Fig. 1). In sum, two organisation principles seem to function in the analytical hearing of musical form: the principle of sameness which constitutes groups and groupings of groups; the principle of difference which differentiates them. The Cue and the Music-Listener Interaction It is likely that a more or less effective interaction between music and listener will be established as a function of the way the cues are distributed in the structures. A combination of psycho-affective requirements are present in the listener and they must be met if he is to adhere to the object. That is where the cues play an important role. In addition to the relationships and ties of similarity they inscribe in the musical development, the cues at the same time generate memories: they create an atmosphere of familiarity, reassuring in particular for the listener with no previous training. The total environment of an individual, ranging from his personal development to his interaction with his social milieu, is punctuated to a considerable extent by imitation and repetition. Could music depart from the general rule of "universal repetition' without prejudice to the contact that must be made with the listener? The cognitive stages of auditory analysis are worked through in active listening thanks to a certain voluntary effort. Without a "reward" in return, that is the feeling of mastering the message, it is unlikely that the listener will keep trying. On the contrary, he is likely to withdraw from an unfruitful attempt and gradually fall back into a pre-attentive stage. Is there, then, a theory of acceptable limits which should, in the case of a learned musical work, be the composer's first preoccupation? Each work is of course a specific case. Once the impact of cues has been accepted, however, it is normal that the ties of similarity they develop in the course of the work be the source of constructive contact between music and listener and a key for 20 Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings t p INFORMATION 0 A S v h s RECEPTION e I a V r E rnaconitic n A C T V E t 0 S s t e n | MEMORY | AUDITIVE ANALYSIS FIG. 1 Schematic diagram of the musical information. the intelligibility of the general outline of the work. The use of repetition would thus not be proof of lack on inventiveness, but a source of coherence: a certain balance between recognised elements and unknown ones opens on to a temporal horizon which is suitably divided into zones of expectation and of relaxation. There is good access to the organisation of the work when this condition is respected: it allows for satisfaction and attentiveness on the part 21 Irene Deliege and Abdessadek El Ahmadi of the listener. However, when the music is overburdened with repetitive structures, as in minimalist music, time becomes static. The composer's use ot the cue makes it too direct: that's why this kind of music has such a success. Nothing but a minimum of expectation is demanded to assimilate the progressive transformations inherent to the context. Buit whatevei thW importance of those transformations, their incidence never will bc comiparable to the weight of repetitions. Inevitably, when no effort is required. attention progressively relaxes and listening slides into the most complete passivitv. Inl contrast, when recognition of cues is made difficult by a too rapid accumulation of information as well as by too few periodic structures, perpetually mobili time offers no signposts nor rest, and the listening process gradually stifies because of the impossibility of handling too much information within thc time frame of the audition. A work deliberately avoiding any fornm ol repetition would, despite its opposite position, be as devoid of factoi-s creating a sense of expectation as one in which reiteration is overuised (Whittall, 1980, p. 710). The Experiment Methodological Position The pyschology of music has up to now barely considered investigating how form in music is perceived. However, this now seems to be an urgent task it our discipline hopes to reach new perspectives through cognitive processes. In this way we could provide useful indications to composers and pedagogues alike. As in all research concerning musical perception, such a project etntaiks specific methodological difficulties concerning the experimental material, which probably justify to a certain extent the reticence of psychologists. 11e usual practice, in our field, as in any scientific discipline, is to isolate thc variable that one wishes to study and to incorporate it in a series of brief and repetitive sound sequences (that are called musical), constructed bv the psychologist for the needs of the experiment, in order to be able to identify it afterward, in appropriate manner, in the statistical analysis of the data. Is such an approach in an adequate conformity with its purpose'? Can Wc seriously imagine the possibility of its topic? Who is, actually, able to asseit he can produce an isolated musical parameter, for example: a pitch without duration, dynamic or timbre and even articulation? Of course, preference can be given to a determinate parameter rather than to another, but in all cases the meaning is context dependent. Unfortunately, many studies in the field of psychology of music scarcely achieve their aims because