Serveur d'exploration sur l'Université de Trèves

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

Position effects in chunked linear orders

Identifieur interne : 001A34 ( Istex/Corpus ); précédent : 001A33; suivant : 001A35

Position effects in chunked linear orders

Auteurs : Rüdiger F. Pohl

Source :

RBID : ISTEX:63A7AD33CD989C3FA69EA2214D8D352CDEBA2A7A

Abstract

Summary: When someone is asked which of two items from a linearly ordered set of items comes first in that order, reaction time decreases with increasing distance between the two items in the given order. This is known as thedistance effect, a very robust finding in studies on linear orders. Surprisingly, investigations onchunked linear orders show unexpected diverse results. For example, reaction times todifferent probes (i. e., probes with items from different chunks) are sometimes longer than those tosame probes (i. e., probes with items from the same chunk). But if subjects' latency depends on the discriminability of chunk labels vs. exact position information within a chunk,different probes should always yield faster order judgments thansame probes. Two experiments investigated whether position effects, including both end-of-list and end-ofchunk effects, can account for the unexpected finding that between-chunk probes are sometimes answered more slowly than within-chunk probes. Moreover, the linea-rorder paradigm was extended to complex action sequences with more than two chunks, using original cooking recipes as materials. The results showed thatchunk-position effects play a crucial role in comparative judgments. Probes from the first and the last chunk were answered faster than those from a middle chunk.Border items (i. e., items next to a chunk boundary) did not produce a significant effect. However, chunking affected subjects' reaction time, as was indicated by the absence of a distance effect fordifferent probes. Besides, action sequences that are ordered in time, as well as concept lists used previously, can be considered linear orders, thus supporting the view that linear orders reflect a basic representation schema of human memory.

Url:
DOI: 10.1007/BF00867214

Links to Exploration step

ISTEX:63A7AD33CD989C3FA69EA2214D8D352CDEBA2A7A

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI wicri:istexFullTextTei="biblStruct">
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">Position effects in chunked linear orders</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pohl, Rudiger F" sort="Pohl, Rudiger F" uniqKey="Pohl R" first="Rüdiger F." last="Pohl">Rüdiger F. Pohl</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>FB 1 - Psychologie, Universität Trier, Postfach 3825, D-5500, Trier, Germany</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">ISTEX</idno>
<idno type="RBID">ISTEX:63A7AD33CD989C3FA69EA2214D8D352CDEBA2A7A</idno>
<date when="1990" year="1990">1990</date>
<idno type="doi">10.1007/BF00867214</idno>
<idno type="url">https://api.istex.fr/document/63A7AD33CD989C3FA69EA2214D8D352CDEBA2A7A/fulltext/pdf</idno>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Istex/Corpus">001A34</idno>
<idno type="wicri:explorRef" wicri:stream="Istex" wicri:step="Corpus" wicri:corpus="ISTEX">001A34</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">Position effects in chunked linear orders</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pohl, Rudiger F" sort="Pohl, Rudiger F" uniqKey="Pohl R" first="Rüdiger F." last="Pohl">Rüdiger F. Pohl</name>
<affiliation>
<mods:affiliation>FB 1 - Psychologie, Universität Trier, Postfach 3825, D-5500, Trier, Germany</mods:affiliation>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<monogr></monogr>
<series>
<title level="j">Psychological Research</title>
<title level="j" type="sub">An International Journal of Perception, Attention, Memory and Action</title>
<title level="j" type="abbrev">Psychol. Res</title>
<idno type="ISSN">0340-0727</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1430-2772</idno>
<imprint>
<publisher>Springer-Verlag</publisher>
<pubPlace>Berlin/Heidelberg</pubPlace>
<date type="published" when="1990-03-01">1990-03-01</date>
<biblScope unit="volume">52</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="issue">1</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" from="68">68</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" to="75">75</biblScope>
</imprint>
<idno type="ISSN">0340-0727</idno>
</series>
<idno type="istex">63A7AD33CD989C3FA69EA2214D8D352CDEBA2A7A</idno>
<idno type="DOI">10.1007/BF00867214</idno>
<idno type="ArticleID">BF00867214</idno>
<idno type="ArticleID">Art11</idno>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
<seriesStmt>
<idno type="ISSN">0340-0727</idno>
</seriesStmt>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
<langUsage>
<language ident="en">en</language>
