Serveur d'exploration Debussy

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

The Effect of Music on Perceived Atmosphere and Purchase Intentions in a Restaurant

Identifieur interne : 002417 ( Istex/Corpus ); précédent : 002416; suivant : 002418

The Effect of Music on Perceived Atmosphere and Purchase Intentions in a Restaurant

Auteurs : Stephanie Wilson

Source :

RBID : ISTEX:177FF7A01B7BF67307C70675E6A526DF9196E178

English descriptors

Abstract

Extending research by North and Hargreaves (1998), this study investigated the effect of music on perceived atmosphere and purchase intentions in a restaurant. Four musical styles (jazz, popular, easy listening and classical) and no music were played in a restaurant over two consecutive weeks. Results indicated that different types of music had different effects on perceived atmosphere and the amount patrons were prepared to spend. Classical, jazz and popular music were associated with patrons being prepared to spend the most on their main meal. This value was found to be significantly lower in the absence of music and when easy listening was played. There was some evidence that the type of music also had an effect on the amount of money patrons actually spent in the restaurant. Overall, the study contributes to the development of a model that seeks to account for the relationship between music and consumer behaviour.

Url:
DOI: 10.1177/0305735603031001327

Links to Exploration step

ISTEX:177FF7A01B7BF67307C70675E6A526DF9196E178

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI wicri:istexFullTextTei="biblStruct">
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">The Effect of Music on Perceived Atmosphere and Purchase Intentions in a Restaurant</title>
<author wicri:is="90%">
<name sortKey="Wilson, Stephanie" sort="Wilson, Stephanie" uniqKey="Wilson S" first="Stephanie" last="Wilson">Stephanie Wilson</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>University of New South Wales,</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>E-mail: stephaniewilson@unsw.edu.au</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">ISTEX</idno>
<idno type="RBID">ISTEX:177FF7A01B7BF67307C70675E6A526DF9196E178</idno>
<date when="2003" year="2003">2003</date>
<idno type="doi">10.1177/0305735603031001327</idno>
<idno type="url">https://api.istex.fr/document/177FF7A01B7BF67307C70675E6A526DF9196E178/fulltext/pdf</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Istex/Corpus">002417</idno>
<idno type="wicri:explorRef" wicri:stream="Istex" wicri:step="Corpus" wicri:corpus="ISTEX">002417</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">The Effect of Music on Perceived Atmosphere and Purchase Intentions in a Restaurant</title>
<author wicri:is="90%">
<name sortKey="Wilson, Stephanie" sort="Wilson, Stephanie" uniqKey="Wilson S" first="Stephanie" last="Wilson">Stephanie Wilson</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>University of New South Wales,</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>E-mail: stephaniewilson@unsw.edu.au</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<monogr></monogr>
<series>
<title level="j">Psychology of music</title>
<idno type="ISSN">0305-7356</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1741-3087</idno>
<imprint>
<publisher>Sage Publications</publisher>
<pubPlace>Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</pubPlace>
<date type="published" when="2003-01">2003-01</date>
<biblScope unit="volume">31</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="issue">1</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" from="93">93</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" to="112">112</biblScope>
</imprint>
<idno type="ISSN">0305-7356</idno>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
<seriesStmt>
<idno type="ISSN">0305-7356</idno>
</seriesStmt>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass>
<keywords scheme="Teeft" xml:lang="en">
<term>Adjective</term>
<term>Alcoholic beverages</term>
<term>Amount patrons</term>
<term>Anova</term>
<term>Appropriateness</term>
<term>Background music</term>
<term>Classical music</term>
<term>Commercial environment</term>
<term>Consumer behaviour</term>
<term>Consumer research</term>
<term>Control condition</term>
<term>Control conditions</term>
<term>Dave brubeck</term>
<term>Different styles</term>
<term>Different types</term>
<term>Dining area</term>
<term>Environmental psychology</term>
<term>Factor analysis</term>
<term>Factor scores</term>
<term>Greater number</term>
<term>Hargreaves</term>
<term>Inferential statistics</term>
<term>Interactive effect</term>
<term>Jazz condition</term>
<term>Less money</term>
<term>Loading factor</term>
<term>Main meal</term>
<term>Maximum amount</term>
<term>Miles davis</term>
<term>Money patrons</term>
<term>More downmarket</term>
<term>More drinks</term>
<term>More money</term>
<term>More people</term>
<term>More time</term>
<term>Music conditions</term>
<term>Music psychology</term>
<term>Music therapy</term>
<term>Musical condition</term>
<term>Musical conditions</term>
<term>Musical styles</term>
<term>Other condition</term>
<term>Other domains</term>
<term>Other factors</term>
<term>Patron</term>
<term>People dining</term>
<term>Piano concerto</term>
<term>Popular music</term>
<term>Present study</term>
<term>Product choice</term>
<term>Purchase intention</term>
<term>Purchase intentions</term>
<term>Rating responses</term>
<term>Restaurant environment</term>
<term>Restaurant patrons</term>
<term>Right association</term>
<term>Sales figures</term>
<term>Same days</term>
<term>Shopping behavior</term>
<term>Significant correlation</term>
<term>Significant correlations</term>
<term>Significant difference</term>
<term>Significant differences</term>
<term>Significant effect</term>
<term>Similar subscripts</term>
<term>Slow music</term>
<term>Social psychology</term>
<term>Testing period</term>
<term>Time subjects</term>
<term>Times patrons</term>
<term>Upbeat</term>
<term>Wine store</term>
</keywords>
</textClass>
<langUsage>
<language ident="en">en</language>
</langUsage>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">Extending research by North and Hargreaves (1998), this study investigated the effect of music on perceived atmosphere and purchase intentions in a restaurant. Four musical styles (jazz, popular, easy listening and classical) and no music were played in a restaurant over two consecutive weeks. Results indicated that different types of music had different effects on perceived atmosphere and the amount patrons were prepared to spend. Classical, jazz and popular music were associated with patrons being prepared to spend the most on their main meal. This value was found to be significantly lower in the absence of music and when easy listening was played. There was some evidence that the type of music also had an effect on the amount of money patrons actually spent in the restaurant. Overall, the study contributes to the development of a model that seeks to account for the relationship between music and consumer behaviour.</div>
</front>
</TEI>
<istex>
<corpusName>sage</corpusName>
<keywords>
<teeft>
<json:string>hargreaves</json:string>
<json:string>classical music</json:string>
<json:string>anova</json:string>
<json:string>money patrons</json:string>
<json:string>patron</json:string>
<json:string>purchase intentions</json:string>
<json:string>testing period</json:string>
<json:string>popular music</json:string>
<json:string>control condition</json:string>
<json:string>people dining</json:string>
<json:string>consumer behaviour</json:string>
<json:string>musical styles</json:string>
<json:string>background music</json:string>
<json:string>main meal</json:string>
<json:string>control conditions</json:string>
<json:string>piano concerto</json:string>
<json:string>more time</json:string>
<json:string>sales figures</json:string>
<json:string>present study</json:string>
<json:string>restaurant environment</json:string>
<json:string>musical conditions</json:string>
<json:string>different styles</json:string>
<json:string>loading factor</json:string>
<json:string>upbeat</json:string>
<json:string>adjective</json:string>
<json:string>jazz condition</json:string>
<json:string>consumer research</json:string>
<json:string>maximum amount</json:string>
<json:string>significant correlation</json:string>
<json:string>other condition</json:string>
<json:string>more drinks</json:string>
<json:string>greater number</json:string>
<json:string>wine store</json:string>
<json:string>factor analysis</json:string>
<json:string>significant differences</json:string>
<json:string>music psychology</json:string>
<json:string>more money</json:string>
<json:string>purchase intention</json:string>
<json:string>restaurant patrons</json:string>
<json:string>other factors</json:string>
<json:string>factor scores</json:string>
<json:string>amount patrons</json:string>
<json:string>social psychology</json:string>
<json:string>times patrons</json:string>
<json:string>commercial environment</json:string>
<json:string>same days</json:string>
<json:string>miles davis</json:string>
<json:string>dining area</json:string>
<json:string>time subjects</json:string>
<json:string>more people</json:string>
<json:string>alcoholic beverages</json:string>
<json:string>musical condition</json:string>
<json:string>interactive effect</json:string>
<json:string>significant correlations</json:string>
<json:string>similar subscripts</json:string>
<json:string>significant effect</json:string>
<json:string>rating responses</json:string>
<json:string>inferential statistics</json:string>
<json:string>music conditions</json:string>
<json:string>more downmarket</json:string>
<json:string>significant difference</json:string>
<json:string>less money</json:string>
<json:string>other domains</json:string>
<json:string>right association</json:string>
<json:string>different types</json:string>
<json:string>shopping behavior</json:string>
<json:string>slow music</json:string>
<json:string>environmental psychology</json:string>
<json:string>music therapy</json:string>
<json:string>dave brubeck</json:string>
<json:string>product choice</json:string>
<json:string>appropriateness</json:string>
</teeft>
</keywords>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>Stephanie Wilson</name>
<affiliations>
<json:string>University of New South Wales,</json:string>
<json:string>E-mail: stephaniewilson@unsw.edu.au</json:string>
</affiliations>
</json:item>
</author>