a musical objective is being sought through the use of material that is both too simple and too trivial. This problem has been considered elsewhere (I. Deliege, 1985). It is mentioned here in order to emphasise its importance and especially because study of the perception of a musical form is an area that justifies one's daring to inaugurate new experimental procedures. On what basis can we undertake this observation, if it is not through judicious questioning of subjects with respect to authentic musical objects and judicious analysis of their answers in relation to the musical structure? Such a procedure is doubly contrary to usual practice: first because the proportions of a musical work are longer than the 22 Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings usual experimental stimuli, and second because real musical material, in its essence, cannot offer dimensions that are in all points controllable and systematically repeatable. Confronting this problem, it has been suggested (I. Deliege, ibid.) to run the experiment on a mixed basis, with a two-part study, one using musical extracts from the repertory, the other with experimental stimuli. Some qualifications must be made, however. Experience shows that the experimen- tal part only makes sense if it is preceded by work in the sphere of music. This observation leads to useful and musically pertinent questions which can then be circumscribed by experiments of a more classical conception if necessary, that is when a reason has made itself clear and if (and only if) the means exist to contain the question within adequate and correct stimuli. As far as investigating the perception of musical form is concerned, until proved to the contrary, it seems to be difficult for a psychologist to "ape" the composer by writing little experimental pieces. Suggesting such a mixed procedure is not a proposal of principle to be applied without discernment to any research project in the psychology of music. Only the type of question posed can justify its use. So in this case the experiment has been limited to the observation of subjects in confrontation with a real work of music. The Experimental Material In function of what has been said about the more or less valid music-listener interaction induced by a well-balanced exploitation of repetitive elements in a composition, it is essential, for an experiment concerning attentive listening, to choose works that meet this requirement. Any other choice would be unfortunate since the attention mechanisms might be less functional if they were solicited too much or too little: The repertoire of works conforming to this criterion is of course extensive, but it seemed desirable to start off with a contemporary work. This is a deliberate choice: we wished to observe the behaviour of non-musician subjects in an unfamiliar context in order to distinguish aspects of perception that are as free as possible from cultural imprints or any other references, and then to compare these results with those obtained with musicians highly accustomed to this kind of music. This repertoire, much frequented by some and virtually unknown to others, offers a good framework for testing the hypothesis. We chose a work by Luciano Berio: Sequenza VI for viola, in the interpretation by Walter Trampler (RCA SB 6846, 1971). The solo instru- ment avoids the problem of plunging the inexperienced subject into a too complex sea of sound. In addition, throughout his evolution, Berio has given invariance an important place in his writing: the effect of the relations of similarity induced in the segmentation by the grouping cues should be clearly evident in the results. Predictions Just as the formation of groups and groupings is a process that has been observed in subjects whatever their degree of acquaintance with music (I. Deliege, 1987 b), so the mechanism of cue extraction which is the result 23 Irene Deliege and Abdessadek El Ahmadi should be functional for both the non-musician and the musician in alctixe listening. However, the results of a previous experiment (I. Deliege, ibid.) showed the difficulty with which non-musicians mastered inequalities in the successiori of different lengths of sound. Lengthening of a sound or a rest weirc frequently felt as a group boundary. Non-musicians split more often the material, therefore their grouping time-spans are reduced in comparisonl witth those of the musicians. The same situation should be logically observed once again in the present work. As in any process of group formation, the demarcation on hearing of' sections of a work is a grouping of preferential ordering. It is not necessarily unique nor the same for everyone, and, a priori, any segmentation should be taken into consideration: an unusual preference is not a symptom of error. Therefore the analysis of the results will not be formulated in terms of correct or incorrect answers. We hypothesise that few or none of the subjects will segment as long as the flow of music retains the same cue. However, the number of segmentations made should increase in the sections where the composition is more characterised by contrasting structures. Internal pauses within the main sections can reveal, not errors, since we are considering preferential segmentation, but temporary uncertainties in the