Serveur d'exploration sur l'oranger

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.

The Influence of Learning on Host Plant Preference in a Significant Phytopathogen Vector, Diaphorina citri

Identifieur interne : 000378 ( Pmc/Corpus ); précédent : 000377; suivant : 000379

The Influence of Learning on Host Plant Preference in a Significant Phytopathogen Vector, Diaphorina citri

Auteurs : Dara G. Stockton ; Xavier Martini ; Joseph M. Patt ; Lukasz L. Stelinski

Source :

RBID : PMC:4773162

Abstract

Although specialist herbivorous insects are guided by innate responses to host plant cues, host plant preference may be influenced by experience and is not dictated by instinct alone. The effect of learning on host plant preference was examined in the Asian citrus psyllid, Diaphorina citri; vector of the causal agent of citrus greening disease or huanglongbing. We investigated: a) whether development on specific host plant species influenced host plant preference in mature D. citri; and b) the extent of associative learning in D. citri in the form of simple and compound conditioning. Learning was measured by cue selection in a 2-choice behavioral assay and compared to naïve controls. Our results showed that learned responses in D. citri are complex and diverse. The developmental host plant species influenced adult host plant preference, with female psyllids preferring the species on which they were reared. However, such preferences were subject to change with the introduction of an alternative host plant within 24–48 hrs, indicating a large degree of experience-dependent response plasticity. Additionally, learning occurred for multiple sensory modalities where novel olfactory and visual environmental cues were associated with the host plant. However, males and females displayed differing discriminatory abilities. In compound conditioning tasks, males exhibited recognition of a compound stimulus alone while females were capable of learning the individual components. These findings suggest D. citri are dynamic animals that demonstrate host plant preference based on developmental and adult experience and can learn to recognize olfactory and visual host plant stimuli in ways that may be sex specific. These experience-based associations are likely used by adults to locate and select suitable host plants for feeding and reproduction and may suggest the need for more tailored lures and traps, which reflect region-specific cultivars or predominate Rutaceae in the area being monitored.


Url:
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0149815
PubMed: 26930355
PubMed Central: 4773162

Links to Exploration step

PMC:4773162

Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI>
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">The Influence of Learning on Host Plant Preference in a Significant Phytopathogen Vector,
<italic>Diaphorina citri</italic>
</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stockton, Dara G" sort="Stockton, Dara G" uniqKey="Stockton D" first="Dara G." last="Stockton">Dara G. Stockton</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="aff001">
<addr-line>Department of Entomology and Nematology, University of Florida, Citrus Research and Education Center, Lake Alfred, Florida, United States of America</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Martini, Xavier" sort="Martini, Xavier" uniqKey="Martini X" first="Xavier" last="Martini">Xavier Martini</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="aff001">
<addr-line>Department of Entomology and Nematology, University of Florida, Citrus Research and Education Center, Lake Alfred, Florida, United States of America</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Patt, Joseph M" sort="Patt, Joseph M" uniqKey="Patt J" first="Joseph M." last="Patt">Joseph M. Patt</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="aff002">
<addr-line>United States Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Services, U.S. Horticultural Research Laboratory, Ft. Pierce, Florida, United States of America</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stelinski, Lukasz L" sort="Stelinski, Lukasz L" uniqKey="Stelinski L" first="Lukasz L." last="Stelinski">Lukasz L. Stelinski</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="aff001">
<addr-line>Department of Entomology and Nematology, University of Florida, Citrus Research and Education Center, Lake Alfred, Florida, United States of America</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">PMC</idno>
<idno type="pmid">26930355</idno>
<idno type="pmc">4773162</idno>
<idno type="url">http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4773162</idno>
<idno type="RBID">PMC:4773162</idno>
<idno type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0149815</idno>
<date when="2016">2016</date>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Corpus">000378</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title xml:lang="en" level="a" type="main">The Influence of Learning on Host Plant Preference in a Significant Phytopathogen Vector,
<italic>Diaphorina citri</italic>
</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stockton, Dara G" sort="Stockton, Dara G" uniqKey="Stockton D" first="Dara G." last="Stockton">Dara G. Stockton</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="aff001">
<addr-line>Department of Entomology and Nematology, University of Florida, Citrus Research and Education Center, Lake Alfred, Florida, United States of America</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Martini, Xavier" sort="Martini, Xavier" uniqKey="Martini X" first="Xavier" last="Martini">Xavier Martini</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="aff001">
<addr-line>Department of Entomology and Nematology, University of Florida, Citrus Research and Education Center, Lake Alfred, Florida, United States of America</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Patt, Joseph M" sort="Patt, Joseph M" uniqKey="Patt J" first="Joseph M." last="Patt">Joseph M. Patt</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="aff002">
<addr-line>United States Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Services, U.S. Horticultural Research Laboratory, Ft. Pierce, Florida, United States of America</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stelinski, Lukasz L" sort="Stelinski, Lukasz L" uniqKey="Stelinski L" first="Lukasz L." last="Stelinski">Lukasz L. Stelinski</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="aff001">
<addr-line>Department of Entomology and Nematology, University of Florida, Citrus Research and Education Center, Lake Alfred, Florida, United States of America</addr-line>
</nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<series>
<title level="j">PLoS ONE</title>
<idno type="eISSN">1932-6203</idno>
<imprint>
<date when="2016">2016</date>
</imprint>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">
<p>Although specialist herbivorous insects are guided by innate responses to host plant cues, host plant preference may be influenced by experience and is not dictated by instinct alone. The effect of learning on host plant preference was examined in the Asian citrus psyllid,
<italic>Diaphorina citri</italic>
; vector of the causal agent of citrus greening disease or huanglongbing. We investigated: a) whether development on specific host plant species influenced host plant preference in mature
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
; and b) the extent of associative learning in
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
in the form of simple and compound conditioning. Learning was measured by cue selection in a 2-choice behavioral assay and compared to naïve controls. Our results showed that learned responses in
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
are complex and diverse. The developmental host plant species influenced adult host plant preference, with female psyllids preferring the species on which they were reared. However, such preferences were subject to change with the introduction of an alternative host plant within 24–48 hrs, indicating a large degree of experience-dependent response plasticity. Additionally, learning occurred for multiple sensory modalities where novel olfactory and visual environmental cues were associated with the host plant. However, males and females displayed differing discriminatory abilities. In compound conditioning tasks, males exhibited recognition of a compound stimulus alone while females were capable of learning the individual components. These findings suggest
