Serveur d'exploration sur l'Université de Trèves

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

On the fate of distractor representations.

Identifieur interne : 000464 ( PubMed/Curation ); précédent : 000463; suivant : 000465

On the fate of distractor representations.

Auteurs : Christian Frings [Allemagne] ; Dirk Wentura ; Peter Wühr

Source :

RBID : pubmed:22428679

English descriptors

Abstract

Research on the topic of distractor inhibition has used different empirical approaches to study how the human mind selects relevant information from the environment, and the results are controversially discussed. One key question that typically arises is how selection deals with the irrelevant information. We used a new selection task, in which participants sometimes had to respond to the distractors instead of the target. Importantly, we varied the time interval between stimuli onset and the cue that signaled participants to respond to the distractors. We analyzed RTs and error rates from responses to distractors as a function of how long the target had been processed (and the distractor ignored) before the cue required a response to the distractor (i.e., stimulus-cue SOA). The data are compatible with selection models assuming that distractor stimuli are initially activated and then deactivated. Thus, we argue for selection models assuming top down deactivation of distractor representations that work in parallel with top down activation of target representations.

DOI: 10.1037/a0027781
PubMed: 22428679

Links toward previous steps (curation, corpus...)


Links to Exploration step

pubmed:22428679

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI>
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">On the fate of distractor representations.</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Frings, Christian" sort="Frings, Christian" uniqKey="Frings C" first="Christian" last="Frings">Christian Frings</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:affiliation>Department of Cognitive Psychology, University of Trier, Trier, Germany. chfrings@uni-trier.de</nlm:affiliation>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Department of Cognitive Psychology, University of Trier, Trier</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wentura, Dirk" sort="Wentura, Dirk" uniqKey="Wentura D" first="Dirk" last="Wentura">Dirk Wentura</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wuhr, Peter" sort="Wuhr, Peter" uniqKey="Wuhr P" first="Peter" last="Wühr">Peter Wühr</name>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">PubMed</idno>
<date when="2012">2012</date>
<idno type="RBID">pubmed:22428679</idno>
<idno type="pmid">22428679</idno>
<idno type="doi">10.1037/a0027781</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/PubMed/Corpus">000464</idno>
<idno type="wicri:explorRef" wicri:stream="PubMed" wicri:step="Corpus" wicri:corpus="PubMed">000464</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/PubMed/Curation">000464</idno>
<idno type="wicri:explorRef" wicri:stream="PubMed" wicri:step="Curation">000464</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title xml:lang="en">On the fate of distractor representations.</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Frings, Christian" sort="Frings, Christian" uniqKey="Frings C" first="Christian" last="Frings">Christian Frings</name>
<affiliation wicri:level="1">
<nlm:affiliation>Department of Cognitive Psychology, University of Trier, Trier, Germany. chfrings@uni-trier.de</nlm:affiliation>
<country xml:lang="fr">Allemagne</country>
<wicri:regionArea>Department of Cognitive Psychology, University of Trier, Trier</wicri:regionArea>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wentura, Dirk" sort="Wentura, Dirk" uniqKey="Wentura D" first="Dirk" last="Wentura">Dirk Wentura</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wuhr, Peter" sort="Wuhr, Peter" uniqKey="Wuhr P" first="Peter" last="Wühr">Peter Wühr</name>
</author>
</analytic>
<series>
<title level="j">Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance</title>
<idno type="eISSN">1939-1277</idno>
<imprint>
<date when="2012" type="published">2012</date>
</imprint>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass>
<keywords scheme="KwdEn" xml:lang="en">
<term>Adult</term>
<term>Attention</term>
<term>Humans</term>
<term>Models, Psychological</term>
<term>Photic Stimulation</term>
<term>Reaction Time</term>
<term>Visual Perception</term>
<term>Young Adult</term>
</keywords>
<keywords scheme="MESH" xml:lang="en">
<term>Adult</term>
<term>Attention</term>
<term>Humans</term>
<term>Models, Psychological</term>
<term>Photic Stimulation</term>
<term>Reaction Time</term>
<term>Visual Perception</term>
<term>Young Adult</term>
</keywords>
</textClass>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">Research on the topic of distractor inhibition has used different empirical approaches to study how the human mind selects relevant information from the environment, and the results are controversially discussed. One key question that typically arises is how selection deals with the irrelevant information. We used a new selection task, in which participants sometimes had to respond to the distractors instead of the target. Importantly, we varied the time interval between stimuli onset and the cue that signaled participants to respond to the distractors. We analyzed RTs and error rates from responses to distractors as a function of how long the target had been processed (and the distractor ignored) before the cue required a response to the distractor (i.e., stimulus-cue SOA). The data are compatible with selection models assuming that distractor stimuli are initially activated and then deactivated. Thus, we argue for selection models assuming top down deactivation of distractor representations that work in parallel with top down activation of target representations.</div>