</langUsage>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">Summary: When someone is asked which of two items from a linearly ordered set of items comes first in that order, reaction time decreases with increasing distance between the two items in the given order. This is known as thedistance effect, a very robust finding in studies on linear orders. Surprisingly, investigations onchunked linear orders show unexpected diverse results. For example, reaction times todifferent probes (i. e., probes with items from different chunks) are sometimes longer than those tosame probes (i. e., probes with items from the same chunk). But if subjects' latency depends on the discriminability of chunk labels vs. exact position information within a chunk,different probes should always yield faster order judgments thansame probes. Two experiments investigated whether position effects, including both end-of-list and end-ofchunk effects, can account for the unexpected finding that between-chunk probes are sometimes answered more slowly than within-chunk probes. Moreover, the linea-rorder paradigm was extended to complex action sequences with more than two chunks, using original cooking recipes as materials. The results showed thatchunk-position effects play a crucial role in comparative judgments. Probes from the first and the last chunk were answered faster than those from a middle chunk.Border items (i. e., items next to a chunk boundary) did not produce a significant effect. However, chunking affected subjects' reaction time, as was indicated by the absence of a distance effect fordifferent probes. Besides, action sequences that are ordered in time, as well as concept lists used previously, can be considered linear orders, thus supporting the view that linear orders reflect a basic representation schema of human memory.</div>
</front>
</TEI>
<istex>
<corpusName>springer</corpusName>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>Rüdiger F. Pohl</name>
<affiliations>
<json:string>FB 1 - Psychologie, Universität Trier, Postfach 3825, D-5500, Trier, Germany</json:string>
</affiliations>
</json:item>
</author>
<articleId>
<json:string>BF00867214</json:string>
<json:string>Art11</json:string>
</articleId>
<language>
<json:string>eng</json:string>
</language>
<originalGenre>
<json:string>OriginalPaper</json:string>
</originalGenre>
<abstract>Summary: When someone is asked which of two items from a linearly ordered set of items comes first in that order, reaction time decreases with increasing distance between the two items in the given order. This is known as thedistance effect, a very robust finding in studies on linear orders. Surprisingly, investigations onchunked linear orders show unexpected diverse results. For example, reaction times todifferent probes (i. e., probes with items from different chunks) are sometimes longer than those tosame probes (i. e., probes with items from the same chunk). But if subjects' latency depends on the discriminability of chunk labels vs. exact position information within a chunk,different probes should always yield faster order judgments thansame probes. Two experiments investigated whether position effects, including both end-of-list and end-ofchunk effects, can account for the unexpected finding that between-chunk probes are sometimes answered more slowly than within-chunk probes. Moreover, the linea-rorder paradigm was extended to complex action sequences with more than two chunks, using original cooking recipes as materials. The results showed thatchunk-position effects play a crucial role in comparative judgments. Probes from the first and the last chunk were answered faster than those from a middle chunk.Border items (i. e., items next to a chunk boundary) did not produce a significant effect. However, chunking affected subjects' reaction time, as was indicated by the absence of a distance effect fordifferent probes. Besides, action sequences that are ordered in time, as well as concept lists used previously, can be considered linear orders, thus supporting the view that linear orders reflect a basic representation schema of human memory.</abstract>
<qualityIndicators>
<score>8</score>
<pdfVersion>1.3</pdfVersion>
<pdfPageSize>598 x 785 pts</pdfPageSize>
<refBibsNative>false</refBibsNative>
<keywordCount>0</keywordCount>
<abstractCharCount>1774</abstractCharCount>
<pdfWordCount>5989</pdfWordCount>
<pdfCharCount>35144</pdfCharCount>
<pdfPageCount>8</pdfPageCount>
<abstractWordCount>263</abstractWordCount>
</qualityIndicators>
<title>Position effects in chunked linear orders</title>
<refBibs>
<json:item>
<host>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>J,R Anderson</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<title>The architecture of cognition</title>
<publicationDate>1983</publicationDate>
</host>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>W,P Banks</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<pages>
<last>160</last>
<first>101</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>The psychology of learning and motivation</title>
<publicationDate>1977</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Encoding and processing of symbolic information in comparative judgments</title>
<publicationDate>1977</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>W,P Banks</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>H White</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>R Mermelstein</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>8</volume>
<pages>
<last>630</last>
<first>623</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Memory & Cognition</title>
<publicationDate>1980</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Position effects in comparative judgments of serial order: List structure vs. differential strength</title>
<publicationDate>1980</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>G,H Bower</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<pages>
<last>201</last>
<first>175</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Adaptation-level theory</title>
<publicationDate>1971</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Adaptation-level coding of stimuli and serial position effects</title>