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
are dynamic animals that demonstrate host plant preference based on developmental and adult experience and can learn to recognize olfactory and visual host plant stimuli in ways that may be sex specific. These experience-based associations are likely used by adults to locate and select suitable host plants for feeding and reproduction and may suggest the need for more tailored lures and traps, which reflect region-specific cultivars or predominate Rutaceae in the area being monitored.</p>
</div>
</front>
<back>
<div1 type="bibliography">
<listBibl>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mori, M" uniqKey="Mori M">M Mori</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bernays, Ea" uniqKey="Bernays E">EA Bernays</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Woodhead, S" uniqKey="Woodhead S">S Woodhead</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Haines, L" uniqKey="Haines L">L Haines</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weiss, Mr" uniqKey="Weiss M">MR Weiss</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kelber, A" uniqKey="Kelber A">A Kelber</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Riffell, Ja" uniqKey="Riffell J">JA Riffell</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Alarc N, R" uniqKey="Alarc N R">R Alarcón</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Abrell, L" uniqKey="Abrell L">L Abrell</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Davidowitz, G" uniqKey="Davidowitz G">G Davidowitz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bronstein, Jl" uniqKey="Bronstein J">JL Bronstein</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hildebrand, Jg" uniqKey="Hildebrand J">JG Hildebrand</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Temple, B" uniqKey="Temple B">B Temple</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bonini, N" uniqKey="Bonini N">N Bonini</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dawson, D" uniqKey="Dawson D">D Dawson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Quinn, W" uniqKey="Quinn W">W Quinn</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Quinn, W" uniqKey="Quinn W">W Quinn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Harris, W" uniqKey="Harris W">W Harris</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Benzer, S" uniqKey="Benzer S">S Benzer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Simpson, S" uniqKey="Simpson S">S Simpson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Raubenheimer, D" uniqKey="Raubenheimer D">D Raubenheimer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dukas, R" uniqKey="Dukas R">R Dukas</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bernays, Ea" uniqKey="Bernays E">EA Bernays</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Von Frisch, K" uniqKey="Von Frisch K">K von Frisch</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Menzel, R" uniqKey="Menzel R">R Menzel</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Giurfa, M" uniqKey="Giurfa M">M Giurfa</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Menzel, R" uniqKey="Menzel R">R Menzel</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Muller, U" uniqKey="Muller U">U Muller</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dyer, F" uniqKey="Dyer F">F Dyer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Carter, R" uniqKey="Carter R">R Carter</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Laverty, T" uniqKey="Laverty T">T Laverty</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leadbeater, E" uniqKey="Leadbeater E">E Leadbeater</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chittka, L" uniqKey="Chittka L">L Chittka</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dawson, Eh" uniqKey="Dawson E">EH Dawson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Avargues Weber, A" uniqKey="Avargues Weber A">A Avarguès-Weber</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chittka, L" uniqKey="Chittka L">L Chittka</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leadbeater, E" uniqKey="Leadbeater E">E Leadbeater</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hunt, Kl" uniqKey="Hunt K">KL Hunt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chittka, L" uniqKey="Chittka L">L Chittka</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Githeko, Ak" uniqKey="Githeko A">AK Githeko</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lindsay, Sw" uniqKey="Lindsay S">SW Lindsay</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Confalonieri, Ue" uniqKey="Confalonieri U">UE Confalonieri</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Patz, Ja" uniqKey="Patz J">JA Patz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Parmesan, C" uniqKey="Parmesan C">C Parmesan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Watson, Rt" uniqKey="Watson R">RT Watson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zinyowera, Mc" uniqKey="Zinyowera M">MC Zinyowera</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Moss, Rh" uniqKey="Moss R">RH Moss</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Patt, Jm" uniqKey="Patt J">JM Patt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Setamou, M" uniqKey="Setamou M">M Sétamou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Henaut, Y" uniqKey="Henaut Y">Y Henaut</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Alauzet, C" uniqKey="Alauzet C">C Alauzet</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dargagnon, D" uniqKey="Dargagnon D">D Dargagnon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lambin, M" uniqKey="Lambin M">M Lambin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aldana, E" uniqKey="Aldana E">E Aldana</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Abramson, Ci" uniqKey="Abramson C">CI Abramson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lizano, E" uniqKey="Lizano E">E Lizano</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vegas, R" uniqKey="Vegas R">R Vegas</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sulbaran Romero, E" uniqKey="Sulbaran Romero E">E Sulbaran-Romero</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Abramson, Ci" uniqKey="Abramson C">CI Abramson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Frasca, J" uniqKey="Frasca J">J Frasca</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fehr, R" uniqKey="Fehr R">R Fehr</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Romero, Es" uniqKey="Romero E">ES Romero</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lizano, E" uniqKey="Lizano E">E Lizano</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aldana, E" uniqKey="Aldana E">E Aldana</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vinauger, C" uniqKey="Vinauger C">C Vinauger</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Buratti, L" uniqKey="Buratti L">L Buratti</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lazzari, Cr" uniqKey="Lazzari C">CR Lazzari</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vinauger, C" uniqKey="Vinauger C">C Vinauger</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Buratti, L" uniqKey="Buratti L">L Buratti</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lazzari, Cr" uniqKey="Lazzari C">CR Lazzari</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vinauger, C" uniqKey="Vinauger C">C Vinauger</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pereira, Mh" uniqKey="Pereira M">MH Pereira</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lazzari, Cr" uniqKey="Lazzari C">CR Lazzari</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Prokopy, Rj" uniqKey="Prokopy R">RJ Prokopy</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lewis, Jw" uniqKey="Lewis J">JW Lewis</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Papaj, Dr" uniqKey="Papaj D">DR Papaj</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lewis, Ac" uniqKey="Lewis A">AC Lewis</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Halbert, Se" uniqKey="Halbert S">SE Halbert</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Manjunath, Kl" uniqKey="Manjunath K">KL Manjunath</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bove, Jm" uniqKey="Bove J">JM Bové</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grafton Cardwell, Ee" uniqKey="Grafton Cardwell E">EE Grafton-Cardwell</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stelinski, Ll" uniqKey="Stelinski