<subject>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>consumer behaviour</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>listening</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>music psychology</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>perceived atmosphere</value>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<lang>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</lang>
<value>restaurant</value>
</json:item>
</subject>
<articleId>
<json:string>10.1177_0305735603031001327</json:string>
</articleId>
<arkIstex>ark:/67375/M70-1BLQJLV5-6</arkIstex>
<language>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</language>
<originalGenre>
<json:string>research-article</json:string>
</originalGenre>
<abstract>Extending research by North and Hargreaves (1998), this study investigated the effect of music on perceived atmosphere and purchase intentions in a restaurant. Four musical styles (jazz, popular, easy listening and classical) and no music were played in a restaurant over two consecutive weeks. Results indicated that different types of music had different effects on perceived atmosphere and the amount patrons were prepared to spend. Classical, jazz and popular music were associated with patrons being prepared to spend the most on their main meal. This value was found to be significantly lower in the absence of music and when easy listening was played. There was some evidence that the type of music also had an effect on the amount of money patrons actually spent in the restaurant. Overall, the study contributes to the development of a model that seeks to account for the relationship between music and consumer behaviour.</abstract>
<qualityIndicators>
<score>8.788</score>
<pdfWordCount>6417</pdfWordCount>
<pdfCharCount>39789</pdfCharCount>
<pdfVersion>1.4</pdfVersion>
<pdfPageCount>20</pdfPageCount>
<pdfPageSize>595 x 842 pts (A4)</pdfPageSize>
<refBibsNative>true</refBibsNative>
<abstractWordCount>149</abstractWordCount>
<abstractCharCount>931</abstractCharCount>
<keywordCount>5</keywordCount>
</qualityIndicators>
<title>The Effect of Music on Perceived Atmosphere and Purchase Intentions in a Restaurant</title>
<genre>
<json:string>research-article</json:string>
</genre>
<host>
<title>Psychology of music</title>
<language>
<json:string>unknown</json:string>
</language>
<issn>
<json:string>0305-7356</json:string>
</issn>
<eissn>
<json:string>1741-3087</json:string>
</eissn>
<publisherId>
<json:string>POM</json:string>
</publisherId>
<volume>31</volume>
<issue>1</issue>
<pages>
<first>93</first>
<last>112</last>
</pages>
<genre>
<json:string>journal</json:string>
</genre>
</host>
<namedEntities>
<unitex>
<date>
<json:string>2003</json:string>
<json:string>1996</json:string>
</date>
<geogName></geogName>
<orgName></orgName>
<orgName_funder></orgName_funder>
<orgName_provider></orgName_provider>
<persName>
<json:string>Findings</json:string>
<json:string>Omar Madji</json:string>
<json:string>Results</json:string>
<json:string>Of Africa</json:string>
<json:string>Dean Ormston</json:string>
<json:string>Andrew Becker</json:string>
<json:string>Gary McPherson</json:string>
<json:string>Emery Schubert</json:string>
<json:string>Meredith Wilson</json:string>
<json:string>Hassan M’Souli</json:string>
</persName>
<placeName></placeName>
<ref_url></ref_url>
<ref_bibl>
<json:string>Mehrabian and Russel (1974)</json:string>
<json:string>North and Hargreaves, 1998: 2267</json:string>
<json:string>Robally et al., 1985</json:string>
<json:string>Yalch and Spangenberg (1990)</json:string>
<json:string>Fried and Berkowitz, 1979</json:string>
<json:string>Dube et al. (1995)</json:string>
<json:string>Stack and Gunlach, 1992</json:string>
<json:string>Alpert and Alpert, 1990</json:string>
<json:string>Bruner (1990)</json:string>
<json:string>Radocy and Boyle (1997)</json:string>
<json:string>supporting Radocy and Boyle, 1997</json:string>
<json:string>Zillmann and Bhatia, 1989</json:string>
<json:string>Milliman (1982)</json:string>
<json:string>Stratton and Zalanowski, 1984</json:string>
<json:string>Areni and Kim, 1993</json:string>
<json:string>Standley, 1995</json:string>
<json:string>Areni and Kim (1993)</json:string>
<json:string>Hargreaves (1996)</json:string>
<json:string>Zullow, 1991</json:string>
<json:string>Radocy and Boyle, 1997</json:string>
<json:string>North and Hargreaves, 1996</json:string>
<json:string>Bleich et al., 1991</json:string>
</ref_bibl>
<bibl></bibl>
</unitex>
</namedEntities>
<ark>
<json:string>ark:/67375/M70-1BLQJLV5-6</json:string>
</ark>
<categories>
<wos>
<json:string>1 - social science</json:string>
<json:string>2 - psychology, experimental</json:string>
<json:string>2 - psychology, educational</json:string>
<json:string>2 - psychology, applied</json:string>
</wos>
<scienceMetrix>
<json:string>1 - health sciences</json:string>
<json:string>2 - psychology & cognitive sciences</json:string>
<json:string>3 - experimental psychology</json:string>
</scienceMetrix>
<scopus>
<json:string>1 - Social Sciences</json:string>
<json:string>2 - Psychology</json:string>
<json:string>3 - Psychology (miscellaneous)</json:string>
<json:string>1 - Social Sciences</json:string>
<json:string>2 - Arts and Humanities</json:string>
<json:string>3 - Music</json:string>
</scopus>
<inist>
<json:string>1 - sciences humaines et sociales</json:string>
</inist>
</categories>
<publicationDate>2003</publicationDate>
<copyrightDate>2003</copyrightDate>
<doi>
<json:string>10.1177/0305735603031001327</json:string>
</doi>
<id>177FF7A01B7BF67307C70675E6A526DF9196E178</id>
<score>1</score>
<fulltext>
<json:item>
<extension>pdf</extension>
<original>true</original>
<mimetype>application/pdf</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/177FF7A01B7BF67307C70675E6A526DF9196E178/fulltext/pdf</uri>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<extension>zip</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>application/zip</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/177FF7A01B7BF67307C70675E6A526DF9196E178/fulltext/zip</uri>
</json:item>
<istex:fulltextTEI uri="https://api.istex.fr/document/177FF7A01B7BF67307C70675E6A526DF9196E178/fulltext/tei">
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">The Effect of Music on Perceived Atmosphere and Purchase Intentions in a Restaurant</title>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<authority>ISTEX</authority>
<publisher scheme="https://publisher-list.data.istex.fr">Sage Publications</publisher>
<pubPlace>Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</pubPlace>
<availability>
<licence>
<p>sage</p>
</licence>
</availability>
<p scheme="https://loaded-corpus.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/XBH-0J1N7DQT-B"></p>
<date>2003</date>
</publicationStmt>
<notesStmt>
<note type="research-article" scheme="https://content-type.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/XTP-1JC4F85T-7">research-article</note>
<note type="journal" scheme="https://publication-type.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/JMC-0GLKJH51-B">journal</note>
</notesStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct type="inbook">
<analytic>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">The Effect of Music on Perceived Atmosphere and Purchase Intentions in a Restaurant</title>
<author xml:id="author-0000">
<persName>
<forename type="first">Stephanie</forename>
<surname>Wilson</surname>
</persName>
<email>stephaniewilson@unsw.edu.au</email>
<affiliation>University of New South Wales,</affiliation>
</author>
<idno type="istex">177FF7A01B7BF67307C70675E6A526DF9196E178</idno>
<idno type="ark">ark:/67375/M70-1BLQJLV5-6</idno>
<idno type="DOI">10.1177/0305735603031001327</idno>
<idno type="article-id">10.1177_0305735603031001327</idno>
</analytic>
<monogr>
<title level="j">Psychology of music</title>
<idno type="pISSN">0305-7356</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1741-3087</idno>
<idno type="publisher-id">POM</idno>
<idno type="PublisherID-hwp">sppom</idno>
<imprint>
<publisher>Sage Publications</publisher>
<pubPlace>Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</pubPlace>
<date type="published" when="2003-01"></date>
<biblScope unit="volume">31</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="issue">1</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" from="93">93</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" to="112">112</biblScope>
</imprint>
</monogr>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<creation>
<date>2003</date>
</creation>
<langUsage>
<language ident="en">en</language>
</langUsage>
<abstract xml:lang="en">
<p>Extending research by North and Hargreaves (1998), this study investigated the effect of music on perceived atmosphere and purchase intentions in a restaurant. Four musical styles (jazz, popular, easy listening and classical) and no music were played in a restaurant over two consecutive weeks. Results indicated that different types of music had different effects on perceived atmosphere and the amount patrons were prepared to spend. Classical, jazz and popular music were associated with patrons being prepared to spend the most on their main meal. This value was found to be significantly lower in the absence of music and when easy listening was played. There was some evidence that the type of music also had an effect on the amount of money patrons actually spent in the restaurant. Overall, the study contributes to the development of a model that seeks to account for the relationship between music and consumer behaviour.</p>
</abstract>
<textClass>
<keywords scheme="keyword">
<list>
<head>keywords</head>
<item>
<term>consumer behaviour</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>listening</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>music psychology</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>perceived atmosphere</term>
</item>
<item>
<term>restaurant</term>
</item>
</list>
</keywords>
</textClass>
</profileDesc>
<revisionDesc>
<change when="2003-01">Published</change>
</revisionDesc>
</teiHeader>
</istex:fulltextTEI>
<json:item>
<extension>txt</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>text/plain</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/177FF7A01B7BF67307C70675E6A526DF9196E178/fulltext/txt</uri>
</json:item>
</fulltext>
<metadata>
<istex:metadataXml wicri:clean="corpus sage not found" wicri:toSee="no header">
<istex:xmlDeclaration>version="1.0" encoding="UTF-8"</istex:xmlDeclaration>