course of listening. In this sense there should not be many of them. It is even likely that some internal pauses which necessarily intervene in attentive listening would be corrected afterward. However, an experimental procedure allowing for such correction has not been envisaged here. All recorded segmentation is definite. Corrections can only be made after the audition, if the subject has memorised or taken note of the modifications he wishes to make, but there is no provision for changes in the course of listening. The Expected Auditory Analysis Two young composers-Claude Ledoux and Gerhard Sporken-were invited to provide a reference analysis to which the results of the two categories of subjects were compared. (a) Overall View. For both composers it is a piece that shows everything that can be done with the viola's chords, and which exploits the timbre of the instrument by using noise effects. They gave the same general division of the work in six main sections suggested by the maintenance of characteristic figures along with the statistical quality of musical events, i.e. the transformations- in the overall density of sound. It may be of interest, in this respect, to anecdotally look at the plan of the work as it was spontaneously sketched out by a non-musician subject. It coincides with this description on all points (Fig. 2), i.e. that sections 1 and 5. which insist on a given figure in a tense and powerful sound context, frame the areas 2 and 4 of average density; section 3 is characterised by single notes of weaker intensity. A coda ends the piece (section 6) in a more and more tenuous atmosphere. Passages between one section and the next are often described as zones of tiling-in the present case, of repetitions of configurations of a profile 24 Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings 1 st SECTION 2d SECTION 3rd SECTION 4th SECTION 5th SECTION 6th SECTION FIG. 2 Plan of the six sections perceived in Sequenza VI for viola solo by L. Berio, as proposed by a non-musician subject. The height and degree of shading of the sections correspond to the perceived volume of sound: sections 1 and 5, great intensity; sections 2 and 4, medium intensity; sections 3 and 6, feeble intensity. analogous to the one just completed-following a break which gives the temporary impression of a change of pace and puts off the real segmentation until the end of the section. (b) Description of the Sections. From the very beginning, the vibrato- tremolo on chords is strongly inscribed (example 1). After a break (0' 15") this loud tremolo remains characteristic, but the chords are generally "tossed out" by sounding isolated notes beforehand in a more or less arpeggio configuration. They create a sort of vision of the parts in their long march up the chromatically ascending steps (example 2) to a plateau "zone" (1' 48") which is maintained for 10" (example 3) before beginning, in a second section, a descent in a less intense climate. Ties with the first section are created by reminders of the vibrato-tremolo and the notes tossed out one by one before the chord (example 4). A break (3' 38") precedes a "tiling" zone which lasts 30" (4' 06"). Real contrast is generated by a more static and less intense atmosphere which is maintained for about two minutes (example 5). A rather long silence (6' 26") introduces a new "tiling" zone which leads up to an alternation of pizzicato, arco and col legno chords (6' 36") which characterise the next section (example 6), which comes to an abrupt conclusion (7' 32"). From there we go back to the atmosphere of the beginning of the piece (example 7). Kinds of glissandi (from 8' 26" to 8' 35") are perceived as a flectional zone leading to a coda which brings back the atmosphere and type of writing similar to the third section of the work (example 8). In short, this reference analysis gives a group of subdivisions (cf. Fig. 3) in which the composers have made a distinction between the limits of the ends of sections (dark lines) and segmentations introducing what they called "tiling" zones (light lines). These segmentations demonstrate the presence of cues as described above. Procedure Two categories of subjects aged 20 to 30 (mean: 26 years)-18 musicians and 18 non-musicians-took part in the experiment. The musicians are professionally involved in contemporary music as members and/or soloists of the "Ensemble Musique Nouvelle" in Liege. All of them were instrumentally 25 26 Irene Deliege and Abdessadek El Ahmadi _ ~ ~~~~c CA - C-~~~~~~~~~~~~~C E~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~ \A A .,B_....................................................................................... t O 6 Z C,. M. a u) Xs qj - VI) 0 r CL. .'s - 4 IN 0 ._ 0 C) C) 'IC -s LIn \C 00 0 C.) C) aDt 0 C1) C.) ~In . _- krJ 0 C) C1) "IC 36 C) ._ . o~ 00 c QOC 0 3) CO ~ S a 000 CO -::C 0 1 0, 3~ C) > a 0 1 8 00 -) O6 c 0:s ~ 0 0 D , ~0 CO = O tC) C) CO~ 3) 0 ._ *_ 0 04 CO 0- Hw Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings 1i all 'IC ON 00 Cl. 11~ cII o ON 11 UN C 1i) r 4 II 00 Cl 00 N 11 00 -,~ t N r - 00 r ON 0 Cl Z N C r Cl L) Cl4 O,N ~-- V) ON, ".O Z r)C rt) ki) N ~) ki) ,& 00 4 Ct-) Cl r C ONON ~~ kf) C '00 ONZ IT ~~ Z ~00 IO t . C 00ON b ~ ~i ~ A 4 00 00 - l rn- -4 r- (4)U 0. 