L">LL Stelinski</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stansly, Pa" uniqKey="Stansly P">PA Stansly</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hodges, Aw" uniqKey="Hodges A">AW Hodges</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Spreen, Th" uniqKey="Spreen T">TH Spreen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tiwari, S" uniqKey="Tiwari S">S Tiwari</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mann, Rs" uniqKey="Mann R">RS Mann</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rogers, Me" uniqKey="Rogers M">ME Rogers</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stelinski, Ll" uniqKey="Stelinski L">LL Stelinski</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Martini, X" uniqKey="Martini X">X Martini</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pelz Stelinski, K" uniqKey="Pelz Stelinski K">K Pelz-Stelinski</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stelinski, Ll" uniqKey="Stelinski L">LL Stelinski</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Croxton, Sd" uniqKey="Croxton S">SD Croxton</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stansly, Pa" uniqKey="Stansly P">PA Stansly</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cook, Sm" uniqKey="Cook S">SM Cook</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Khan, Zr" uniqKey="Khan Z">ZR Khan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pickett, Ja" uniqKey="Pickett J">JA Pickett</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hall, Dg" uniqKey="Hall D">DG Hall</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hentz, Mg" uniqKey="Hentz M">MG Hentz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ciomperlik, Ma" uniqKey="Ciomperlik M">MA Ciomperlik</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Paris, Tm" uniqKey="Paris T">TM Paris</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Croxton, Sd" uniqKey="Croxton S">SD Croxton</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stansly, Pa" uniqKey="Stansly P">PA Stansly</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Allan, Sa" uniqKey="Allan S">SA Allan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Setamou, M" uniqKey="Setamou M">M Sétamou</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sanchez, A" uniqKey="Sanchez A">A Sanchez</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Salda A, Rr" uniqKey="Salda A R">RR Saldaña</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Patt, Jm" uniqKey="Patt J">JM Patt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Summy, R" uniqKey="Summy R">R Summy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Patt, Jm" uniqKey="Patt J">JM Patt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meikle, Wg" uniqKey="Meikle W">WG Meikle</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mafra Neto, A" uniqKey="Mafra Neto A">A Mafra-Neto</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Setamou, M" uniqKey="Setamou M">M Sétamou</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mangan, R" uniqKey="Mangan R">R Mangan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yang, C" uniqKey="Yang C">C Yang</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mankin, Rw" uniqKey="Mankin R">RW Mankin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rohde, Bb" uniqKey="Rohde B">BB Rohde</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcneill, Sa" uniqKey="Mcneill S">SA Mcneill</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Paris, Tm" uniqKey="Paris T">TM Paris</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zagvazdina, Ni" uniqKey="Zagvazdina N">NI Zagvazdina</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Greenfeder, S" uniqKey="Greenfeder S">S Greenfeder</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rohde, B" uniqKey="Rohde B">B Rohde</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Paris, Tm" uniqKey="Paris T">TM Paris</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Heatherington, Em" uniqKey="Heatherington E">EM Heatherington</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hall, Dg" uniqKey="Hall D">DG Hall</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mankin, Rw" uniqKey="Mankin R">RW Mankin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mann, Rs" uniqKey="Mann R">RS Mann</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ali, Jg" uniqKey="Ali J">JG Ali</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hermann, Sl" uniqKey="Hermann S">SL Hermann</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tiwari, S" uniqKey="Tiwari S">S Tiwari</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pelz Stelinski, Ks" uniqKey="Pelz Stelinski K">KS Pelz-Stelinski</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Alborn, Ht" uniqKey="Alborn H">HT Alborn</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Martini, X" uniqKey="Martini X">X Martini</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kuhns, Eh" uniqKey="Kuhns E">EH Kuhns</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hoyte, A" uniqKey="Hoyte A">A Hoyte</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stelinski, Ll" uniqKey="Stelinski L">LL Stelinski</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Patt, Jm" uniqKey="Patt J">JM Patt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Setamou, M" uniqKey="Setamou M">M Setamou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aksenov, Aa" uniqKey="Aksenov A">AA Aksenov</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Martini, X" uniqKey="Martini X">X Martini</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zhao, W" uniqKey="Zhao W">W Zhao</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stelinski, Ll" uniqKey="Stelinski L">LL Stelinski</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Davis, Ce" uniqKey="Davis C">CE Davis</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mann, Rs" uniqKey="Mann R">RS Mann</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rouseff, Rl" uniqKey="Rouseff R">RL Rouseff</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smoot, J" uniqKey="Smoot J">J Smoot</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rao, N" uniqKey="Rao N">N Rao</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meyer, Wl" uniqKey="Meyer W">WL Meyer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lapointe, S" uniqKey="Lapointe S">S Lapointe</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Godfrey, Ke" uniqKey="Godfrey K">KE Godfrey</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Galindo, C" uniqKey="Galindo C">C Galindo</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Patt, Jm" uniqKey="Patt J">JM Patt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Luque Williams, M" uniqKey="Luque Williams M">M Luque-Williams</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Coutinho Abreu, Iv" uniqKey="Coutinho Abreu I">IV Coutinho-Abreu</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcinally, S" uniqKey="Mcinally S">S McInally</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Forster, L" uniqKey="Forster L">L Forster</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Luck, R" uniqKey="Luck R">R Luck</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ray, A" uniqKey="Ray A">A Ray</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Patt, Jm" uniqKey="Patt J">JM Patt</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stockton, D" uniqKey="Stockton D">D Stockton</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meikle, Wg" uniqKey="Meikle W">WG Meikle</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Setamou, M" uniqKey="Setamou M">M Sétamou</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mafra Neto, A" uniqKey="Mafra Neto A">A Mafra-Neto</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Adamczyk, Jj" uniqKey="Adamczyk J">JJ Adamczyk</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stelinski, Ll" uniqKey="Stelinski L">LL Stelinski</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tiwari, S" uniqKey="Tiwari S">S Tiwari</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mann, Rs" uniqKey="Mann R">RS Mann</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rouseff, Rl" uniqKey="Rouseff R">RL Rouseff</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smoot, Jm" uniqKey="Smoot J">JM Smoot</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Castle, Ws" uniqKey="Castle W">WS Castle</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stelinski, Ll" uniqKey="Stelinski L">LL