<istex:docType PUBLIC="-//NLM//DTD Journal Publishing DTD v2.3 20070202//EN" URI="journalpublishing.dtd" name="istex:docType"></istex:docType>
<istex:document>
<article article-type="research-article" dtd-version="2.3" xml:lang="EN">
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="hwp">sppom</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="publisher-id">POM</journal-id>
<journal-title>Psychology of Music</journal-title>
<issn pub-type="ppub">0305-7356</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Sage Publications</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</publisher-loc>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0305735603031001327</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="publisher-id">10.1177_0305735603031001327</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Articles</subject>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>The Effect of Music on Perceived Atmosphere and Purchase Intentions in a Restaurant</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Wilson</surname>
<given-names>Stephanie</given-names>
</name>
<aff>University of New South Wales,
<email xlink:type="simple">stephaniewilson@unsw.edu.au</email>
</aff>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<pub-date pub-type="ppub">
<month>01</month>
<year>2003</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>31</volume>
<issue>1</issue>
<fpage>93</fpage>
<lpage>112</lpage>
<abstract>
<p>Extending research by North and Hargreaves (1998), this study investigated the effect of music on perceived atmosphere and purchase intentions in a restaurant. Four musical styles (jazz, popular, easy listening and classical) and no music were played in a restaurant over two consecutive weeks. Results indicated that different types of music had different effects on perceived atmosphere and the amount patrons were prepared to spend. Classical, jazz and popular music were associated with patrons being prepared to spend the most on their main meal. This value was found to be significantly lower in the absence of music and when easy listening was played. There was some evidence that the type of music also had an effect on the amount of money patrons actually spent in the restaurant. Overall, the study contributes to the development of a model that seeks to account for the relationship between music and consumer behaviour.</p>
</abstract>
<kwd-group>
<kwd>consumer behaviour</kwd>
<kwd>listening</kwd>
<kwd>music psychology</kwd>
<kwd>perceived atmosphere</kwd>
<kwd>restaurant</kwd>
</kwd-group>
<custom-meta-wrap>
<custom-meta xlink:type="simple">
<meta-name>sagemeta-type</meta-name>
<meta-value>Journal Article</meta-value>
</custom-meta>
<custom-meta xlink:type="simple">
<meta-name>search-text</meta-name>
<meta-value> The effect of music on perceived atmosphere and purchase intentions in a restaurant93 A RT I C L E Psychology of Music Psychology of Music Copyright 2003 Society for Education, Music and Psychology Research vol 31(1): 93112 [0305-7356 (200301) 31:1; 93112; 029327] S T E P H A N I E W I L S O N U N I V E R S I T Y O F N E W S O U T H WA L E S A B S T R AC T Extending research by North and Hargreaves (1998), this study investigated the effect of music on perceived atmosphere and purchase intentions in a restaurant. Four musical styles (jazz, popular, easy listening and classical) and no music were played in a restaurant over two consecutive weeks. Results indicated that different types of music had different effects on perceived atmos- phere and the amount patrons were prepared to spend. Classical, jazz and popu- lar music were associated with patrons being prepared to spend the most on their main meal. This value was found to be significantly lower in the absence of music and when easy listening was played. There was some evidence that the type of music also had an effect on the amount of money patrons actually spent in the restaurant. Overall, the study contributes to the development of a model that seeks to account for the relationship between music and consumer behaviour. K E Y WO R D S : consumer behaviour, listening, music psychology, perceived atmosphere, restaurant Introduction Despite the amount of money spent on music-related resources in the com- mercial sector, research which investigates relationships between music and consumer behaviour is relatively sparse. Studies in social psychology have demonstrated a range of interactions between music and the social context in which it is heard (Fried and Berkowitz, 1979; Stratton and Zalanowski, 1984; Zillmann and Bhatia, 1989; Bleich et al., 1991; Zullow, 1991; Stack and Gunlach, 1992; Standley, 1995). Few studies, however, have considered the influence of music in everyday commercial environments such as restau- rants and other retail outlets. Research in this area has the potential to pro- vide commercial practitioners with guidelines regarding how to enhance sempre : business and increase sales through the use of music, in addition to making a contribution to an important area of social psychology. THE EFFECT OF MUSIC ON PERCEIVED ATMOSPHERE In 1996, North and Hargreaves studied the effect of music on atmosphere in a university cafeteria (North and Hargreaves, 1996). Results suggested that customers' 'liking' of the cafeteria increased with their 'liking' for the music played within it. In a follow-up study, North and Hargreaves (1998) investi- gated whether affective responses to music of different styles could influence the perceived characteristics of a commercial environment. Results suggested that different musical conditions influenced subjects' perceptions about the cafeteria. Furthermore, the nature of subjects' perceptions of the cafeteria showed a relationship to their perception of the music. North and Hargreaves (1998) stated that the implications of their study needed to be validated by future research. In response, the present study explores the effect of music on perceived atmosphere and purchase intentions in a restaurant. While the methodology employed in the North and Hargreaves study is maintained to a large extent, the present research design has been expanded to incorporate a broader range of musical conditions. Further, as suggested by North and Hargreaves, the present study utilizes a non-student subject sample. THE EFFECT OF MUSIC ON PRODUCT CHOICE AND PURCHASE INTENTIONS A number of studies suggest that music has the potential to influence prod- uct choice (Alpert and Alpert, 1990; Areni and Kim, 1993). The idea that music conveys and triggers relevant information that may prime consumers' beliefs about a product has been described as musical 'fit'. Areni and Kim (1993) applied the idea of musical 'fit' to shopping behaviour by playing classi- cal music and Top-40 music in a wine store. The results showed that classical music led to consumers purchasing more expensive wine. These results sup- port MacInnis and Park's (1991) notion that persuasion is enhanced when the music is appropriate for the context in which it is played, and Yalch and Spangenberg's (1990) suggestion that classical music is associated with the perception of higher-priced store items. The effect of musical tempo on the speed of consumer behaviour and the amount of money spent has also been investigated. For example, Milliman (1982) found that slower music was associated with a slower shopping pace, and increased gross sales. In a further study, Milliman (1986) tested the effect of fast and slow music in an 'upscale' restaurant. The results showed that diners ate more quickly when fast music was playing (see also Robally et al., 1985). On the nights when slow music was playing, customers spent signifi- cantly more time in the restaurant and more money on alcoholic beverages. Bruner (1990) suggests that the style of music played is likely to have a more significant effect on customers' perceptions and choices. This is 94Psychology of Music 31(1) supported by Yalch and Spangenberg (1990), who investigated the influence of different styles of music on customers' estimates of the amount of time they spent shopping. The study suggested that consumers who are exposed to 'non-typical' stimuli in the environment (for example, younger customers encountering easy listening music) tend to overestimate time durations. Several studies of in-store music have drawn on an environmental psy- chology model proposed by Mehrabian and Russel (1974). This model states that people respond to environments according to two primary factors: pleas- ure and arousal. In a more pleasurable environment consumers are more likely to demonstrate 'approach behaviours' towards it. In turn, an environ- ment with arousal-evoking qualities is likely to enhance the effects of pleas- ure. Dube et al. (1995) demonstrated the effect of pleasure and arousal induced by music on consumers' desire to engage in buyerseller interactions in a bank. Similarly, North and Hargreaves (1996) found that 'liked' music was positively related to patrons' willingness to return to a dining area, and their willingness to interact with others. The literature reviewed above provides evidence that music can influence factors such as the speed of consumer activity, product choice, customers' desire to affiliate, and their estimation of time. One area that has been given comparatively little attention in the literature is the extent to which music might actually influence the amount of money patrons are prepared to spend. Introductory evidence has been provided by North and Hargreaves (1998), who found that the type of music played in a cafeteria influenced the amount of money patrons were prepared to spend. In addition, they found that sales figures for the cafeteria were significantly higher when classical and popular music were played. AWARENESS AND APPROPRIATENESS It has been suggested that people are likely to spend more time and money in a restaurant or retail environment if the music being played is considered appropriate (Radocy and Boyle, 1997). Therefore, patrons in the present study were asked to indicate whether they thought the music being played in the restaurant was appropriate. Responses are considered in terms of patrons' perception of atmosphere and the amount of money they are willing to spend. Patrons were also asked to rate their level of awareness of the music being played. Based on the characteristics of stimulative and sedative music described by Radocy and Boyle (1997), it was predicted that subjects' awareness of the music would be greater in the popular, jazz and control conditions. Aim To extend