0 ,4.) u +-O u~~~Id Id Z Q.Im. 0 0 In +- Cl 37 N 'I 11 CZl N fl~ 71 r- On 11 C) CN cr-- 11 6 Cl 1C Cl~ r ON 6 11 Z. lf II 00 r-~ oI Cl r4 - Cq 0 *_ V) _n C.) Qn 0 0 *_ Ct V) Irene Deliege and Abdessadek El Ahmadi (b) Grouping Time-Span Evolution (i) From the first to the second audition. The analysis shows mnuch clesicrl what was briefly seen above, i.e. that musicians are mnore stable in their grouping behaviour than non-musicians. Globally considered. the nlean, grouping time-span is scarcely modified in musicians, while some broadenino., is met in non-musicians at second audition. The high correlation coefficienlt (r = *991, n = 6, p < .001) between the musician's mean results shows that both their listenings are similar. On the contrary, the two non-musicians' auditions are less significantly correlated on this point of view (r = .817. 1i 6 p < 05): listening the piece a second time has led to Ca miiodified groupino strategy. It is of interest to compare Table II facing the curves of performanices ot the subjects for each section of the piece (cf. Fig. 7). The distance between the mean grouping time-spans in musicians and non-musicians is reduced tfot sections 1, 2, 4 and 5, at the second listening; moreover, the two curves are almost the same for section 4, where there is a near-equivalence betwecn strategies during the second listening. In contrast, it gets broader for sections 3 and 6. But, in the description of the sections sketched out above (ct. point 4b), a kind of relationship between the sound atmosphere and a mnore static writing was precisely pointed out for these two sections. 1-hus one might infei that solidarity and coherence between musical structures is apparently less spontaneously audible in non-musicians when the flow loses intensits. (ii) In relation to the real duration of the sections from the piece. Another way to insist once again about the grouping stability strategies by musicianis and, in contrast, on a fluctuation from this point of view, in non-musicianis, from the one to the other listening, lies in the correlation level existinlg between the real duration of each section of the piece and the groupilng duration perceived by the subjects for each of both auditions. Although not significant, a "high proximity" is observed (respectively, r 643. a 6, for the first audition, and r = *620, n - 6, for the second one) int musicians grouping behaviour. On the contrary, in non-musicians, there could be a training effect from the one to the other listening as the correlation levels show a considerable "progress" (respectively, r - 15, n - 6. for the first, and r = *512, n = 6, for the second). Furthermore, could the duration of the sections have an influence on the groupings perceived? Without going into details, one may notice that in analytic listening situation, groupings seem not to be easily broader than almost one minute, but this is no more than an hypothesis which needs furthet- investigations. (iii) In connection with the musical terms. Attention has been paid on the near-equivalence in segmentation behaviour by musicians whatever the listening. Nevertheless, some remarks in connection with particular sections of the piece may qualify the similarity noticed between the two listenings Indeed (cf. description, point 2, 4 b, above) the two first sections of the Sequenza exploit very near cues. Let us mention, in this respect, that already in the first hearing, the habituation with this kind of invariant was gixven a broadening of the grouping time-span in musicians: from about 50" for thie first section, their mean reaches 56" for the second one. ln addition, during 38 Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings the second hearing, their grouping time-span becomes stabilised at the level of 56" for these two sections where near cues are perceived. Some kind of musical structures seemed to have an increased pertinence as cue, evidence in this respect is given by the grouping time-spans observed for the fourth section of the piece where both categories of subjects show a similar behaviour in the second audition. For the sixth section, the sensation of the coda was felt, but materialised in the segmentations given in the second audition, by musicians. There is a broadening of the grouping time-span which reaches almost the duration of the section itself: musicians have thus noticed and memorised retrospectively the sense of this final section. In contrast-and this aspect has already been noticed above (see point 1)-non-musicians have felt a different influence: the numerous breaks introduced in the music at the end of the piece was the reason for more chunkings, and thus, a contraction of the grouping time- span, in the second audition. In sum, an influence of a more trained memory is observed here in musicians which allows them to master much consciously their perceptual analysis: as a result, the second audition takes advantage of it. Discussion Cues and Grouping Time-Span The two parts of the data analysis have let us from local aspects allowing to locate, in the score, particular musical structures having induced a more or less great convergence of segmentations, through more global reflections. At the end of the part concerning the time-span aspect between the different segmentations, one may wonder, perhaps, what the role of the cue is in the