<publicationDate>1971</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>D,A Broadbent</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<pages>
<last>18</last>
<first>3</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Studies in long-term memory</title>
<publicationDate>1975</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>The magical number seven after fifteen years</title>
<publicationDate>1975</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>J Cattell</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name> Mck</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>19</volume>
<pages>
<last>68</last>
<first>63</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Philosophische Studien</title>
<publicationDate>1902</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>The time of perception as a measure of differences in intensity</title>
<publicationDate>1902</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>W,G Chase</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>K,A Ericsson</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<pages>
<last>58</last>
<first>2</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>The psychology of learning and motivation</title>
<publicationDate>1982</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Skill and working memory</title>
<publicationDate>1982</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<host>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>D,K Crowder</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<title>Principles of learning and memory</title>
<publicationDate>1976</publicationDate>
</host>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>E,M Duncan</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>C,E Mcfarland</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>8</volume>
<pages>
<last>622</last>
<first>612</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Memory & Cognition</title>
<publicationDate>1980</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Isolating the effects of symbolic distance and semantic congruity in comparative judgments: An additive factor analysis</title>
<publicationDate>1980</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<host>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>L Fersen</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name> Yon</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>C,D L Wynne</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>J,D Delius</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>J,E P Staddon</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<title>Inferential reasoning in pigeons</title>
<publicationDate>1989</publicationDate>
</host>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>R,W Frick</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>79</volume>
<pages>
<last>240</last>
<first>213</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>British Journal of Psychology</title>
<publicationDate>1988</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Issues of representation and limited capacity in the auditory short-term store</title>
<publicationDate>1988</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>R,W Frick</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>79</volume>
<pages>
<last>308</last>
<first>289</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>British Journal of Psychology</title>
<publicationDate>1988</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Issues of representation and limited capacity in the visuospatial sketchpad</title>
<publicationDate>1988</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>J,A Galambos</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>L,J Rips</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>21</volume>
<pages>
<last>281</last>
<first>260</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior</title>
<publicationDate>1982</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Memory for routines</title>
<publicationDate>1982</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>D,J Gillan</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>7</volume>
<pages>
<last>164</last>
<first>150</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes</title>
<publicationDate>1981</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Reasoning in the chimpanzee: II. Transitive inference</title>
<publicationDate>1981</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>J,M E Hamilton</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>A,J Sanford</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>30</volume>
<pages>
<last>43</last>
<first>33</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology</title>
<publicationDate>1978</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>The symbolic distance effect for alphabetic order judgments: A subjective report and reaction time analysis</title>
<publicationDate>1978</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>K,J Holyoak</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>10</volume>
<pages>
<last>243</last>
<first>203</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Cognitive Psychology</title>
<publicationDate>1978</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Comparative judgments with numerical reference points</title>
<publicationDate>1978</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>K,J Holyoak</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>W,A Mah</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>14</volume>
<pages>
<last>352</last>