</front>
</TEI>
<pubmed>
<MedlineCitation Status="MEDLINE" Owner="NLM">
<PMID Version="1">22428679</PMID>
<DateCreated>
<Year>2012</Year>
<Month>06</Month>
<Day>05</Day>
</DateCreated>
<DateCompleted>
<Year>2012</Year>
<Month>10</Month>
<Day>15</Day>
</DateCompleted>
<DateRevised>
<Year>2012</Year>
<Month>06</Month>
<Day>05</Day>
</DateRevised>
<Article PubModel="Print-Electronic">
<Journal>
<ISSN IssnType="Electronic">1939-1277</ISSN>
<JournalIssue CitedMedium="Internet">
<Volume>38</Volume>
<Issue>3</Issue>
<PubDate>
<Year>2012</Year>
<Month>Jun</Month>
</PubDate>
</JournalIssue>
<Title>Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance</Title>
<ISOAbbreviation>J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform</ISOAbbreviation>
</Journal>
<ArticleTitle>On the fate of distractor representations.</ArticleTitle>
<Pagination>
<MedlinePgn>570-5</MedlinePgn>
</Pagination>
<ELocationID EIdType="doi" ValidYN="Y">10.1037/a0027781</ELocationID>
<Abstract>
<AbstractText>Research on the topic of distractor inhibition has used different empirical approaches to study how the human mind selects relevant information from the environment, and the results are controversially discussed. One key question that typically arises is how selection deals with the irrelevant information. We used a new selection task, in which participants sometimes had to respond to the distractors instead of the target. Importantly, we varied the time interval between stimuli onset and the cue that signaled participants to respond to the distractors. We analyzed RTs and error rates from responses to distractors as a function of how long the target had been processed (and the distractor ignored) before the cue required a response to the distractor (i.e., stimulus-cue SOA). The data are compatible with selection models assuming that distractor stimuli are initially activated and then deactivated. Thus, we argue for selection models assuming top down deactivation of distractor representations that work in parallel with top down activation of target representations.</AbstractText>
</Abstract>
<AuthorList CompleteYN="Y">
<Author ValidYN="Y">
<LastName>Frings</LastName>
<ForeName>Christian</ForeName>
<Initials>C</Initials>
<AffiliationInfo>
<Affiliation>Department of Cognitive Psychology, University of Trier, Trier, Germany. chfrings@uni-trier.de</Affiliation>
</AffiliationInfo>
</Author>
<Author ValidYN="Y">
<LastName>Wentura</LastName>
<ForeName>Dirk</ForeName>
<Initials>D</Initials>
</Author>
<Author ValidYN="Y">
<LastName>Wühr</LastName>
<ForeName>Peter</ForeName>
<Initials>P</Initials>
</Author>
</AuthorList>
<Language>eng</Language>
<PublicationTypeList>
<PublicationType UI="D016428">Journal Article</PublicationType>
</PublicationTypeList>
<ArticleDate DateType="Electronic">
<Year>2012</Year>
<Month>03</Month>
<Day>19</Day>
</ArticleDate>
</Article>
<MedlineJournalInfo>
<Country>United States</Country>
<MedlineTA>J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform</MedlineTA>
<NlmUniqueID>7502589</NlmUniqueID>
<ISSNLinking>0096-1523</ISSNLinking>
</MedlineJournalInfo>
<CitationSubset>IM</CitationSubset>
<MeshHeadingList>
<MeshHeading>
<DescriptorName UI="D000328" MajorTopicYN="N">Adult</DescriptorName>
</MeshHeading>
<MeshHeading>
<DescriptorName UI="D001288" MajorTopicYN="Y">Attention</DescriptorName>
</MeshHeading>
<MeshHeading>
<DescriptorName UI="D006801" MajorTopicYN="N">Humans</DescriptorName>
</MeshHeading>
<MeshHeading>
<DescriptorName UI="D008960" MajorTopicYN="N">Models, Psychological</DescriptorName>
</MeshHeading>
<MeshHeading>
<DescriptorName UI="D010775" MajorTopicYN="N">Photic Stimulation</DescriptorName>
</MeshHeading>
<MeshHeading>
<DescriptorName UI="D011930" MajorTopicYN="N">Reaction Time</DescriptorName>
</MeshHeading>
<MeshHeading>
<DescriptorName UI="D014796" MajorTopicYN="Y">Visual Perception</DescriptorName>
</MeshHeading>
<MeshHeading>
<DescriptorName UI="D055815" MajorTopicYN="N">Young Adult</DescriptorName>
</MeshHeading>
</MeshHeadingList>
</MedlineCitation>
<PubmedData>
<History>
<PubMedPubDate PubStatus="entrez">
<Year>2012</Year>
<Month>3</Month>
<Day>21</Day>
<Hour>6</Hour>
<Minute>0</Minute>
</PubMedPubDate>
<PubMedPubDate PubStatus="pubmed">
<Year>2012</Year>
<Month>3</Month>
<Day>21</Day>
<Hour>6</Hour>
<Minute>0</Minute>
</PubMedPubDate>
<PubMedPubDate PubStatus="medline">
<Year>2012</Year>
<Month>10</Month>
<Day>16</Day>
<Hour>6</Hour>
<Minute>0</Minute>
</PubMedPubDate>
</History>
<PublicationStatus>ppublish</PublicationStatus>
<ArticleIdList>
<ArticleId IdType="pubmed">22428679</ArticleId>
<ArticleId IdType="pii">2012-07326-001</ArticleId>
<ArticleId IdType="doi">10.1037/a0027781</ArticleId>
</ArticleIdList>
</PubmedData>
</pubmed>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Wicri/Rhénanie/explor/UnivTrevesV1/Data/PubMed/Curation
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 000464 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/PubMed/Curation/biblio.hfd -nk 000464 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Wicri/Rhénanie
   |area=    UnivTrevesV1
   |flux=    PubMed
   |étape=   Curation
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     pubmed:22428679
   |texte=   On the fate of distractor representations.
}}

Pour générer des pages wiki

HfdIndexSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/PubMed/Curation/RBID.i   -Sk "pubmed:22428679" \
       | HfdSelect -Kh $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/PubMed/Curation/biblio.hfd   \
       | NlmPubMed2Wicri -a UnivTrevesV1 

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.31.
Data generation: Sat Jul 22 16:29:01 2017. Site generation: Wed Feb 28 14:55:37 2024