Stelinski</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yamamoto, Rt" uniqKey="Yamamoto R">RT Yamamoto</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fraenkel, Gs" uniqKey="Fraenkel G">GS Fraenkel</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="St Dler, E" uniqKey="St Dler E">E Städler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hanson, Fe" uniqKey="Hanson F">FE Hanson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jermy, T" uniqKey="Jermy T">T Jermy</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hanson, Fe" uniqKey="Hanson F">FE Hanson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dethier, Vg" uniqKey="Dethier V">VG Dethier</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Raguso, Ra" uniqKey="Raguso R">RA Raguso</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Willis, Ma" uniqKey="Willis M">MA Willis</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Couvillon, Pa" uniqKey="Couvillon P">PA Couvillon</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bitterman, Me" uniqKey="Bitterman M">ME Bitterman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Goyret, J" uniqKey="Goyret J">J Goyret</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pfaff, M" uniqKey="Pfaff M">M Pfaff</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Raguso, Ra" uniqKey="Raguso R">RA Raguso</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kelber, A" uniqKey="Kelber A">A Kelber</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Martini, X" uniqKey="Martini X">X Martini</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hoyte, A" uniqKey="Hoyte A">A Hoyte</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stelinski, Ll" uniqKey="Stelinski L">LL Stelinski</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lewis Rosenblum, H" uniqKey="Lewis Rosenblum H">H Lewis-Rosenblum</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Martini, X" uniqKey="Martini X">X Martini</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tiwari, S" uniqKey="Tiwari S">S Tiwari</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Stelinski, Ll" uniqKey="Stelinski L">LL Stelinski</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hall, Dg" uniqKey="Hall D">DG Hall</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hentz, Mg" uniqKey="Hentz M">MG Hentz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rescorla, Ra" uniqKey="Rescorla R">RA Rescorla</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wagner, Ar" uniqKey="Wagner A">AR Wagner</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Black, Ah" uniqKey="Black A">AH Black</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Prokasy, Wf" uniqKey="Prokasy W">WF Prokasy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Miller, Rr" uniqKey="Miller R">RR Miller</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Barnet, Rc" uniqKey="Barnet R">RC Barnet</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Grahame, Nj" uniqKey="Grahame N">NJ Grahame</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Morris, Rg" uniqKey="Morris R">RG Morris</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wessnitzer, J" uniqKey="Wessnitzer J">J Wessnitzer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mangan, M" uniqKey="Mangan M">M Mangan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Webb, B" uniqKey="Webb B">B Webb</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wenninger, Ej" uniqKey="Wenninger E">EJ Wenninger</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hall, Dg" uniqKey="Hall D">DG Hall</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Huang, Xp" uniqKey="Huang X">XP Huang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Renwick, Jaa" uniqKey="Renwick J">JAA Renwick</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Szentesi, A" uniqKey="Szentesi A">A Szentesi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bernays, Ea" uniqKey="Bernays E">EA Bernays</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct></biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chan, Stl" uniqKey="Chan S">STL Chan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Abang, F" uniqKey="Abang F">F Abang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Beattie, A" uniqKey="Beattie A">A Beattie</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kueh, Rjh" uniqKey="Kueh R">RJH Kueh</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wong, Sk" uniqKey="Wong S">SK Wong</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Farnier, K" uniqKey="Farnier K">K Farnier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dyer, Ag" uniqKey="Dyer A">AG Dyer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Steinbauer, Mj" uniqKey="Steinbauer M">MJ Steinbauer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Farnier, K" uniqKey="Farnier K">K Farnier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dyer, Ag" uniqKey="Dyer A">AG Dyer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Taylor, Gs" uniqKey="Taylor G">GS Taylor</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Peters, Ra" uniqKey="Peters R">RA Peters</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Steinbauer, Mj" uniqKey="Steinbauer M">MJ Steinbauer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
</listBibl>
</div1>
</back>
</TEI>
<pmc article-type="research-article">
<pmc-dir>properties open_access</pmc-dir>
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="nlm-ta">PLoS One</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="iso-abbrev">PLoS ONE</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="publisher-id">plos</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="pmc">plosone</journal-id>
<journal-title-group>
<journal-title>PLoS ONE</journal-title>
</journal-title-group>
<issn pub-type="epub">1932-6203</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Public Library of Science</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>San Francisco, CA USA</publisher-loc>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmid">26930355</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmc">4773162</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0149815</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="publisher-id">PONE-D-15-35846</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Research Article</subject>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Neuroscience</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Cognitive Science</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Cognitive Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Learning</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Cognitive Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Learning</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Social Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Cognitive Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Learning</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Neuroscience</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Learning and Memory</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Learning</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Behavior</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Behavioral Conditioning</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Organisms</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Animals</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Invertebrates</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Arthropoda</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Insects</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Organisms</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Plants</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Behavior</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Conditioned Response</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Ecology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Plant Ecology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Plant-Animal Interactions</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Plant-Herbivore Interactions</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Ecology and Environmental Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Ecology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Plant Ecology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Plant-Animal Interactions</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Plant-Herbivore Interactions</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Plant Science</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Plant Ecology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Plant-Animal Interactions</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Plant-Herbivore Interactions</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Medicine and Health Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Epidemiology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Disease Vectors</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Insect Vectors</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