grouping time-span. Are these two aspects to be mixed up? Grouping time- span-it has been mentioned-has helped to grasp more exactly the contribu- tion of music education in listening behaviour and the fast progress in non- musicians' abilities. The mean grouping time-spans-it is true-are generally lower than those of the sections of the piece and, regarding the hypothesis, one believes perhaps that a correlation must be observed on this point. Nonetheless, this aspect should not obliterate the first part of the data analysis where the high convergence of segmentations observed at the end of the sections (see Fig. 3, p. 30) is the evidence of the "signal" effect carried out by the cues extraction. One may postulate on such a basis that there are certainly hierarchical differences between the segmentations perceived. Nothing, however, was foreseen to, give prominence to this question in the procedure of the present work. Conclusion and Perspectives This experiment has allowed us to follow the establishment of a system of relations between structures based on the evaluation of same and different as basic principles in the organisation of perception by subjects during attentive listening. One could, however, argue that each of the perceived section boundaries is in function of a rest, and nothing more. There is no point in discussing the presence nor the roles of interruptions in the perception of segmentations: in all groups formations-it is evident-there is a superimposed 39 Irene Deliege and Abdessadek El Ahmadi Is ! T tl- I t T I 3 TiL_ 11__ ~ 4.__ . . , "~~~~~ I;T. r 0 ~~~ vv t~ I T-' U~ r a sabelua;jad bu!dno J D s! Ce X l ~ ~ ~~~1 0 9 I--^ 9 I 'I T5 l I7 Tf--' K-?* . 0 t5 I C: m ~~~~TIT i . - 1! C - I- i I 0 I Tz 2 T ' ,It T'T . .* T * , # 1 F 1 I sabeiuaajad buidnoi9 I 2 ' E 40 C 9111 9 7- C0 I 0 Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings 6 u I --_ I i 1 _ C r. S I I I seu . . sabejuaa.iad buidnoi D -----------------------. 1 $ S. . . ,j I. I I l I s- ------------ . - sabeiuaojad buidnoi D I $ _> s I c i 9 r, .5 0. 9 0 41 . rC, Irene Deliege and Abdessadek El Ahmadi effect of interruption, be it subjective. However, we hacxe noticed that whcic more numerous segmentations appear, there exists some elemeint ot sound, other than a rest. Furthermore, rests alone cannot deteirmine thfe overall plaii of the work: the boundaries of the "ttiling" zones that both composers noted iln the passages between sections are proof of this. In fact, it is their funictionl sAs indicators of groupings that allows one to consider them as belongiing to tllc preceding section rather than the following one. Theii presence determiines relations of similarity and creates ties of relatedness. Thev organisc tlhc structures and by the same token grant the rests their true weight. In thrc, structuring of the plan of a work, the weight of a rest is not lust in function ol its length. To be convinced of this, one must only go back to examiple 7 where the interruption that precedes the "tiling" zone is longer thani the olle that introduces the fourth section. Three-eighth note rests (at 6' 26") are thus less weighty than two-sixteenth rests (at 6' 36"). Such a situation. apparentlx illogical, creates ambiguity and temporary suspension. First registered in terms of its real impact, i.e. of its objective length, the rest's importance is only stabilised retrospectively. Any rest followed by the continuation of the same cue could be reinterpreted as an internal break within the sectioni. On the contrary, the absence of the cue alerts one's attention toward a change. a turning point in the formal organisation. The more the contrast is clear cut. the more the break is perceived as important, whatever the real length of the rest might be. So a conflict may be introduced at this level which finds its solution in the similarity/contrast ratio between grouping cues. Witholut erasing the presence of rests, the cues determine their degree of importance. Thus they make it possible for the listener to grasp the formal organisation The research described here is the initial part of experimentation concerning the extraction mechanism of cues which facilitate groupings in music auial analysis. The investigation needs now to be pursued and extended to works of other composers and other periods in order to clarify as much as possible the functioning of the mechanism. In this case, the only aspect einvisaged, oni tl-it basis of the proposed hypothesis, is the perception of the divisionl of the work in its main groupings or sections. But the representation of musical form is not limited to this aspect alone. In particular, the next experiments will stucdv the hierarchical organisation of groupings within a given work. In addition. a procedure will be tested that allows a subject to "erase retrospectively segmentation marked at a moment of temporary hesitation, or by rmistakte The lack of familiarity with the contemporary repertory has meant that non-musicians felt astonished and unsure of their abilities in accomplishing the task. The reality of the results is far from this negative appreciation. OIl the contrary, this first approach has demonstrated that there are important similarities in the perceptive procedures of both musicians and non-musicians Divergencies occur to the