<first>328</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Cognitive Psychology</title>
<publicationDate>1982</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Cognitive reference points in judgments of symbolic magnitude</title>
<publicationDate>1982</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>K,J Holyoak</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>K,K Patterson</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>7</volume>
<pages>
<last>1302</last>
<first>1283</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of E~perimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance</title>
<publicationDate>1981</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>A positional discriminability model of linear-order judgments</title>
<publicationDate>1981</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>R,W Howard</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>32</volume>
<pages>
<last>633</last>
<first>625</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology</title>
<publicationDate>1980</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Category use in abstract mental comparisons</title>
<publicationDate>1980</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>D Klahr</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>W,G Chase</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>E,A Lovelace</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>9</volume>
<pages>
<last>477</last>
<first>462</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning , Memory, and Cognition</title>
<publicationDate>1983</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Structure and process in alphabetic retrieval</title>
<publicationDate>1983</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<host>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>S,M Kosslyn</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<title>Iraage and mind</title>
<publicationDate>1980</publicationDate>
</host>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>S,M Kosslyn</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>G,L Murphy</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>M,E Bemesderfer</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>K,J Feinstein</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>106</volume>
<pages>
<last>375</last>
<first>341</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Experimental Psychology: General</title>
<publicationDate>1977</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Category and continuum in mental comparisons</title>
<publicationDate>1977</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>R,H Maki</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>7</volume>
<pages>
<last>32</last>
<first>15</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Learning andMemory</title>
<publicationDate>1981</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Categorization and distance effects with spatial linear orders</title>
<publicationDate>1981</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<host>
<pages>
<last>264</last>
<first>252</first>
</pages>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>R,H Maki</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<title>Why do categorization effects occur in comparative judgment tasks? Memo~7 & Cognition</title>
</host>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>E Martin</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>D,L Noreen</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>6</volume>
<pages>
<last>435</last>
<first>421</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Cognitive Psychology</title>
<publicationDate>1974</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Serial learning: Identification of subjective subsequences</title>
<publicationDate>1974</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>R,S Moyer</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>R,H Bayer</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>8</volume>
<pages>
<last>246</last>
<first>228</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Cognitive Psychology</title>
<publicationDate>1976</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Mental comparisons and the symbolic distance effect</title>
<publicationDate>1976</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>R,S Moyer</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>S,T Dumais</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<pages>
<last>155</last>
<first>117</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>The psychology of learning and motivation</title>
<publicationDate>1978</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Mental comparisons</title>
<publicationDate>1978</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>R,S Moyer</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>T,K Landauer</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>215</volume>
<pages>
<last>1520</last>
<first>1519</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Nature</title>
<publicationDate>1967</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Time required for judgments of numerical inequality</title>
<publicationDate>1967</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>B,B Murdock</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>67</volume>
<pages>
<last>31</last>
<first>16</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Psychological Review</title>
<publicationDate>1960</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>The distinctiveness of stimuli</title>