the literature on perceived atmosphere and purchase intentions in the commercial environment, the present study has four aims: Wilson: The effect of music in a restaurant95 1. To investigate the extent to which the type of music being played influ- ences patrons' perception of a restaurant environment; 2. To examine the influence of different musical styles on the amount of money patrons are prepared to spend, and the amount of money they actually spend; 3. To investigate interactive effects between music, perceived atmosphere and a range of other variables such as the number of people dining, the number of times patrons have been to the restaurant before, the amount of alcohol consumed and patrons' perception of the quality of food and service; and 4. To examine whether the appropriateness of the music influences patrons' perception of the restaurant and the amount of money they spend. Method PARTICIPANTS All patrons present in the restaurant during the testing period were eligible for the study. The sample comprised 300 subjects, that is, a total of 50 sub- jects for each condition over the 12-day testing period. Testing was carried out between 7.30 pm and 11.30 pm from Monday to Saturday over the two weeks. Of the 300 subjects, 45.4 percent were male and 54.6 percent were female. The distribution of participants according to gender and age is pre- sented in Figure 1. As shown, the majority of subjects were aged between 20 and 39 years. Patrons were approached at their tables at the end of their meal and asked to 96Psychology of Music 31(1) F I G U R E 1 Distribution of participants by age and gender. % 051 01 5 2 02 5 3 0 <10 yrs1019 yrs2029 yrs3039 yrs4049 yrs5059 yrs6069 yrs70 yrs Age MaleFemale complete a questionnaire about the restaurant. The first questionnaire of the evening was not administered until the music had been playing for at least 30 minutes to ensure that participants had sufficient exposure before responding. DESIGN The experiment was conducted at Out Of Africa, a popular Sydney restaurant. The choice of restaurant was governed by the following criteria: (a) seating capacity >100; (b) high-quality stereo system and speakers; (c) close com- petitors with other restaurants in the area; and (d) a diverse range of clien- tele. In week one the presentation of conditions occurred in the following order: jazz, no music, classical, easy listening, control and popular. This order was changed in the second week (classical, jazz, popular, control, no music, easy listening) to reduce the effect associated with day of the week. The experiment was based on a time series design with the aim of examin- ing the intervention of a series of conditions. As shown, a control group was incorporated by administering questionnaires under 'normal conditions' on two nights during the testing period (where restaurant owners would play the music they would normally play). The control condition comprised a mix- ture of world music (played on CD), and live music (a 3-piece African band). While the live music component may be seen as reducing the homogeneity of the control as a single condition, its inclusion was viewed as providing a more balanced and accurate representation of what was normally played (and experienced) in the restaurant. MATERIALS With the exception of the 'no-music' condition and the non-intervention control, each condition employed several hours of music that was previously recorded onto a series of CDs (see Appendix). The four musical styles present- ed included classical, popular, easy listening and jazz. Following North and Hargreaves (1998), the pieces selected for the study were typical examples of the style. On each night during the study, the music was played on a high-quality CD player through four speakers that were suspended in each corner of the restaurant. The volume of the music was held constant, and at a level where it was clearly audible while still allowing patrons to talk over it comfortably. QUESTIONNAIRES Two separate questionnaires were designed for the study. The first, completed by restaurant management prior to testing, was designed to collect informa- tion about the type of music typically played, and details of the consistency of other factors in the restaurant (for example, lighting, decor, service, menu) which might influence the results of the study. The second questionnaire, described below, was administered during the testing period to restaurant patrons.Wilson: The effect of music in a restaurant97 PATRON QUESTIONNAIRE The first section of the questionnaire asked patrons to indicate the number of times they had been to the restaurant previously, their reason for dining, the number of people they were dining with, and whether they thought they would return to the restaurant. This information was obtained to allow a more detailed investigation of the interaction between music and consumer behaviour. For example, we might expect these extraneous factors to enhance or reduce the impact of music on patrons' perception of the restaurant envi- ronment, and the amount they are prepared to spend. These early questions also acted as 'distracters' the intention being that subjects would be less inclined to think that the survey was about the music being played. Following this, subjects were asked to rate the characteristics of the restaurant accord- ing to a list of 20 adjectives. For the purpose of comparison, the adjectives used in the study were identical to those used by North and Hargreaves (1998). Patrons made their responses on an 11-point scale (0 = 'the restaurant definitely does not possess this characteristic' to 10 = 'the restau- rant definitely does possess this characteristic'). Subjects were then asked to rate the quality of food and service in the restaurant, the extent to which they were aware of the music being played (0 = 'not at all aware' to 5 = 'extremely aware'), and whether they thought the music being played was appropriate. The second section of the questionnaire asked patrons to respond to the music being played. This section was answered by all subjects with the excep- tion of those in the 'no music' group. Subjects were asked to rate the music according to the same set of adjectives used previously so that the relation- ship between music and perceived atmosphere could be examined directly. Once again, patrons responded on an 11-point scale (0 = 'the music definitely does not possess this characteristic' to 10 = 'the music definitely does possess this characteristic'). As a means of examining purchase intention, the final section of the questionnaire asked patrons to indicate the maximum amount of money they were prepared to spend on their main meal. Results PERCEIVED CHARACTERISTICS OF THE RESTAURANT Differences between the four musical styles, no music and the control condi- tion on participants' ratings of atmosphere were investigated using a MANO- VA. The overall difference between musical styles was found to be significant, F(294, 5) = 12.55, p < .0001. Table 1 summarizes differences between the conditions for subjects' rat- ings of atmosphere. The results of Tukey HSD post-hoc tests are also present- ed, indicating several significant differences between conditions. The relationship between responses to the restaurant and responses to the music for each adjective was investigated by a series of correlations. The 98Psychology of Music 31(1) result for each adjective was found to be significant at p = .001, with r values ranging between .23 and .52. A factor analysis was conducted to further examine subjects' responses to the restaurant according to the 20 adjectives. Varimax rotation of the princi- pal components solution generated five factors with eigenvalues greater than 1. In total, these factors accounted for 61.3 percent of the variance in patrons' responses. Table 2 presents the details of factor loadings greater than .30. These loadings led to Factor 1 being interpreted as upbeat; Factor 2 as peaceful/passive; Factor 3 as tacky; Factor 4 as invigorating/stimulating; and Factor 5 as upmarket/sophisticated. Differences between the musical condi- tions on each factor were explored using factor scores. The results of one-way ANOVAs and Tukey HSD post-hoc tests are reported in Table 3. These results suggest that different musical styles produced differences in the general perceived characteristics of the restaurant. For example, no music was associated with the restaurant being perceived as the least upbeat, classical music with the restaurant being perceived as the most Wilson: The effect of music in a restaurant99 TA B L E 1 MANOVA and Tukey tests concerning the effects of music on the perceived characteristics of the restaurant M NoEasy Characteristicmusiclistening ClassicalJazzPopularControlFp Upmarket4.77abcde6.44c6.36b7.02a6.34e6.68d6.98< .0001 Sophisticated4.33abcde6.17c6.13b6.71a6.29e6.61d7.35< .0001 Happy6.93abcd8.12b7.78e8.22a8.20d8.87ce5.74< .0001 Restful5.775.256.56a6.324.66a5.923.11< .05 Fresh6.637.477.717.567.007.681.82.11 Exciting6.37a7.496.787.207.177.71a3.09< .05 Rebellious4.273.904.874.683.974.711.50.19 Cerebral4.134.664.693.803.54 a4.77 a2.26< .05 Feminine2.97b3.78d4.49abc3.02a2.14cde3.55e5.39< .0001 Fun6.43a7.586.60b7.247.408.01ab4.34< .05 Downmarket2.372.291.801.931.942.070.45.81 Youthful5.736.696.405.766.666.631.68.14 Peaceful4.773.47ac6.18cde5.44ab3.37be4.00d8.89< .0001 Spiritual3.13ad3.03beg5.49def5.17abc3.29cf4.54g8.33< .0001 Tacky2.43 a2.241.401.17 a1.511.282.34< .05 Masculine4.073.58a4.87c5.34ab3.14bcd4.61d5.23< .0001 Invigorating4.80a4.81b5.315.634.80c6.28abc3.82< .05 Aggressive2.373.15a2.042.442.291.86a1.89.09 Fashionable6.206.476.676.907.036.680.65.66 Sensual4.57b4.66ac5.24cd6.00a6.62abd6.62bce 16.64< .0001 Note. Means marked by similar subscripts differ at p = .05 for each characteristic. upmarket/sophisticated, and popular music with the restaurant being per- ceived as the most upbeat. Jazz music was associated with the restaurant being perceived as the least peaceful/passive and the most invigorating/stimulating, and easy listening with the restaurant being perceived as the most tacky. 