extent that the untrained listener finds it relatively more difficult to conceive structures as unitary if there are internal interrup- tions. Therefore, as it has been shown by the grouping time-span analysis, groupings duration is smaller in non-musicians. But the basic aptitudes allowing anyone to accede to music as a conscious listener are potentiallv available. An adequate pedagogical approach that took advantage of this potential could lead anyone to acquire mastery of the listening process-that "musical intelligence" as Andre Souris used the term (1976. p. 251. )(4) 42 Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings 43 Notes (1) Copyright 1985, Mark of the Unicorn, 222 Third Street, Cambridge, MA 02142. (2) Let us notice also the eight points where the curve drops to zero, which indicates, as it has been mentioned earlier, the segmentations not chosen by musicians and the two points (2' 10" and 2' 30") not chosen in both auditions. (3) Let us recall again that the LogRank test (LR) gives more weight at long duration events, whereas the Wilcoxon test (W) reverses more or less this perspective: that is the reason why a significant difference for the first section/first audition is observed with LR test, because of the big amount of short groupings given by non-musicians. (4) This study has benefited enormously from insightful remarks made by Fred Lerdahl on reading the first draft of the paper. I would also like to thank John Sloboda, Piet Vos and an anonymous reviewer for helpful comments on the manuscript; Marie-Isabelle Collart for help in translation, as well as to Stephane Lejoly, Jean-Marc Sullon and Christian Vanderbeeken for the graphics, and all those-composers, musicians of the Ensemble Musique Nouvelle and non-musicians who participated in the experiment as subjects. My thanks go to all of them. Parts of this study were the subject of a paper read at the Symposium Composition et Perception, Geneva, March 19-22, 1987, and at the Second Conference on Science and Music, London, City University, 17-19 September 1987. References Apel, W. (1972). Form. In: Harvard Dictionary of Music, 326-327. Cox, D. R. and Oakes, D. (1984-1988). Analysis of Survival Data, New York, Champan and Hall. Deliege, I. (1985). Perception des Formations elementaires de la Musique. Analyse Musicale, 1, 21-28. Deliege, I. (19871a]). Le Parallelisme, Support d'une Analyse auditive de la Musique: Vers un Modele des Parcours cognitifs de l'Information musicale. Application au SYRYNX de Debussy. Analyse Musicale, 6, 73-79. Deliege, I. (1987[bI). Grouping Conditions in Listening to Music: An Approach to Lerdahl and Jackendoff's Grouping Preference Rules. Music Perception, 4(4), 325-360. Deliege, I. (1989). A Perceptual Approach to contemporary Musical Forms. In: S. McAdams and I. Deliege (Eds.), The Music and the Cognitive Sciences, Contemporary Music Review, 4, 213-230, London, Harwood. Translated in the French. In: La Musique et les Sciences cognitives, Bruxelles, Pierre Mardaga, 305-326. Deli&ge, I. and El Ahmadi, A. (1989). Mecanisme d'Extraction d'Indices dans le Groupement. Etude de Perception sur la Sequenza VI,pour alto solo de Luciano Berio. Actes du Symposium "Composition et Perception Musicales", Universite de Geneve, mars 1987, Contrechamps, 10, 85-104. Fraisse, P. (1956). Les Structures rythmiques. Louvain, Publications universitaires. Fraisse, P. (1974). La Psychologie du Rythme, Paris, PUF. Fraisse, P. (1982). Rhythm and Tempo. In: D. Deutsch (Ed.), The Psychology of Music, New York, Academic Press, 149-180. Gibson, J. J. (1982). What is involved in Surface Perception? In: J. Beck (Ed.), Organisation and Representation in Perception, Hillsdale, N.J., Lawrence Erlbaum, 151-157. Imberty, M. (1985). La Cathedrale Engloutie de Claude Debussy: de la Perception au Sens. Revue de Musique des Universites canadiennes, 6, 90-160. Lerdahl, F. and Jackendoff, R. (1983). A Generative Theory of Tonal Music, Cambridge, Mass., MIT Press. Lieury, A. (1980). Les Procedes mnemotechniques: Science ou Charlatanisme? Bruxelles, Pierre Mardaga. Moore, B. C. J. (1982). An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing. New York, Academic Press. Palmer, S. E. (1982). Symmetry, Transformation and the Structure of Perceptual Systems. In: J. Beck (Ed.), Organisation and Representation in Perception, Hillsdale, N.J., Lawrence Erlbaum, 95-144. 44 Irene Deliege and Abdessadek El Ahmadi Sloboda, J. A., Hermelin, B. and O'Connor, N. (1985). An Exceptional Musical Meemory. Ml4su Perception, 3(2), 155-170. Souris, A. (1976). Coniditionts de la Musique et autres Ecrits. Bruxelles, Editions dew lit U13 adld Paris, CNRS. Whittall, A. (1986). Form. In: S. Sadie (Ed.), fIlie NeuL Grove l)ictionarY Of 4Mu.sit-a nwd Musicians., VI, 709-710. </meta-value>
</custom-meta>
</custom-meta-wrap>
</article-meta>
</front>
<back>
<ref-list>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Apel, W.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1972</year>
).
<article-title>Form</article-title>
. In:
<source>Harvard Dictionary of Music</source>
,
<fpage>326</fpage>
-
<lpage>327</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Cox, D. R.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Oakes, D.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1984-1988</year>
).