<publicationDate>1960</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>B,B Murdock</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>64</volume>
<pages>
<last>488</last>
<first>482</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Experimental Psychology</title>
<publicationDate>1962</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>The serial position effect of free recall</title>
<publicationDate>1962</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>G Nottenburg</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>E,J Shoben</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>16</volume>
<pages>
<last>347</last>
<first>329</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Experimental Social Psychology</title>
<publicationDate>1980</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Scripts as linear orders</title>
<publicationDate>1980</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>A Paivio</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>6</volume>
<pages>
<last>208</last>
<first>199</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Memory & Cognition</title>
<publicationDate>1978</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Mental comparisons involving abstract attributes</title>
<publicationDate>1978</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>R,M Pliske</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>K,H Smith</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>7</volume>
<pages>
<last>302</last>
<first>297</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Memory & Cognition</title>
<publicationDate>1979</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Semantic categorization in a linear order problem</title>
<publicationDate>1979</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<host>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>R Pohl</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<title>Kategorielle EinfliJsse auf die Repr~isentation linearer Ordnungen</title>
<publicationDate>1987</publicationDate>
</host>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<host>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>S Schumacher</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>R Pohl</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<title>Was tat Mister X zuerst? Handlungssequenzen als hierarchisierte lineare Ordnung. Paper presented at the 31</title>
<publicationDate>1989</publicationDate>
</host>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>E,J Shoben</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>C,G Cech</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>P,J Schwanenflugel</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>9</volume>
<pages>
<last>241</last>
<first>226</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Petformance</title>
<publicationDate>1983</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>The role of substractions in comparative judgments involving numerical reference points</title>
<publicationDate>1983</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>E,J Shoben</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>C,G Cech</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>P,J Schwanenflugel</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>K,M Sailor</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>15</volume>
<pages>
<last>286</last>
<first>273</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance</title>
<publicationDate>1989</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Serial position effects in comparative judgments</title>
<publicationDate>1989</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>T Trabasso</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>C,A Riley</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<pages>
<last>410</last>
<first>381</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Information processing and cognition</title>
<publicationDate>1975</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>The cons~uction and use of representations involving linear order</title>
<publicationDate>1975</publicationDate>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<host>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>J,W Tnkey</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<title>Exploratory data analysis</title>
<publicationDate>1977</publicationDate>
</host>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<author>
<json:item>
<name>F,D Woocher</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>A,L Glass</name>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<name>K,L Holyoak</name>
</json:item>
</author>
<host>
<volume>6</volume>
<pages>
<last>173</last>
<first>165</first>
</pages>
<author></author>
<title>Memory & Cognition</title>
<publicationDate>1978</publicationDate>
</host>
<title>Positional discriminability in linear orderings</title>
<publicationDate>1978</publicationDate>
</json:item>
</refBibs>
<genre>
<json:string>research-article</json:string>
</genre>
<host>
<volume>52</volume>
<pages>
<last>75</last>
<first>68</first>
</pages>
<issn>
<json:string>0340-0727</json:string>
</issn>
<issue>1</issue>
<subject>
<json:item>
<value>Psychology, general</value>
</json:item>
</subject>
<journalId>
<json:string>426</json:string>
</journalId>
<genre>
<json:string>journal</json:string>
</genre>
<language>
<json:string>unknown</json:string>
</language>
<eissn>
<json:string>1430-2772</json:string>
</eissn>
<title>Psychological Research</title>