<subj-group subj-group-type="Discipline-v3">
<subject>Biology and Life Sciences</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Agriculture</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Pest Control</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>The Influence of Learning on Host Plant Preference in a Significant Phytopathogen Vector,
<italic>Diaphorina citri</italic>
</article-title>
<alt-title alt-title-type="running-head">Learning in
<italic>Diaphorina citri</italic>
</alt-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Stockton</surname>
<given-names>Dara G.</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff001">
<sup>1</sup>
</xref>
<xref ref-type="corresp" rid="cor001">*</xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Martini</surname>
<given-names>Xavier</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff001">
<sup>1</sup>
</xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Patt</surname>
<given-names>Joseph M.</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff002">
<sup>2</sup>
</xref>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Stelinski</surname>
<given-names>Lukasz L.</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff001">
<sup>1</sup>
</xref>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<aff id="aff001">
<label>1</label>
<addr-line>Department of Entomology and Nematology, University of Florida, Citrus Research and Education Center, Lake Alfred, Florida, United States of America</addr-line>
</aff>
<aff id="aff002">
<label>2</label>
<addr-line>United States Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Services, U.S. Horticultural Research Laboratory, Ft. Pierce, Florida, United States of America</addr-line>
</aff>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Renou</surname>
<given-names>Michel</given-names>
</name>
<role>Editor</role>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="edit1"></xref>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<aff id="edit1">
<addr-line>INRA-UPMC, FRANCE</addr-line>
</aff>
<author-notes>
<fn fn-type="conflict" id="coi001">
<p>
<bold>Competing Interests: </bold>
The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.</p>
</fn>
<fn fn-type="con" id="contrib001">
<p>Conceived and designed the experiments: DGS LLS JMP XM. Performed the experiments: DGS. Analyzed the data: DGS XM. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: LLS. Wrote the paper: DGS XM LLS JMP.</p>
</fn>
<corresp id="cor001">* E-mail:
<email>dara.stockton@ufl.edu</email>
</corresp>
</author-notes>
<pub-date pub-type="epub">
<day>1</day>
<month>3</month>
<year>2016</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="collection">
<year>2016</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>11</volume>
<issue>3</issue>
<elocation-id>e0149815</elocation-id>
<history>
<date date-type="received">
<day>14</day>
<month>8</month>
<year>2015</year>
</date>
<date date-type="accepted">
<day>4</day>
<month>2</month>
<year>2016</year>
</date>
</history>
<permissions>
<license xlink:href="https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/">
<license-p>This is an open access article, free of all copyright, and may be freely reproduced, distributed, transmitted, modified, built upon, or otherwise used by anyone for any lawful purpose. The work is made available under the
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/">Creative Commons CC0</ext-link>
public domain dedication.</license-p>
</license>
</permissions>
<self-uri content-type="pdf" xlink:type="simple" xlink:href="pone.0149815.pdf"></self-uri>
<abstract>
<p>Although specialist herbivorous insects are guided by innate responses to host plant cues, host plant preference may be influenced by experience and is not dictated by instinct alone. The effect of learning on host plant preference was examined in the Asian citrus psyllid,
<italic>Diaphorina citri</italic>
; vector of the causal agent of citrus greening disease or huanglongbing. We investigated: a) whether development on specific host plant species influenced host plant preference in mature
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
; and b) the extent of associative learning in
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
in the form of simple and compound conditioning. Learning was measured by cue selection in a 2-choice behavioral assay and compared to naïve controls. Our results showed that learned responses in
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
are complex and diverse. The developmental host plant species influenced adult host plant preference, with female psyllids preferring the species on which they were reared. However, such preferences were subject to change with the introduction of an alternative host plant within 24–48 hrs, indicating a large degree of experience-dependent response plasticity. Additionally, learning occurred for multiple sensory modalities where novel olfactory and visual environmental cues were associated with the host plant. However, males and females displayed differing discriminatory abilities. In compound conditioning tasks, males exhibited recognition of a compound stimulus alone while females were capable of learning the individual components. These findings suggest
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
are dynamic animals that demonstrate host plant preference based on developmental and adult experience and can learn to recognize olfactory and visual host plant stimuli in ways that may be sex specific. These experience-based associations are likely used by adults to locate and select suitable host plants for feeding and reproduction and may suggest the need for more tailored lures and traps, which reflect region-specific cultivars or predominate Rutaceae in the area being monitored.</p>
</abstract>
<funding-group>
<funding-statement>This research was funded by the University of Florida and the Citrus Research and Development Foundation to LLS. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.</funding-statement>
</funding-group>
<counts>
<fig-count count="6"></fig-count>
<table-count count="2"></table-count>
<page-count count="17"></page-count>
</counts>
<custom-meta-group>
<custom-meta id="data-availability">
<meta-name>Data Availability</meta-name>
<meta-value>All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.</meta-value>
</custom-meta>
</custom-meta-group>
</article-meta>
<notes>
<title>Data Availability</title>
<p>All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.</p>
</notes>
</front>
<body>
<sec sec-type="intro" id="sec001">
<title>Introduction</title>
<p>Although herbivorous insects rely heavily on innate olfactory and/or visual preferences to locate and select host plants [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref001" ref-type="bibr">1</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref002" ref-type="bibr">2</xref>
], innate responses may be shaped by the organism’s experiences in a process known as learning [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref003" ref-type="bibr">3</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref005" ref-type="bibr">5</xref>
]. Insects are not only capable of learning, but can accomplish highly complicated tasks as a result. Fruit flies can associate odor, color, and visual patterns with both appetitive and aversive conditions [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref006" ref-type="bibr">6</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref007" ref-type="bibr">7</xref>
]; and grasshoppers have the sophisticated ability to learn visual stimuli associated with nutritionally valuable food [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref008" ref-type="bibr">8</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref009" ref-type="bibr">9</xref>