100Psychology of Music 31(1) TA B L E 2 Factor analysis of responses to the restaurant environment Factor 1Factor 2Factor 3Factor 4Factor 5 Characteristicloadingloadingloadingloadingloading Upmarket0.77 Sophisticated0.71 Happy0.83 Restful0.71 Fresh0.62 Exciting0.79 Rebellious0.65 Cerebral0.380.47 Feminine0.68 Fun0.90 Downmarket0.60 Youthful0.72 Peaceful0.79 Spiritual0.420.45 Tacky0.74 Masculine0.370.55 Invigorating0.79 Aggressive0.83 Fashionable0.340.35 Sensual0.62 Eigenvalue5.612.461.811.291.07 % of variance28.112.39.06.55.4 TA B L E 3 One-way ANOVAs and Tukey HSD tests to investigate differences between factor scores and conditions M NoEasy Factormusiclistening ClassicalJazzPopular ControlFp 1. Upbeat0.50abc0.16ad0.43def 0.26g0.31beg 0.18cf5.94 < .0001 2. Peaceful/passive 0.140.060.210.370.260.222.49.03 3. Tacky0.260.36a0.980.170.160.24a3.01 < .05 4. Invigorating/ stimulating0.14a0.42bd0.060.59abc 0.44ce0.25de 6.95 < .0001 5. Upmarket/ sophisticated0.170.22c0.63cde0.36ab0.23be 0.28ad 7.82 < .0001 Note. Means marked by similar subscripts differ at p = .05. PURCHASE INTENTIONS Based on an ANOVA, the effect of music on the maximum amount of money patrons were prepared to spend on their main meal was also significant (p = .001). The no music condition produced the least maximum price subjects were willing to pay (Aus$17.12). Tukey HSD tests showed that this amount was significantly lower (p = .05) than every other condition: easy listening Aus$19.67, classical Aus$20.20, control Aus$20.63, popular Aus$21.01, and jazz Aus$21.82. Easy listening produced the next lowest amount subjects were willing to pay, and this was significantly different from the jazz condition. It could be expected that patrons who had been to the restaurant more than once might be familiar with restaurant prices, and that this may have influenced the maximum price they were prepared to spend. However, no correlation was found (p = .05). Similarly, while it might be expected that subjects with a higher income would be prepared to spend more money on their main meal, this relationship was not found to be significant (p = .05). SALES FIGURES Restaurant sales figures were obtained during the testing period, and for the same days two weeks before and after testing. The variation in sales figures in the weeks before and after the testing period made it difficult to determine the influence of the intervening musical conditions on the amount of money spent. For example, a chi-square goodness-of-fit test revealed significant dif- ferences in sales over the six weeks. However, on four of these days, the high- est and lowest figures occurred outside the testing period. This suggests that the number of people dining in the restaurant was largely responsible for differences in sales figures. On the remaining days, sales were lower when classical music was played, and higher when popular music was played than on the same days before and after testing. As information regarding the fre- quency of patrons was not available to the researcher, it is impossible to draw conclusions regarding the relationship between the type of music played and the amount of money patrons actually spent. However, there are a number of other factors which suggest a relationship between sales figures and the type of music played. Due to the fact that all subjects completed the questionnaire while waiting for their bill, we can assume that no more purchases were made after this time. The final item on the questionnaire asked subjects to indicate what time it was. This information provided some indication of the amount of time (and perhaps money) subjects spent in the restaurant, or at least which conditions may have contributed to patrons remaining in the restaurant later in the evening. Table 4 shows that classical music was associated with relatively few people remaining in the restaurant after 11 pm, and a greater number of subjects leaving the restaurant earlier in the evening, between 8 pm and Wilson: The effect of music in a restaurant101 10 pm. An ANOVA showed that the time subjects filled out the questionnaire when classical music was played differed significantly from all other condi- tions (p = .05). This implies that the type of music being played had an effect on how long patrons remained in the restaurant. SITUATIONAL INFLUENCES ON THE PERCEPTION OF ATMOSPHERE In addition to responding to the characteristics of the restaurant and the music being played, subjects were asked to provide details such as (a) the number of people they were dining with; (b) the reason they were dining out; (c) the amount of alcohol they had consumed; (d) the number of times they had previously dined at the restaurant; and (e) the quality of food and service. Patrons were asked to indicate the number of people they were dining with (including themselves) on a 5-point scale (1 = one to 5 = five or more people). Based on an ANOVA, the relationship between the number of people dining and patrons' responses to the characteristics of the restaurant overall was not significant. However, the number of people dining did yield a significant result for the adjectives 'cerebral' and 'aggressive' (p = .05). In both the jazz and control conditions, mean responses for these adjectives increased incre- mentally with the number of people dining. It was considered that patrons dining with friends or family might respond differently to the environment than patrons attending a business meeting or function. However, based on an ANOVA, subjects' reason for dining out did not produce significant differences in their responses to the restaurant (p = .05). The figures reported in Table 5 suggest that the more upbeat styles of music were associated with a greater number of people consuming three or more drinks. Only a relatively small proportion of subjects (11.1%) consumed three or more drinks on the nights when classical music was played. An ANOVA showed that the number of drinks consumed differed significantly between classical music and every other condition except no music (p = .05). A MANOVA was used to investigate the interaction between the number of 102Psychology of Music 31(1) TA B L E 4 Frequency distribution of subjects (%) according to the time the questionnaire was completed Condition89 pm910 pm1011 pmAfter 11 pm Jazz2.4421.9570.734.88 No music3.3335.0050.0011.67 Classical6.6737.7853.332.22 Easy listening0.0015.2574.5810.17 Control5.6316.9064.6512.82 Popular0.002.8687.1410.00 drinks consumed, subjects' perception of the atmosphere, and musical condi- tion. The amount of alcohol consumed did not significantly affect patrons' responses to atmosphere overall. However, an interactive effect was found between the number of drinks consumed, the type of music played, and sub- jects' responses to 'invigorating' and 'masculine' (p = .001). Specifically, the jazz condition produced significant correlations (p = .001) between the num- ber of drinks consumed and the following adjectives: invigorating, masculine, sophisticated, rebellious and cerebral. In each case, mean ratings increased with the amount of alcohol consumed. Patrons' responses to the characteristics of the restaurant were also con- sidered in terms of the number of times they had been to the restaurant before. While there was no significant correlation overall, the number of times subjects had been to the restaurant was found to correlate with their perception of the restaurant as 'fun'. This relationship was found to be signifi- cant in the jazz and control conditions only (p = .05). Patrons' responses to the quality of service indicated that 92.2 percent of subjects rated the service in the restaurant as 'slightly above average' or high- er. A MANOVA was used to investigate the interaction between musical con- dition, patrons' responses to service, and their responses to atmosphere. Overall, a significant effect (p = .001) was found between patrons' responses to service and their responses to the restaurant. More specifically, significant results were found for the following adjectives: upmarket, sophisticated, happy, fresh, exciting, fun and fashionable. The jazz condition yielded a signif- icant correlation between patrons' responses to service and their responses to 'happy', 'fun' and 'fresh', while popular music generated a significant corre- lation between patrons' responses to service and their responses to 'exciting'. That is, the higher the rating for service, the higher the rating responses for these adjectives. Classical, easy listening, no music and the control condition did not yield significant correlations between patrons' responses to service and the characteristics of the restaurant. It was considered that the time of night subjects filled out the question- naire may have influenced their ratings of the restaurant. Based on an ANOVA, a significant effect was found between the time subjects filled out the questionnaire and their responses to 'invigorating' (p = .001). Inferential statistics showed that rating responses for 'invigorating' were lower when Wilson: The effect of music in a restaurant103 TA B L E 5 Number of alcoholic beverages consumed (%) Easy No. of drinksJazzNo musicClassicallisteningControlPopular None14.610.024.416.915.517.1 1241.563.364.440.742.345.7 3 or more43.926.711.142.442.337.1 responses were made later in the evening for the easy listening and no music conditions only. Two additional factors were explored in relation to patrons' responses to the restaurant: patrons' awareness of the music, and the degree to which they thought the music being played was appropriate. Figure 2 reports the distribution of responses according to patrons' awareness of the music being played. Patrons were less aware of the music in the classical condition, and more aware of the music in the jazz, popular and control conditions. Based on an ANOVA, the relationship between patrons' awareness of the music and their responses to atmosphere was not found to be significant (p = .05). In terms of appropriateness, the control condition generated the most pos- itive response (94.4% considered the music appropriate), followed by jazz (87.8%) and popular music (77.1%). Generally, the more upbeat styles of music were considered to be more appropriate than classical (46.7%) and easy listening (62.7%). Classical music was considered the least appropriate style for the restaurant and, based on an ANOVA, was significantly different from every other condition (p = .01). Easy listening was found to be signifi- cantly different from jazz and the control condition, and the number of 104Psychology of Music 31(1) % 0510 1520 2530 3540 4550 Not at all awareSlightly awareModerately awareVery awareTotally aware Level of awareness JazzClassicalEasy listeningControlPopular F I G U R E 2 Distribution of participants according to awareness of the music. people who thought the music was appropriate was significantly higher