<publicationDate>1990</publicationDate>
<copyrightDate>1990</copyrightDate>
</host>
<categories>
<wos>
<json:string>social science</json:string>
<json:string>psychology, experimental</json:string>
</wos>
<scienceMetrix>
<json:string>health sciences</json:string>
<json:string>psychology & cognitive sciences</json:string>
<json:string>experimental psychology</json:string>
</scienceMetrix>
</categories>
<publicationDate>1990</publicationDate>
<copyrightDate>1990</copyrightDate>
<doi>
<json:string>10.1007/BF00867214</json:string>
</doi>
<id>63A7AD33CD989C3FA69EA2214D8D352CDEBA2A7A</id>
<score>0.6321506</score>
<fulltext>
<json:item>
<extension>pdf</extension>
<original>true</original>
<mimetype>application/pdf</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/63A7AD33CD989C3FA69EA2214D8D352CDEBA2A7A/fulltext/pdf</uri>
</json:item>
<json:item>
<extension>zip</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>application/zip</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/63A7AD33CD989C3FA69EA2214D8D352CDEBA2A7A/fulltext/zip</uri>
</json:item>
<istex:fulltextTEI uri="https://api.istex.fr/document/63A7AD33CD989C3FA69EA2214D8D352CDEBA2A7A/fulltext/tei">
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">Position effects in chunked linear orders</title>
<respStmt>
<resp>Références bibliographiques récupérées via GROBID</resp>
<name resp="ISTEX-API">ISTEX-API (INIST-CNRS)</name>
</respStmt>
<respStmt>
<resp>Références bibliographiques récupérées via GROBID</resp>
<name resp="ISTEX-API">ISTEX-API (INIST-CNRS)</name>
</respStmt>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<authority>ISTEX</authority>
<publisher>Springer-Verlag</publisher>
<pubPlace>Berlin/Heidelberg</pubPlace>
<availability>
<p>Springer-Verlag, 1990</p>
</availability>
<date>1989-05-24</date>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct type="inbook">
<analytic>
<title level="a" type="main" xml:lang="en">Position effects in chunked linear orders</title>
<author xml:id="author-1">
<persName>
<forename type="first">Rüdiger</forename>
<surname>Pohl</surname>
</persName>
<affiliation>FB 1 - Psychologie, Universität Trier, Postfach 3825, D-5500, Trier, Germany</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<monogr>
<title level="j">Psychological Research</title>
<title level="j" type="sub">An International Journal of Perception, Attention, Memory and Action</title>
<title level="j" type="abbrev">Psychol. Res</title>
<idno type="journal-ID">426</idno>
<idno type="pISSN">0340-0727</idno>
<idno type="eISSN">1430-2772</idno>
<idno type="issue-article-count">14</idno>
<idno type="volume-issue-count">4</idno>
<imprint>
<publisher>Springer-Verlag</publisher>
<pubPlace>Berlin/Heidelberg</pubPlace>
<date type="published" when="1990-03-01"></date>
<biblScope unit="volume">52</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="issue">1</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" from="68">68</biblScope>
<biblScope unit="page" to="75">75</biblScope>
</imprint>
</monogr>
<idno type="istex">63A7AD33CD989C3FA69EA2214D8D352CDEBA2A7A</idno>
<idno type="DOI">10.1007/BF00867214</idno>
<idno type="ArticleID">BF00867214</idno>
<idno type="ArticleID">Art11</idno>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<creation>
<date>1989-05-24</date>
</creation>
<langUsage>
<language ident="en">en</language>
</langUsage>
<abstract xml:lang="en">
<p>Summary: When someone is asked which of two items from a linearly ordered set of items comes first in that order, reaction time decreases with increasing distance between the two items in the given order. This is known as thedistance effect, a very robust finding in studies on linear orders. Surprisingly, investigations onchunked linear orders show unexpected diverse results. For example, reaction times todifferent probes (i. e., probes with items from different chunks) are sometimes longer than those tosame probes (i. e., probes with items from the same chunk). But if subjects' latency depends on the discriminability of chunk labels vs. exact position information within a chunk,different probes should always yield faster order judgments thansame probes. Two experiments investigated whether position effects, including both end-of-list and end-ofchunk effects, can account for the unexpected finding that between-chunk probes are sometimes answered more slowly than within-chunk probes. Moreover, the linea-rorder paradigm was extended to complex action sequences with more than two chunks, using original cooking recipes as materials. The results showed thatchunk-position effects play a crucial role in comparative judgments. Probes from the first and the last chunk were answered faster than those from a middle chunk.Border items (i. e., items next to a chunk boundary) did not produce a significant effect. However, chunking affected subjects' reaction time, as was indicated by the absence of a distance effect fordifferent probes. Besides, action sequences that are ordered in time, as well as concept lists used previously, can be considered linear orders, thus supporting the view that linear orders reflect a basic representation schema of human memory.</p>
</abstract>
<textClass>
<keywords scheme="Journal Subject">
<list>
<head>Psychology</head>
<item>
<term>Psychology, general</term>
</item>
</list>
</keywords>
</textClass>
</profileDesc>
<revisionDesc>
<change when="1989-05-24">Created</change>
<change when="1990-03-01">Published</change>
<change xml:id="refBibs-istex" who="#ISTEX-API" when="2016-11-22">References added</change>