]. Hymenopterans display some of the most sophisticated learning abilities investigated thus far. The honeybee is capable of learning and interpreting complex waggle dances to encode information about flower location [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref010" ref-type="bibr">10</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref013" ref-type="bibr">13</xref>
]. Meanwhile the bumble bee,
<italic>Bombus terrestris</italic>
, is known to use spatial learning to efficiently locate and navigate flowers [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref014" ref-type="bibr">14</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref015" ref-type="bibr">15</xref>
] and recent work has identified observational learning and even false memories in this species [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref016" ref-type="bibr">16</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref018" ref-type="bibr">18</xref>
]. However, beyond the models described above, much less is known about learning in other insect taxa, particularly phytophagous Hemipterans. This is important because Hemipterans represent a diverse and threatening group of agricultural pests, many of which transmit plant disease pathogens, particularly those in the monophyletic suborder Sternorrhyncha, which includes aphids, whiteflies, psyllids and scale insects. Current models predict that within a century, the occurrence and abundance of agricultural disease vectors will spread latitudinally as climate change expands the range of migration, and across geographically isolated regions as human movement continues to perpetuate invasions by non-native species [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref019" ref-type="bibr">19</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref021" ref-type="bibr">21</xref>
].</p>
<p>To our knowledge, only a few species in the order Hemiptera have been investigated in the context of learning. Patt & Setamou [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref022" ref-type="bibr">22</xref>
] found evidence of olfactory learning in nymphs of the glassy winged sharpshooter (
<italic>Homalodisca vitripennis</italic>
). Orientation towards a visual target was reportedly influenced by experience in a species of the minute pirate bug,
<italic>Orius majusculus</italic>
[
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref023" ref-type="bibr">23</xref>
]. Finally, visual and olfactory learning in the context of host preference have been documented in the predatory bug,
<italic>Rhodnius prolixus</italic>
[
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref024" ref-type="bibr">24</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref028" ref-type="bibr">28</xref>
]. While there is a general lack of knowledge about learning in Hemipterans, even less is known about how learning may affect pest management [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref029" ref-type="bibr">29</xref>
]. Furthermore, the increased demand for pest management strategies, alternative to traditional synthetic pesticides, necessitates investigation of the behavioral and cognitive ecology of agricultural pests.</p>
<p>Of current concern is the introduction and spread of the Asian citrus psyllid,
<italic>Diaphorina citri</italic>
Kuwayama (Hemiptera: Liviidae), a phloem-feeding citrus pest that transmits
<italic>Candidatus</italic>
Liberibacter asiaticus (
<italic>C</italic>
Las), the putative causal agent of citrus greening disease or huanglongbing (HLB) [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref030" ref-type="bibr">30</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref033" ref-type="bibr">33</xref>
]. Management of HLB relies on the intensive use of insecticides to reduce
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
populations [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref031" ref-type="bibr">31</xref>
]; however, insecticide resistance has recently been detected in
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
[
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref034" ref-type="bibr">34</xref>
]. The dependence upon chemical controls in the face of emerging insecticide resistance, along with the recent concerns about neonicotinoids, necessitates the development of alternative control techniques for managing
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
populations [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref035" ref-type="bibr">35</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref036" ref-type="bibr">36</xref>
]. As a result, better tools are needed to detect and monitor this pathogen vector. Development of successful alternative control techniques, such as trap cropping and ‘attract and kill’ strategies will require a thorough understanding of the insect’s ecology and cognitive abilities with respect to finding host plants and conspecifics [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref037" ref-type="bibr">37</xref>
].</p>
<p>Currently, we know that
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
employ several sensory modalities when selecting hosts including vision [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref038" ref-type="bibr">38</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref041" ref-type="bibr">41</xref>
] gustation [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref041" ref-type="bibr">41</xref>
], audition (substrate-borne) [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref042" ref-type="bibr">42</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref043" ref-type="bibr">43</xref>
], and olfaction [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref041" ref-type="bibr">41</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref044" ref-type="bibr">44</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref046" ref-type="bibr">46</xref>
]. Furthermore, the response of the insect to olfactory cues is affected by bacterial infection of plants, which changes the bouquet of volatiles released from infected plants [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref047" ref-type="bibr">47</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref048" ref-type="bibr">48</xref>
]. To some extent, the behaviors of
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
have already been exploited for pest management. Traps for monitoring
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
populations have been optimized to exploit color preferences [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref040" ref-type="bibr">40</xref>
] and olfactory lures based on preferred host plant volatile profiles are under development [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref049" ref-type="bibr">49</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref050" ref-type="bibr">50</xref>
]. However, the success of such applications has been limited, and it is unclear whether the preferences displayed by
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
are learned or innate.</p>
<p>Previous research on feeding behavior in adult
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
indicates a possible role for experience in host selection and acceptance [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref051" ref-type="bibr">51</xref>
], despite strong innate responses to certain host plant volatiles [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref038" ref-type="bibr">38</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref041" ref-type="bibr">41</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref044" ref-type="bibr">44</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref046" ref-type="bibr">46</xref>
]. It remains unclear whether information about stimuli, perceived by developing psyllids, is retained and used in host plant selection as reproductively mature adults. This is an important pest management consideration because if
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
host preferences are experience-dependent, then differences in local citrus cultivar abundance may influence monitoring (i.e., orientation to a target) and the development of alternative control measures (i.e., selection of trap crop cultivar) [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref051" ref-type="bibr">51</xref>
].</p>
<p>The goals of the present study were to investigate the extent of associative learning in
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
in the form of simple and compound conditioning tasks while exploring memory duration, and the relative salience of visual versus olfactory information. Due to the economic importance of this pest species, an ancillary goal was to determine the factors that may contribute to effective application of behavioral modification as a future management tool for