in the control condition than the popular condition (p = .01). Subjects' responses to the appropriateness of the music were found to cor- relate significantly (p = .05) with their responses to the restaurant for the adjectives 'downmarket' and 'tacky'. That is, patrons perceived the restaurant to be more downmarket and more tacky when they did not consider the music to be appropriate. Inferential statistics also suggested that subjects who had been to the restaurant before considered classical and easy listening music to be less appropriate than those who were dining at the restaurant for the first time; however, an ANOVA did not yield a significant difference (p = .05). While 53.3 percent of patrons in the classical group considered the music inappropriate, a t-test showed that the amount of money they were prepared to spend did not differ significantly from the 46.7 percent who con- sidered classical music appropriate. Based on an ANOVA, the age of subjects was not found to significantly influence their responses to the appropriateness of the music, the amount of money they were prepared to spend, or their responses to atmosphere or music (p = .05). Similarly, a t-test did not reveal significant differences between males and females according to these factors. An ANOVA was also used to investigate whether there was a relationship between subjects' income and their responses to the appropriateness of the musical style being played. No differences were found between income groups (p = .05). Discussion PERCEPTION OF THE RESTAURANT The results reported in Table 1 indicate that different styles of music, and the absence of music, influenced patrons' perceptions of the restaurant environ- ment. A positive relationship was also found between patrons' perceptions of the restaurant and their perception of the music. These findings are consis- tent with North and Hargreaves (1998). A factor analysis of responses to the restaurant provided evidence that different styles of music (and no music) led to differences in the general perceived characteristics of the restaurant, a finding also consistent with the North and Hargreaves (1998) study. PURCHASING (INTENTIONS AND ACTUAL SALES) Patrons' responses to the maximum amount of money they were prepared to spend on their main meal yielded several differences between conditions. The most extreme differences occurred between no music and each of the five musical conditions. In accord with North and Hargreaves (1998), these results provide evidence that different musical styles, and no music, have the potential to influence patrons' purchase intentions. Due to the fact that the number of people dining in the restaurant fluctu- ated considerably on the same day before, during and after the testing period, Wilson: The effect of music in a restaurant105 it is difficult to assess the influence of music on actual sales. However, classical music was associated with relatively few people remaining in the restaurant after 11 pm, and a greater number of people leaving the restau- rant earlier in the evening. This may be related to the fact that 53.3 percent of subjects considered classical music to be inappropriate. While this did not appear to affect the amount of money patrons in this group indicated they were prepared to spend, the perceived inappropriateness of the music may have influenced the amount of time and perhaps money they actually spent (supporting Radocy and Boyle, 1997). Classical music was also associated with fewer drinks being consumed, and consequently less money spent on alcoholic drinks. INTERACTIONS The results of this study suggest that several other factors were influencing the relationship between music and consumer perceptions. For example, results showed that the more people dining at a table, the more the restaurant was perceived as 'cerebral' and 'aggressive'. This relationship was found to be significant in both the jazz and control conditions. Results also suggested that an interactive effect occurred between the style of music, subjects' perception of the atmosphere, and the amount of alcohol consumed. For example, rat- ings for several adjectives increased with the amount of alcohol consumed when jazz was playing. A significant interactive effect was also found between the style of music played, patrons' responses to the quality of service, and responses to atmos- phere. Lending support to Dube et al. (1995) and North and Hargreaves (1996), these results suggest that music may be positively related to patrons' willingness to interact. Findings also suggested an interaction between music, perceived atmosphere and time of night. That is, the restaurant atmosphere was perceived as less invigorating later in the evening for the easy listening and no music conditions. AWARENESS AND APPROPRIATENESS OF THE MUSIC Results showed that subjects' awareness of the music differed across condi- tions. Overall, subjects were more aware of the music when upbeat styles were played. In contrast, classical and easy listening conditions were associ- ated with more people indicating that they were 'not at all aware' of the music being played. Findings suggest that subjects considered the more upbeat (or stimulative) styles of music to be more appropriate for the restaurant. This may be attrib- uted to the concentration of subjects aged between 20 and 39. For example, the number of patrons indicating that classical and easy listening music were appropriate increased according to age. Conversely, the number of people who considered popular, jazz and the control condition appropriate decreased with age (although not significantly). Subjects who did not consider the 106Psychology of Music 31(1) music to be appropriate perceived the restaurant as more downmarket and more tacky. The music that was considered more appropriate for the restaurant can be described as having a high level of 'fit'. In this environment, the degree of fit between the music and the restaurant appears to have influenced several fac- tors which are directly related to enhancing business and increasing sales. For example, the musical styles with a higher degree of 'fit' were associated with more alcohol being consumed, higher purchase estimates, more positive responses to atmosphere, and more patrons remaining in the restaurant later in the evening. While there was no significant difference between classical music and the more upbeat styles in terms of the amount patrons were pre- pared to spend, the lack of fit between classical music and the restaurant may have caused patrons to spend less time in the restaurant, and consequently less money on food and drinks. IMPLICATIONS The fact that patrons were prepared to spend more when popular, jazz and classical music were played suggests that spending might be increased by music that creates the perception of an upbeat or upmarket environment. This finding corresponds with North and Hargreaves (1998) who found that classical and popular music had a more positive effect on purchase intentions than easy listening and no music. Similarly, Areni and Kim (1993) found that people were prepared to spend more in a wine store when classical music was playing. In the present context, classical music was not considered to be appropriate by a large number of patrons. This suggests that there may be some discrepancy between the amount of money patrons indicated they would be prepared to spend, and the amount of money actually spent. Overall, however, the findings reported here are consistent with a model of the effects of music on purchasing which states that 'the nature of people's responses to music activate contextually relevant knowledge or behavior in other domains' (North and Hargreaves, 1998: 2267). The present findings support Radocy and Boyle's (1997) suggestion that people might be inclined to spend more time and money in a restaurant or store when the music being played is considered appropriate. Similarly, find- ings support MacInnis and Parks' (1991) notion that persuasion is enhanced when the music is appropriate for the context in which it is played. Findings suggest that responses to the characteristics of the restaurant were positively influenced by factors such as the number of people dining at a table, the amount of alcohol consumed, the quality of service, and the number of times patrons had been to the restaurant before. These positive relationships were only found to exist when the more upbeat styles of music were played. Again, these findings support the notion that people's responses to music may activate contextually relevant behavior in other domains. There are several practical applications of the results reported in this Wilson: The effect of music in a restaurant107 study. Firstly, results suggest that music can be used by restaurant and store owners to create a specific atmosphere which will distinguish the envi- ronment from competitors. Findings also suggest that stores which play upbeat or upmarket music may be able to charge higher prices. Overall, the absence of music had the most negative effect on atmosphere and the amount of money patrons were prepared to spend. This research has demonstrated that music can influence the perceived characteristics of the environment in which it is played. In addition, it has provided evidence that different types of music can produce specific atmospheres such as upmarket and upbeat. Importantly, the study demon- strated that music can influence the amount of money patrons are prepared to spend, and perhaps the amount of money they actually spend. Overall, it is clearly evident that music has the potential to influence commercial processes. AC K N OW L E D G E M E N T S I am grateful to APRA (Australasian Performing Right Association) for providing financial support for this study and especially to Dean Ormston for his contribution to this project. Thanks are extended to Andrew Becker from SMA (Satellite Music Australia) for assisting with the musical stimuli used in the study. I would also like to thank the owners of Out Of Africa, Omar Madji and Hassan M'Souli, for their partici- pation in this study, and Meredith Wilson, Associate Professor Gary McPherson and Dr Emery Schubert for their valuable comments on this paper. R E F E R E N C E S Alpert, J.I. and Alpert, M.I. (1990) 'Music influences on mood and purchase intention', Psychology and Marketing 7: 10933. Areni, C.S. and Kim, D. (1993) 'The Influence of Background Music on Shopping Behavior: Classical versus Top-40 Music in a Wine Store', Advances in Consumer Research 20: 33640. Bleich, S., Zillmann, D. and Weaver, J. (1991) 'Enjoyment and Consumption of Defiant Rock Music as a Function of Adolescent Rebelliousness', Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media 35: 35166. Bruner, G.C. (1990) 'Music, Mood and Marketing', Journal of Marketing 54: 94104. Dube, L., Chebat, J.