<change xml:id="refBibs-istex" who="#ISTEX-API" when="2017-01-21">References added</change>
</revisionDesc>
</teiHeader>
</istex:fulltextTEI>
<json:item>
<extension>txt</extension>
<original>false</original>
<mimetype>text/plain</mimetype>
<uri>https://api.istex.fr/document/63A7AD33CD989C3FA69EA2214D8D352CDEBA2A7A/fulltext/txt</uri>
</json:item>
</fulltext>
<metadata>
<istex:metadataXml wicri:clean="Springer, Publisher found" wicri:toSee="no header">
<istex:xmlDeclaration>version="1.0" encoding="UTF-8"</istex:xmlDeclaration>
<istex:docType PUBLIC="-//Springer-Verlag//DTD A++ V2.4//EN" URI="http://devel.springer.de/A++/V2.4/DTD/A++V2.4.dtd" name="istex:docType"></istex:docType>
<istex:document>
<Publisher>
<PublisherInfo>
<PublisherName>Springer-Verlag</PublisherName>
<PublisherLocation>Berlin/Heidelberg</PublisherLocation>
</PublisherInfo>
<Journal>
<JournalInfo JournalProductType="ArchiveJournal" NumberingStyle="Unnumbered">
<JournalID>426</JournalID>
<JournalPrintISSN>0340-0727</JournalPrintISSN>
<JournalElectronicISSN>1430-2772</JournalElectronicISSN>
<JournalTitle>Psychological Research</JournalTitle>
<JournalSubTitle>An International Journal of Perception, Attention, Memory and Action</JournalSubTitle>
<JournalAbbreviatedTitle>Psychol. Res</JournalAbbreviatedTitle>
<JournalSubjectGroup>
<JournalSubject Type="Primary">Psychology</JournalSubject>
<JournalSubject Type="Secondary">Psychology, general</JournalSubject>
</JournalSubjectGroup>
</JournalInfo>
<Volume>
<VolumeInfo VolumeType="Regular" TocLevels="0">
<VolumeIDStart>52</VolumeIDStart>
<VolumeIDEnd>52</VolumeIDEnd>
<VolumeIssueCount>4</VolumeIssueCount>
</VolumeInfo>
<Issue IssueType="Regular">
<IssueInfo TocLevels="0">
<IssueIDStart>1</IssueIDStart>
<IssueIDEnd>1</IssueIDEnd>
<IssueArticleCount>14</IssueArticleCount>
<IssueHistory>
<CoverDate>
<DateString>1990</DateString>
<Year>1990</Year>
<Month>3</Month>
</CoverDate>
</IssueHistory>
<IssueCopyright>
<CopyrightHolderName>Springer-Verlag</CopyrightHolderName>
<CopyrightYear>1990</CopyrightYear>
</IssueCopyright>
</IssueInfo>
<Article ID="Art11">
<ArticleInfo Language="En" ArticleType="OriginalPaper" NumberingStyle="Unnumbered" TocLevels="0" ContainsESM="No">
<ArticleID>BF00867214</ArticleID>
<ArticleDOI>10.1007/BF00867214</ArticleDOI>
<ArticleSequenceNumber>11</ArticleSequenceNumber>
<ArticleTitle Language="En">Position effects in chunked linear orders</ArticleTitle>
<ArticleFirstPage>68</ArticleFirstPage>
<ArticleLastPage>75</ArticleLastPage>
<ArticleHistory>
<RegistrationDate>
<Year>2004</Year>
<Month>12</Month>
<Day>6</Day>
</RegistrationDate>
<Received>
<Year>1989</Year>
<Month>5</Month>
<Day>24</Day>
</Received>
<Accepted>
<Year>1989</Year>
<Month>12</Month>
<Day>10</Day>
</Accepted>
</ArticleHistory>
<ArticleCopyright>
<CopyrightHolderName>Springer-Verlag</CopyrightHolderName>
<CopyrightYear>1990</CopyrightYear>
</ArticleCopyright>
<ArticleGrants Type="Regular">
<MetadataGrant Grant="OpenAccess"></MetadataGrant>
<AbstractGrant Grant="OpenAccess"></AbstractGrant>
<BodyPDFGrant Grant="Restricted"></BodyPDFGrant>
<BodyHTMLGrant Grant="Restricted"></BodyHTMLGrant>
<BibliographyGrant Grant="Restricted"></BibliographyGrant>
<ESMGrant Grant="Restricted"></ESMGrant>
</ArticleGrants>
<ArticleContext>
<JournalID>426</JournalID>
<VolumeIDStart>52</VolumeIDStart>
<VolumeIDEnd>52</VolumeIDEnd>
<IssueIDStart>1</IssueIDStart>
<IssueIDEnd>1</IssueIDEnd>
</ArticleContext>
</ArticleInfo>
<ArticleHeader>
<AuthorGroup>
<Author AffiliationIDS="Aff1">
<AuthorName DisplayOrder="Western">
<GivenName>Rüdiger</GivenName>
<GivenName>F.</GivenName>
<FamilyName>Pohl</FamilyName>
</AuthorName>
</Author>
<Affiliation ID="Aff1">
<OrgDivision>FB 1 - Psychologie</OrgDivision>
<OrgName>Universität Trier</OrgName>
<OrgAddress>
<Postbox>Postfach 3825</Postbox>
<Postcode>D-5500</Postcode>
<City>Trier</City>
<Country>Germany</Country>
</OrgAddress>
</Affiliation>
</AuthorGroup>
<Abstract ID="Abs1" Language="En">
<Heading>Summary</Heading>
<Para>When someone is asked which of two items from a linearly ordered set of items comes first in that order, reaction time decreases with increasing distance between the two items in the given order. This is known as the
<Emphasis Type="Italic">distance effect</Emphasis>
, a very robust finding in studies on linear orders. Surprisingly, investigations on
<Emphasis Type="Italic">chunked</Emphasis>
linear orders show unexpected diverse results. For example, reaction times to
<Emphasis Type="Italic">different</Emphasis>
probes (i. e., probes with items from different chunks) are sometimes longer than those to
<Emphasis Type="Italic">same</Emphasis>
probes (i. e., probes with items from the same chunk). But if subjects' latency depends on the discriminability of chunk labels vs. exact position information within a chunk,
<Emphasis Type="Italic">different</Emphasis>
probes should always yield faster order judgments than
<Emphasis Type="Italic">same</Emphasis>
probes. Two experiments investigated whether position effects, including both end-of-list and end-ofchunk effects, can account for the unexpected finding that between-chunk probes are sometimes answered more slowly than within-chunk probes. Moreover, the linea-rorder paradigm was extended to complex action sequences with more than two chunks, using original cooking recipes as materials. The results showed that
<Emphasis Type="Italic">chunk-position</Emphasis>
effects play a crucial role in comparative judgments. Probes from the first and the last chunk were answered faster than those from a middle chunk.