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
and, by extension, other phytopathogen vectors. For this reason, we sought to study learning in the context of host preference. If learning plays an important role in
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
host plant preference, traps and lures tailored to regional
<italic>Citrus</italic>
or ornamental
<italic>Murraya</italic>
diversity and abundance may enhance the efficacy of current monitoring and disruption techniques.</p>
</sec>
<sec sec-type="materials|methods" id="sec002">
<title>Materials and Methods</title>
<sec id="sec003">
<title>Insect colony and Host Plant Maintenance</title>
<p>
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
were obtained from a
<italic>C</italic>
Las-free colony maintained at the University of Florida Citrus Research and Education Center in Lake Alfred, Florida. The psyllids were originally obtained from Valencia orange (
<italic>Citrus sinensis</italic>
L.) trees and were subsequently maintained on Valencia orange and orange jasmine (
<italic>Murraya paniculata</italic>
(L.) Jack.). The trees were fertilized with a granular fertilizer monthly, and weekly with a soil drench fertilizer (MaxiGro
<sup></sup>
). To establish colonies reared on a specific host plant species, the psyllids from the general colony were moved into cages with either potted orange jasmine or sour orange (
<italic>Citrus</italic>
x
<italic>aurantium</italic>
L.) plants. After initial oviposition, general colony adults were removed to ensure that all developing insects were only experienced with the developmental host plant species; either orange jasmine or sour orange. All colonies were maintained at 28°C under a L14:D10 light cycle. To maintain
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
reproduction and ensure maximum plant health, host plants were rotated out of colony cages once per month.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec004">
<title>Y-tube Behavioral Assay Set-up</title>
<p>Olfactory and visual preference tests were performed with a Y-tube apparatus with a 1 cm inner diameter. The length of the Y-tube measured 13 cm from the crux to the release end. The two arms of the Y-tube measured 8 cm from the crux to the odor source ends. The arms of the Y-tube received charcoal filtered and humidified air pumped at 0.2 liters per minute (LPM) from a flowmeter (ARS Inc., Gainesville, FL). The Y-tube was mounted vertically with two white compact fluorescent lights (Sylvania; 13W, 800 lumens) suspended at equal heights from the distal ends of the Y-tube arms. A vertical Y-tube mount was used due to positive phototaxis and negative geotaxis displayed by
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
[
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref052" ref-type="bibr">52</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref053" ref-type="bibr">53</xref>
]. White bulbs were used unless otherwise stated. In visual conditioning experiments involving different light colors, the white bulbs were replaced with colored compact florescent bulbs of the same intensity (Mood-lites
<sup>®</sup>
; 13W, 800 lumens).</p>
<p>To remove the effects of possible differences in heat and light intensity between the different bulbs, the bulbs were presented within a ‘shade’ container that was constructed from white cardstock (white bulb: 81°C unshaded, 39°C shaded; blue bulb: 80°C unshaded, 38°C shaded). This diffused the light and created a consistent 5 cm buffer between the bulbs and the arms of the Y-tube. Using a shade, the heat measured on the arms of the Y-tube was approximately 26°C regardless of bulb color. Ambient room temperature was approximately 23°C. Temperature was measured with a Fluke 62 Max+ Infrared (IR) thermometer. To control for positional bias, the Y-tube was rotated 180° every 10 trials. To control for potential chemical deposition, which could influence
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
behavior [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref045" ref-type="bibr">45</xref>
], the Y-tube was cleaned and replaced every 5 trials. All glassware was cleaned with Sparkleen detergent (Fischerbrand), rinsed with distilled water, rinsed again with acetone, and dried in an oven at 80°C for at least 15 min. Each trial lasted 300s, or until an individual made a selection, which was designated as a minimum 1 cm entry into a particular arm. When a selection was made, the insect was removed from the Y-tube and the trial was ended. Individuals that failed to make a selection within 300 s were designated “nonresponsive.” Data were collected only on days when the response rate was above 80%. The usual response rate was about 90–95% but varied based on barometric pressure. The latency to selection and orientation of the selected arm data (left or right) were recorded in addition to odor selection data. Time to selection data were used to compare response time between male and female
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
, as well as, naïve and experienced
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
in experiment 3.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec005">
<title>Experiment 1: Experience-Dependent Host Plant Preference</title>
<p>To determine the effect of host plant experience on adult female host plant preference,
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
were reared as described above on either sour orange (SO) or orange jasmine (OJ) for two generations. Female
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
collected from each host plant colony were then assayed using a Y-tube olfactometer for host plant odor preference. Each arm was baited with odor from either 0.25 g sour orange leaves or 0.25 g orange jasmine leaves. Foliage used in testing was obtained from caged, undamaged plants that had never been fed upon. The age of the plants used in the olfactometer assays was the same as the plants used in the colonies; approximately 2 years. Both sets of plants were treated similarly (e.g., watering, pruning and fertilizer schedules) with the exception of
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
herbivory. Only young flushing shoots were used and all leaves used in this experiment were visually approximated to maintain consistent leaf age. Adult psyllids used in these tests ranged in age from 4–7 days post-emergence. Only females were used in experiment 1 due low male response level in preliminary tests.</p>
<p>To determine the plasticity of such preferences, shifts in host plant preference after short-term experience feeding on an alternative host plant species was measured in adults. In this test, four-day old adult females reared on orange jasmine were moved to sour orange and assayed for orange jasmine or sour orange preference at 0-, 24-, 48-, and 72 hrs post host plant transfer. To control for handling and transfer of
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
, a separate group was moved from the original host plant (orange jasmine) and placed on new orange jasmine plants in a different cage. These psyllids were assayed at the same time points as the experimental group. This experiment was repeated using sour orange as the natal host plant and orange jasmine as the novel adult host plant.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec006">
<title>Experiment 2: Single Stimulus Conditioning</title>
<p>Two experiments were conducted to investigate differences in stimulus acquisition by adult psyllids across sensory modalities. The first experiment evaluated single stimulus conditioning toward a novel, non-host plant associated volatile, vanillin, while the second experiment evaluated single stimulus conditioning to another novel, non-host plant associated stimulus, blue light. In the first experiment, adult
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
were released onto caged sour orange trees baited with vanillin (Sigma-Aldrich; CAS 121-33-5). Baits were created by adding 5 ml of a 2.5% ethanolic vanillin solution to a cotton wick. Vanillin was dissolved into solution with 100% ethanol. To prevent direct contact by
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
with vanillin, the wicks were enclosed in perforated plastic cups with lids. One bait cup was placed inside the pot of each sour orange plant.