-C. and Morin, S. (1995) 'The Effects of Background Music on Consumers' Desire to Affiliate in BuyerSeller Interactions', Psychology and Marketing 12: 30519. Fried, R. and Berkowitz, L. (1979) 'Music Hath Charms . . . and Can Influence Helpfulness', Journal of Applied Psychology 9: 199208. MacInnis, D.J. and Park, C. (1991) 'The Differential Role of Characteristics of Music on High- and Low-Involvement Consumers' Processing of Ads', Journal of Consumer Research 18: 16173. Mehrabian, A. and Russel, J. (1974) An Approach to Environmental Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Milliman, R.E. (1982) 'Using Background Music to Affect the Behavior of Supermarket Shoppers', Journal of Marketing 46(3): 8691. 108Psychology of Music 31(1) Milliman, R.E. (1986) 'The Influence of Background Music on the Behavior of Restaurant Patrons', Journal of Consumer Research 13: 2869. North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (1996) 'The Effects of Music on Responses to a Dining Area', Journal of Environmental Psychology 16: 5564. North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (1998) 'The Effect of Music on Atmosphere and Purchase Intentions in a Cafeteria', Journal of Applied Psychology 28(4): 225473. Radocy, R.E. and Boyle, J.D. (1997) Psychological Foundations of Musical Behavior, 3rd edn. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Robally, T.C., McGreevy, C., Rongo, R.R., Schwantes, M.L., Steger, P.J., Wininger, M.A. and Gardner, E.B. (1985) 'The Effect of Music on Eating Behavior', Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 23: 2212. Stack, S. and Gunlach, J. (1992) 'The Effect of Country Music on Suicide', Social Forces 71: 21118. Standley, J. (1995) 'Music as a Therapeutic Intervention in Medical and Dental Treatment: Research and Clinical Applications', in T. Wigram, B. Saperstone and R. West (eds) The Art and Science of Music Therapy. Langhorne, PA: Harwood Academic/Gordon and Breach. Stratton, V.N. and Zalanowski, A. (1984) 'The Effect of Background Music on Verbal Interaction of Groups', Journal of Music Therapy 21: 1626. Yalch, R. and Spangenberg, E. (1990) 'Effects of Store Music on Shopping Behavior', Journal of Services Marketing 4: 319. Zillmann, D. and Bhatia, A. (1989) 'Effects of Associating with Musical Genres on Heterosexual Attraction', Communication Research 16: 26388. Zullow, H.M. (1991) 'Pessimistic Rumination in Popular Songs and News Magazines Predict Economic Recession via Decreased Consumer Optimism and Spending', Journal of Economic Psychology 12: 50126. Appendix: musical stimuli JAZZ A Night in Tunisia: Clifford Brown A Stanley Steamer: Earl Hines A Taste of Honey: Charlie Bird Be Yourself: Kenny Burrell Better Get It in Your Soul: Charles Mingus Black Coffee: Earl Hines Blues for ZW: Leroy Jones Deodato: Bangles and Beads For All We Know: Dave Brubeck Gone with the Wind: Dave Brubeck Indiana (Back Home Again in Indiana): Milt Hinton Love for Sale: Miles Davis Midnight at the Oasis: Hubert Laws Miles: Miles Davis My Funny Valentine: Chet Baker and Gerry Mulligan Rumble in the Jungle: Max Roach Something Else: Miles DavisWilson: The effect of music in a restaurant109 Take Five: Dave Brubeck That Beautiful Sadness: Mark Isham The New Message: Art Blakey and the Jazz Messengers This Can't Be Love: Ellis Marsalis What Now My Love: Lou Donaldson Yesterday's Dreams: Freddie Hubbard POPULAR All in Your Hands: Lamb Alone: Ben Harper Appletree: Erika Badu At the River: Groove Armada Blow Up the Pokies: The Whitlams Buses and Trains: Bachelor Girl Crash and Burn: Savage Garden Don't Call Me Baby: Madison Avenue Even When I'm Sleeping: Leonardo's Bride Everybody Here Wants You: Jeff Buckley Freshmint: Regurgitator Friendly Pressure: Jhelisa Glockenpop: Spiderbait Half the Man: Jamiroquai I Think I'm in Love with You: Jessica Simpson I Try: Macy Gray It Ain't Over 'Til It's Over: Lenny Kravitz Karmacoma: Massive Attack Keep Me Lifted: Spearhead Lucky Star: Alex Lloyd Nothing Much Happens: Ben Lee One More Time: Groove Terminator Revenge on the Number: Portishead Shine: Vanessa Amorosi Spinning Around: Kylie Minogue Still a Friend of Mine: Incognito Sunshine on a Rainy Day: Christine Anu Thank You (For Loving Me at My Worst): The Whitlams Tropicalia: Beck Try Whistling This: Neil Finn Weir: Killing Heidi Why Does My Heart Feel So Bad?: Moby CLASSICAL Bach: Air on a G String Bach: Brandenberg Concerto No. 1 in E, RV 269 (II: Largo) 110Psychology of Music 31(1) Beethoven: Moonlight Sonata Beethoven: Symphony No. 4 in B flat major, Op. 60 (II: Adagio) Debussy: Prelude to the Afternoon of a Faun Elgar: Serenade for Strings, Op. 20 (II. Largetto) Grieg: Morning (from Peer Gynt) Handel: Water Music: Suite (II) Hummel: Piano Concerto in A minor, Op. 85 (II: Larghetto) Mahler: Symphony No. 5 in C sharp minor (IV: Adagietto) Mendelssohn: Violin Concerto No. 2 in E minor, Op. 64 (Andante) Mozart: Piano Concerto No. 19 KV 459 (Allegretto) Mozart: Piano Concerto No. 20 KV 466 (Romance) Mozart: Piano Concerto No. 21 in C major, K467 (II: Andante) Rachmaninov: Concerto for Piano and Orchestra No. 2 in C minor, Op. 18 (II: Adagio Sostenuto) Schubert: Symphony No. 5 in B flat Major, D485 (II: Andante con moto) Sibelius: Andante Festivo Tchaikovsky: Piano Concerto No. 1 Vivaldi: The Four Seasons (Spring) EASY LISTENING Adeline: Richard Clayderman All At Once: Fairfield All I Have to Do is Dream: John Fox All My Life: Nick Ingman Always On My Mind: Pan Pipes Annie's Song: Johnny Pearson Are You Free: Images Around Every Corner: Grant Geissman Catalina: Fernando Jonas Cinema Paradiso: Roger Woodward Drivetime: Tommy Emmanuel Endless Love: Richard Tucker Everything I Do I Do It For You: Guitar Moods Forever Love: Gary Barlow Greensleeves: London Symphony Orchestra Lara's Theme: Richard Clayderman Memory: James Galway Never on Sunday: The 101 Strings Orchestra Once Again: Earl Klugh Sadness: Roger Woodward Saving All My Love For You: Paul Mauriat Stay Another Day: Pierre Belmonde Stranger on the Shore: Kenny G The Greatest Love of All: Starsound OrchestraWilson: The effect of music in a restaurant111 The Summer Knows: Glenn Long and His Orchestra Thinking of You: Oscar Lopez Tonight I Celebrate My Love For You: Hill/Wiltstchinski Guitar Your Song: Samantha Blue S T E P H A N I E W I L S O N obtained her PhD at the School of Music and Music Education at the University of New South Wales in 1999. Her doctoral thesis investigated pattern perception and temporality in the music of Steve Reich. Since this time, she has con- ducted research in the area of music psychology for the Australasian Performing Right Association and has been involved in both music teaching and performance. Currently, she is working as Project Development Co-ordinator for the Office of the Pro-Vice-Chancellor (Education) at the University of New South Wales. This role involves supporting a range of learning and teaching initiatives and projects across the University. Address: Learning and Teaching@UNSW, Office ofthe Pro-Vice-Chancellor (Education), University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia 2052. [email: stephaniewilson@unsw.edu.au] 112Psychology of Music 31(1) </meta-value>
</custom-meta>
</custom-meta-wrap>
</article-meta>
</front>
<back>
<ref-list>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Alpert, J.I.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Alpert, M.I.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1990</year>
)
<article-title>‘Music influences on mood and purchase intention’</article-title>
,
<source>Psychology and Marketing</source>
<volume>7</volume>
:
<fpage>109</fpage>
-
<lpage>133</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Areni, C.S.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Kim, D.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1993</year>
)
<article-title>‘The Influence of Background Music on Shopping Behavior: Classical versus Top-40 Music in a Wine Store’</article-title>
,
<source>Advances in Consumer Research</source>
<volume>20</volume>
:
<fpage>336</fpage>
-
<lpage>340</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Bleich, S.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Zillmann, D.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Weaver, J.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1991</year>
)
<article-title>‘Enjoyment and Consumption of Defiant Rock Music as a Function of Adolescent Rebelliousness’</article-title>
,
<source>Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media</source>
<volume>35</volume>
:
<fpage>351</fpage>
-
<lpage>366</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Bruner, G.C.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1990</year>
)
<article-title>‘Music, Mood and Marketing’</article-title>
,
<source>Journal of Marketing</source>
<volume>54</volume>
:
<fpage>94</fpage>
-
<lpage>104</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Dube, L.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Chebat, J.-C.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Morin, S.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1995</year>
)
<article-title>‘The Effects of Background Music on Consumers’ Desire to Affiliate in Buyer-Seller Interactions’</article-title>
,
<source>Psychology and Marketing</source>
<volume>12</volume>
:
<fpage>305</fpage>
-
<lpage>319</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Fried, R.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Berkowitz, L.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1979</year>
)
<article-title>‘Music Hath Charms... and Can Influence Helpfulness’</article-title>
,
<source>Journal of Applied Psychology</source>
<volume>9</volume>
:
<fpage>199</fpage>
-
<lpage>208</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>MacInnis, D.J.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Park, C.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1991</year>
)
<article-title>‘The Differential Role of Characteristics of Music on High-and Low-Involvement Consumers’ Processing of Ads’</article-title>
,
<source>Journal of Consumer Research</source>
<volume>18</volume>
:
<fpage>161</fpage>