<Emphasis Type="Italic">Border items</Emphasis>
(i. e., items next to a chunk boundary) did not produce a significant effect. However, chunking affected subjects' reaction time, as was indicated by the absence of a distance effect for
<Emphasis Type="Italic">different</Emphasis>
probes. Besides, action sequences that are ordered in time, as well as concept lists used previously, can be considered linear orders, thus supporting the view that linear orders reflect a basic representation schema of human memory.</Para>
</Abstract>
</ArticleHeader>
<NoBody></NoBody>
</Article>
</Issue>
</Volume>
</Journal>
</Publisher>
</istex:document>
</istex:metadataXml>
<mods version="3.6">
<titleInfo lang="en">
<title>Position effects in chunked linear orders</title>
</titleInfo>
<titleInfo type="alternative" contentType="CDATA" lang="en">
<title>Position effects in chunked linear orders</title>
</titleInfo>
<name type="personal">
<namePart type="given">Rüdiger</namePart>
<namePart type="given">F.</namePart>
<namePart type="family">Pohl</namePart>
<affiliation>FB 1 - Psychologie, Universität Trier, Postfach 3825, D-5500, Trier, Germany</affiliation>
<role>
<roleTerm type="text">author</roleTerm>
</role>
</name>
<typeOfResource>text</typeOfResource>
<genre type="research-article" displayLabel="OriginalPaper"></genre>
<originInfo>
<publisher>Springer-Verlag</publisher>
<place>
<placeTerm type="text">Berlin/Heidelberg</placeTerm>
</place>
<dateCreated encoding="w3cdtf">1989-05-24</dateCreated>
<dateIssued encoding="w3cdtf">1990-03-01</dateIssued>
<copyrightDate encoding="w3cdtf">1990</copyrightDate>
</originInfo>
<language>
<languageTerm type="code" authority="rfc3066">en</languageTerm>
<languageTerm type="code" authority="iso639-2b">eng</languageTerm>
</language>
<physicalDescription>
<internetMediaType>text/html</internetMediaType>
</physicalDescription>
<abstract lang="en">Summary: When someone is asked which of two items from a linearly ordered set of items comes first in that order, reaction time decreases with increasing distance between the two items in the given order. This is known as thedistance effect, a very robust finding in studies on linear orders. Surprisingly, investigations onchunked linear orders show unexpected diverse results. For example, reaction times todifferent probes (i. e., probes with items from different chunks) are sometimes longer than those tosame probes (i. e., probes with items from the same chunk). But if subjects' latency depends on the discriminability of chunk labels vs. exact position information within a chunk,different probes should always yield faster order judgments thansame probes. Two experiments investigated whether position effects, including both end-of-list and end-ofchunk effects, can account for the unexpected finding that between-chunk probes are sometimes answered more slowly than within-chunk probes. Moreover, the linea-rorder paradigm was extended to complex action sequences with more than two chunks, using original cooking recipes as materials. The results showed thatchunk-position effects play a crucial role in comparative judgments. Probes from the first and the last chunk were answered faster than those from a middle chunk.Border items (i. e., items next to a chunk boundary) did not produce a significant effect. However, chunking affected subjects' reaction time, as was indicated by the absence of a distance effect fordifferent probes. Besides, action sequences that are ordered in time, as well as concept lists used previously, can be considered linear orders, thus supporting the view that linear orders reflect a basic representation schema of human memory.</abstract>
<relatedItem type="host">
<titleInfo>
<title>Psychological Research</title>
<subTitle>An International Journal of Perception, Attention, Memory and Action</subTitle>
</titleInfo>
<titleInfo type="abbreviated">
<title>Psychol. Res</title>
</titleInfo>
<genre type="journal" displayLabel="Archive Journal"></genre>
<originInfo>
<dateIssued encoding="w3cdtf">1990-03-01</dateIssued>
<copyrightDate encoding="w3cdtf">1990</copyrightDate>
</originInfo>
<subject>
<genre>Psychology</genre>
<topic>Psychology, general</topic>
</subject>
<identifier type="ISSN">0340-0727</identifier>
<identifier type="eISSN">1430-2772</identifier>
<identifier type="JournalID">426</identifier>
<identifier type="IssueArticleCount">14</identifier>
<identifier type="VolumeIssueCount">4</identifier>
<part>
<date>1990</date>
<detail type="volume">
<number>52</number>
<caption>vol.</caption>
</detail>
<detail type="issue">
<number>1</number>
<caption>no.</caption>
</detail>
<extent unit="pages">
<start>68</start>
<end>75</end>
</extent>
</part>
<recordInfo>
<recordOrigin>Springer-Verlag, 1990</recordOrigin>
</recordInfo>
</relatedItem>
<identifier type="istex">63A7AD33CD989C3FA69EA2214D8D352CDEBA2A7A</identifier>
<identifier type="DOI">10.1007/BF00867214</identifier>
<identifier type="ArticleID">BF00867214</identifier>
<identifier type="ArticleID">Art11</identifier>
<accessCondition type="use and reproduction" contentType="copyright">Springer-Verlag, 1990</accessCondition>
<recordInfo>
<recordContentSource>SPRINGER</recordContentSource>
<recordOrigin>Springer-Verlag, 1990</recordOrigin>
</recordInfo>
</mods>
</metadata>
<serie></serie>
</istex>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Wicri/Rhénanie/explor/UnivTrevesV1/Data/Istex/Corpus
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 001A34 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Istex/Corpus/biblio.hfd -nk 001A34 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Wicri/Rhénanie
   |area=    UnivTrevesV1
   |flux=    Istex
   |étape=   Corpus
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     ISTEX:63A7AD33CD989C3FA69EA2214D8D352CDEBA2A7A
   |texte=   Position effects in chunked linear orders
}}

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.31.
Data generation: Sat Jul 22 16:29:01 2017. Site generation: Wed Feb 28 14:55:37 2024