<source>Analysis of Survival Data</source>
,
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
,
<publisher-name>Champan and Hall</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="other" xlink:type="simple">Deliege, I. (1985). Perception des Formations elementaires de la Musique.
<italic>Analyse Musicale, 1,</italic>
21-28.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="other" xlink:type="simple">Deliege, I. (19871[a]). Le Parallelisme, Support d'une Analyse auditive de la Musique: Vers un Modele des Parcours cognitifs de l'Information musicale. Application au SYRYNX de Debussy.
<italic>Analyse Musicale, 6,</italic>
73-79.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Deliege, I.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1987[b]</year>
).
<article-title>Grouping Conditions in Listening to Music: An Approach to Lerdahl and Jackendoff's Grouping Preference Rules</article-title>
.
<source>Music Perception</source>
,
<volume>4</volume>
(
<issue>4</issue>
),
<fpage>325</fpage>
-
<lpage>360</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="other" xlink:type="simple">Deliege, I. (1989). A Perceptual Approach to contemporary Musical Forms. In: S. McAdams and I. Deliege (Eds.),
<italic>The Music and the Cognitive Sciences, Contemporary Music Review,
<bold>
<italic>4,</italic>
</bold>
</italic>
213-230, London, Harwood. Translated in the French. In:
<italic>La Musique et les Sciences cognitives,</italic>
Bruxelles, Pierre Mardaga, 305-326.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="other" xlink:type="simple">Deliege, I. and El Ahmadi, A. (1989). Mecanisme d'Extraction d'Indices dans le Groupement. Etude de Perception sur la Sequenza VI,pour alto solo de Luciano Berio.
<italic>Actes du Symposium "Composition et Perception Musicales",</italic>
Universite de Geneve, mars 1987,
<italic>Contrechamps,</italic>
10, 85-104.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="other" xlink:type="simple">Fraisse, P. (1956).
<italic>Les Structures rythmiques.</italic>
Louvain, Publications universitaires.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="other" xlink:type="simple">Fraisse, P. (1974).
<italic>La Psychologie du Rythme,</italic>
Paris, PUF.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Fraisse, P.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1982</year>
).
<article-title>Rhythm and Tempo</article-title>
. In:
<name name-style="western">
<surname>D. Deutsch</surname>
</name>
(Ed.),
<source>The Psychology of Music</source>
,
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
,
<publisher-name>Academic Press</publisher-name>
,
<fpage>149</fpage>
-
<lpage>180</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Gibson, J. J.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1982</year>
).
<article-title>What is involved in Surface Perception?</article-title>
In:
<name name-style="western">
<surname>J. Beck</surname>
</name>
(Ed.),
<source>Organisation and Representation in Perception</source>
,
<publisher-loc>Hillsdale, N.J.</publisher-loc>
,
<publisher-name>Lawrence Erlbaum</publisher-name>
,
<fpage>151</fpage>
-
<lpage>157</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="other" xlink:type="simple">Imberty, M. (1985). La Cathedrale Engloutie de Claude Debussy: de la Perception au Sens.
<italic>Revue de Musique des Universites canadiennes,</italic>
6, 90-160.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Lerdahl, F.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Jackendoff, R.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1983</year>
).
<source>A Generative Theory of Tonal Music</source>
,
<publisher-loc>Cambridge, Mass.</publisher-loc>
,
<publisher-name>MIT Press</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="other" xlink:type="simple">Lieury, A. (1980).
<italic>Les Procedes mnemotechniques: Science ou Charlatanisme?</italic>
Bruxelles, Pierre Mardaga.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Moore, B. C. J.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1982</year>
).
<source>An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
,
<publisher-name>Academic Press</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Palmer, S. E.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1982</year>
).
<article-title>Symmetry, Transformation and the Structure of Perceptual Systems</article-title>
. In:
<name name-style="western">
<surname>J. Beck</surname>
</name>
(Ed.),
<source>Organisation and Representation in Perception</source>
,
<publisher-loc>Hillsdale, N.J.</publisher-loc>
,
<publisher-name>Lawrence Erlbaum</publisher-name>
,
<fpage>95</fpage>
-
<lpage>144</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Sloboda, J. A.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Hermelin, B.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>O'Connor, N.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1985</year>
).
<article-title>An Exceptional Musical Meemory</article-title>
.
<source>Ml4su Perception</source>
,
<volume>3</volume>
(
<issue>2</issue>
),
<fpage>155</fpage>
-
<lpage>170</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="other" xlink:type="simple">Souris, A. (1976).