-
<lpage>173</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Mehrabian, A.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Russel, J.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1974</year>
)
<source>An Approach to Environmental Psychology</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>MIT Press</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Milliman, R.E.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1982</year>
)
<article-title>‘Using Background Music to Affect the Behavior of Supermarket Shoppers’</article-title>
,
<source>Journal of Marketing</source>
<volume>46</volume>
(
<issue>3</issue>
):
<fpage>86</fpage>
-
<lpage>91</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Milliman, R.E.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1986</year>
)
<article-title>‘The Influence of Background Music on the Behavior of Restaurant Patrons’</article-title>
,
<source>Journal of Consumer Research</source>
<volume>13</volume>
:
<fpage>286</fpage>
-
<lpage>289</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>North, A.C.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Hargreaves, D.J.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1996</year>
)
<article-title>‘The Effects of Music on Responses to a Dining Area’</article-title>
,
<source>Journal of Environmental Psychology</source>
<volume>16</volume>
:
<fpage>55</fpage>
-
<lpage>64</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>North, A.C.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Hargreaves, D.J.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1998</year>
)
<article-title>‘The Effect of Music on Atmosphere and Purchase Intentions in a Cafeteria’</article-title>
,
<source>Journal of Applied Psychology</source>
<volume>28</volume>
(
<issue>4</issue>
):
<fpage>2254</fpage>
-
<lpage>2273</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Radocy, R.E.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Boyle, J.D.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1997</year>
)
<source>Psychological Foundations of Musical Behavior</source>
,
<edition>3rd edn.</edition>
<publisher-loc>Springfield, IL</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Charles C. Thomas</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Robally, T.C.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>McGreevy, C.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Rongo, R.R.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Schwantes, M.L.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Steger, P.J.</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Wininger, M.A.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Gardner, E.B.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1985</year>
)
<article-title>‘The Effect of Music on Eating Behavior’</article-title>
,
<source>Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society</source>
<volume>23</volume>
:
<fpage>221</fpage>
-
<lpage>222</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Stack, S.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Gunlach, J.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1992</year>
)
<article-title>‘The Effect of Country Music on Suicide’</article-title>
,
<source>Social Forces</source>
<volume>71</volume>
:
<fpage>211</fpage>
-
<lpage>218</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="book" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Standley, J.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1995</year>
)
<article-title>‘Music as a Therapeutic Intervention in Medical and Dental Treatment: Research and Clinical Applications’</article-title>
, in
<name name-style="western">
<surname>T. Wigram</surname>
</name>
,
<name name-style="western">
<surname>B. Saperstone</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>R. West</surname>
</name>
(eds)
<source>The Art and Science of Music Therapy</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Langhorne, PA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Harwood Academic/Gordon and Breach</publisher-name>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Stratton, V.N.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Zalanowski, A.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1984</year>
)
<article-title>‘The Effect of Background Music on Verbal Interaction of Groups’</article-title>
,
<source>Journal of Music Therapy</source>
<volume>21</volume>
:
<fpage>16</fpage>
-
<lpage>26</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Yalch, R.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Spangenberg, E.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1990</year>
)
<article-title>‘Effects of Store Music on Shopping Behavior’</article-title>
,
<source>Journal of Services Marketing</source>
<volume>4</volume>
:
<fpage>31</fpage>
-
<lpage>39</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Zillmann, D.</surname>
</name>
and
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Bhatia, A.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1989</year>
)
<article-title>‘Effects of Associating with Musical Genres on Heterosexual Attraction’</article-title>
,
<source>Communication Research</source>
<volume>16</volume>
:
<fpage>263</fpage>
-
<lpage>288</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
<ref>
<citation citation-type="journal" xlink:type="simple">
<name name-style="western">
<surname>Zullow, H.M.</surname>
</name>
(
<year>1991</year>
)
<article-title>‘Pessimistic Rumination in Popular Songs and News Magazines Predict Economic Recession via Decreased Consumer Optimism and Spending’</article-title>
,
<source>Journal of Economic Psychology</source>
<volume>12</volume>
:
<fpage>501</fpage>
-
<lpage>526</lpage>
.</citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</article>
</istex:document>
</istex:metadataXml>
<mods version="3.6">
<titleInfo lang="en">
<title>The Effect of Music on Perceived Atmosphere and Purchase Intentions in a Restaurant</title>
</titleInfo>
<titleInfo type="alternative" lang="en" contentType="CDATA">
<title>The Effect of Music on Perceived Atmosphere and Purchase Intentions in a Restaurant</title>
</titleInfo>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Stephanie</namePart>
<namePart type="family">Wilson</namePart>
<affiliation>University of New South Wales,</affiliation>
<affiliation>E-mail: stephaniewilson@unsw.edu.au</affiliation>
</name>
<typeOfResource>text</typeOfResource>
<genre type="research-article" displayLabel="research-article" authority="ISTEX" authorityURI="https://content-type.data.istex.fr" valueURI="https://content-type.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/XTP-1JC4F85T-7">research-article</genre>
<originInfo>
<publisher>Sage Publications</publisher>
<place>
<placeTerm type="text">Sage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA</placeTerm>
</place>
<dateIssued encoding="w3cdtf">2003-01</dateIssued>
<copyrightDate encoding="w3cdtf">2003</copyrightDate>
</originInfo>
<language>
<languageTerm type="code" authority="iso639-2b">eng</languageTerm>
<languageTerm type="code" authority="rfc3066">en</languageTerm>
</language>
<abstract lang="en">Extending research by North and Hargreaves (1998), this study investigated the effect of music on perceived atmosphere and purchase intentions in a restaurant. Four musical styles (jazz, popular, easy listening and classical) and no music were played in a restaurant over two consecutive weeks. Results indicated that different types of music had different effects on perceived atmosphere and the amount patrons were prepared to spend. Classical, jazz and popular music were associated with patrons being prepared to spend the most on their main meal. This value was found to be significantly lower in the absence of music and when easy listening was played. There was some evidence that the type of music also had an effect on the amount of money patrons actually spent in the restaurant. Overall, the study contributes to the development of a model that seeks to account for the relationship between music and consumer behaviour.</abstract>
<subject>
<genre>keywords</genre>
<topic>consumer behaviour</topic>
<topic>listening</topic>
<topic>music psychology</topic>
<topic>perceived atmosphere</topic>
<topic>restaurant</topic>
</subject>
<relatedItem type="host">
<titleInfo>
<title>Psychology of music</title>
</titleInfo>
<genre type="journal" authority="ISTEX" authorityURI="https://publication-type.data.istex.fr" valueURI="https://publication-type.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/JMC-0GLKJH51-B">journal</genre>
<identifier type="ISSN">0305-7356</identifier>
<identifier type="eISSN">1741-3087</identifier>
<identifier type="PublisherID">POM</identifier>
<identifier type="PublisherID-hwp">sppom</identifier>
<part>
<date>2003</date>
<detail type="volume">
<caption>vol.</caption>
<number>31</number>
</detail>
<detail type="issue">
<caption>no.</caption>
<number>1</number>
</detail>
<extent unit="pages">
<start>93</start>
<end>112</end>
</extent>
</part>
</relatedItem>
<identifier type="istex">177FF7A01B7BF67307C70675E6A526DF9196E178</identifier>
<identifier type="ark">ark:/67375/M70-1BLQJLV5-6</identifier>
<identifier type="DOI">10.1177/0305735603031001327</identifier>
<identifier type="ArticleID">10.1177_0305735603031001327</identifier>
<recordInfo>
<recordContentSource authority="ISTEX" authorityURI="https://loaded-corpus.data.istex.fr" valueURI="https://loaded-corpus.data.istex.fr/ark:/67375/XBH-0J1N7DQT-B">sage</recordContentSource>
</recordInfo>
</mods>
<json:item>
<extension>json</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>application/json</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/177FF7A01B7BF67307C70675E6A526DF9196E178/metadata/json</uri>
</json:item>
</metadata>
<serie></serie>
</istex>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Wicri/Musique/explor/DebussyV1/Data/Istex/Corpus
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 002417 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Istex/Corpus/biblio.hfd -nk 002417 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Wicri/Musique
   |area=    DebussyV1
   |flux=    Istex
   |étape=   Corpus
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     ISTEX:177FF7A01B7BF67307C70675E6A526DF9196E178
   |texte=   The Effect of Music on Perceived Atmosphere and Purchase Intentions in a Restaurant
}}

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.33.
Data generation: Tue Sep 25 16:34:07 2018. Site generation: Mon Mar 11 10:31:28 2024