<italic>Coniditionts de la Musique et autres Ecrits.</italic>
Bruxelles, Editions dew lit U13 adld Paris, CNRS.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="other" xlink:type="simple">Whittall, A. (1986). Form. In: S. Sadie (Ed.),
<italic>fIlie NeuL Grove l)ictionarY Of 4Mu.sit-a nwd</italic>
Musicians., VI, 709-710.</citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</article>
</istex:document>
</istex:metadataXml>
<mods version="3.6">
<titleInfo lang="en">
<title>Mechanisms of Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings: A Study of Perception on Sequenza VI for Viola Solo by Luciano Berio</title>
</titleInfo>
<titleInfo type="alternative" lang="en" contentType="CDATA">
<title>Mechanisms of Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings: A Study of Perception on Sequenza VI for Viola Solo by Luciano Berio</title>
</titleInfo>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Irene</namePart>
<namePart type="family">Deliege</namePart>
</name>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Abdessadek</namePart>
<namePart type="family">El Ahnmadi</namePart>
<affiliation>Unite de Recherche en Psychologie (tie Ia Miqsltue, Laboratoire de Psychologie Experinientale. Universite de Liege, Sart Tilman, B132 4000 Liege, Belgium</affiliation>
</name>
<typeOfResource>text</typeOfResource>
<genre type="research-article" displayLabel="research-article" authority="ISTEX" authorityURI="https://content-type.data.istex.fr" valueURI="https://content-type.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/XTP-1JC4F85T-7">research-article</genre>
<originInfo>
<publisher>Sage Publications</publisher>
<place>
<placeTerm type="text">Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</placeTerm>
</place>
<dateIssued encoding="w3cdtf">1990-04</dateIssued>
<copyrightDate encoding="w3cdtf">1990</copyrightDate>
</originInfo>
<language>
<languageTerm type="code" authority="iso639-2b">eng</languageTerm>
<languageTerm type="code" authority="rfc3066">en</languageTerm>
</language>
<abstract lang="en">Eighteen musicians and 18 non-musicians were asked to indicate perceived segmentations in a performance of Berio's Sequenza VI. Segmentations were marked during active listening by depressing a key oil a compLitCie console. The segmentations of both musicians and non-musicians were i general agreement with a reference analysis pi-ovided bv two compposers. These segmentations had a high degree of stability over two 1istenings. J'he results are discussed in terms of a theory of cue extraction allowing identification of the beginning and end of major compositioned units</abstract>
<relatedItem type="host">
<titleInfo>
<title>Psychology of music</title>
</titleInfo>
<genre type="journal" authority="ISTEX" authorityURI="https://publication-type.data.istex.fr" valueURI="https://publication-type.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/JMC-0GLKJH51-B">journal</genre>
<identifier type="ISSN">0305-7356</identifier>
<identifier type="eISSN">1741-3087</identifier>
<identifier type="PublisherID">POM</identifier>
<identifier type="PublisherID-hwp">sppom</identifier>
<part>
<date>1990</date>
<detail type="volume">
<caption>vol.</caption>
<number>18</number>
</detail>
<detail type="issue">
<caption>no.</caption>
<number>1</number>
</detail>
<extent unit="pages">
<start>18</start>
<end>44</end>
</extent>
</part>
</relatedItem>
<identifier type="istex">6510B7F256B8D453C34D907F0678D969B2490CA4</identifier>
<identifier type="ark">ark:/67375/M70-MWBZPQRV-V</identifier>
<identifier type="DOI">10.1177/0305735690181003</identifier>
<identifier type="ArticleID">10.1177_0305735690181003</identifier>
<recordInfo>
<recordContentSource authority="ISTEX" authorityURI="https://loaded-corpus.data.istex.fr" valueURI="https://loaded-corpus.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/XBH-0J1N7DQT-B">sage</recordContentSource>
</recordInfo>
</mods>
<json:item>
<extension>json</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>application/json</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/6510B7F256B8D453C34D907F0678D969B2490CA4/metadata/json</uri>
</json:item>
</metadata>
<serie></serie>
</istex>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Wicri/Musique/explor/DebussyV1/Data/Istex/Corpus
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 002758 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Istex/Corpus/biblio.hfd -nk 002758 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Wicri/Musique
   |area=    DebussyV1
   |flux=    Istex
   |étape=   Corpus
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     ISTEX:6510B7F256B8D453C34D907F0678D969B2490CA4
   |texte=   Mechanisms of Cue Extraction in Musical Groupings: A Study of Perception on Sequenza VI for Viola Solo by Luciano Berio
}}

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.33.
Data generation: Tue Sep 25 16:34:07 2018. Site generation: Mon Mar 11 10:31:28 2024