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
were allowed to feed freely on the vanillin baited plants for 72 hrs (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g001">Fig 1</xref>
). After 72 hrs, male and female
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
were assayed for orientation response to vanillin using the Y-tube olfactometry described above. One arm of the Y-tube was baited with 1ml 2.5% vanillin solution on a cotton wick. The other arm was used as a control and was baited with 1 ml of ethanol on a cotton wick. The wicks were air dried for 30 min prior to use to allow the ethanol to fully evaporate. The results of the vanillin experienced
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
were compared with naïve
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
.</p>
<fig id="pone.0149815.g001" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0149815.g001</object-id>
<label>Fig 1</label>
<caption>
<title>Experimental conditioning procedure.</title>
<p>In single stimulus tests, either blue light or vanillin were paired with the host plant. In compound conditioning, all test groups received simultaneous presentation of the visual and olfactory stimuli.</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0149815.g001"></graphic>
</fig>
<p>In the second experiment, adult
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
(age non-specific) were released onto caged sour orange trees illuminated with blue light (Mood-lites
<sup>®</sup>
; compact florescent, 13W) (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g001">Fig 1</xref>
).
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
were allowed to feed freely on the blue illuminated plants for 72 hrs. After 72 hrs, male and female
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
were assayed for orientation response to blue light using modified Y-tube olfactometry. The test treatment arm of the Y-tube was illuminated with blue light as described above, while the control arm was illuminated with white light. The illumination from each light source was isolated by using a solid white divider that was placed between the two arms of the Y-tube, resting upon the crux.</p>
<p>The results of the blue-light experienced
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
were compared with naïve
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
. All adult
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
were at least 4 days old (fully sclerotized and reproductively mature) before use in any part of experiment 2.</p>
</sec>
<sec id="sec007">
<title>Experiment 3: Compound Conditioning</title>
<p>To evaluate the relative salience of olfactory versus visual stimuli, as well as, the ability to acquire complex multi-modal information, compound conditioning was performed on male and female
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
using the olfactory stimulus, vanillin, and the visual stimulus, blue light.
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
(age non-specific) were released onto caged sour orange trees illuminated with blue light and baited with 5 ml of 2.5% vanillin solution.
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
were allowed to feed freely on the plants for 72 hrs. After 72 hrs, male and female
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
were placed in one of six test groups and assayed using Y-tube olfactometry (
<xref ref-type="table" rid="pone.0149815.t001">Table 1</xref>
). The test groups represented Y-tube choice-test permutations, which quantified responses to vanillin and blue light independently and as a compound stimulus. The behavioral results of the experienced
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
were compared with naïve
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
. Similar to experiment 2, all adult
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
were at least 4 days old before use in any part of this study.</p>
<table-wrap id="pone.0149815.t001" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0149815.t001</object-id>
<label>Table 1</label>
<caption>
<title>Compound conditioning experimental test groups.</title>
<p>Each group differs only in stimulus presentation during testing. Each single stimulus is the presentation of the olfactory or visual stimuli alone. The compound stimulus is presentation of the olfactory and visual stimuli simultaneously.</p>
</caption>
<alternatives>
<graphic id="pone.0149815.t001g" xlink:href="pone.0149815.t001"></graphic>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<colgroup span="1">
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
</colgroup>
<thead>
<tr>
<th align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Test Group</th>
<th align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Arm 1</th>
<th align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Arm 2</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">a</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Olfactory</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Blank</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">b</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Visual</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Blank</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">c</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Olfactory</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Visual</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">d</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Olfactory +Visual</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Blank</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">e</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Olfactory +Visual</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Olfactory</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="center" rowspan="1" colspan="1">f</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Olfactory +Visual</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Visual</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</alternatives>
</table-wrap>
</sec>
<sec id="sec008">
<title>Statistical Analysis</title>
<p>Y-tube data from the host preference tests and the simple and compound conditioning tests were analyzed with Chi-square tests for within-group comparisons and Chi-square 2 x 2 contingency tables for between-group comparisons, alpha ≤ 0.05. Within-group comparisons were performed for selection differences towards either arm of the Y-tube (i.e. orange jasmine odor versus sour orange odor; vanillin odor versus blank; white light versus blue light). Between-group comparisons were performed for overall selection differences between groups (i.e. orange jasmine psyllids versus sour orange psyllids; naïve psyllids versus experienced psyllids). Differences between groups at each time point in Experiment 1 were calculated with chi-square contingency tables. Standard error was calculated for binomial data where
<italic>p</italic>
is the proportion selecting the target,
<italic>q</italic>
is the proportion selecting the alternative target, and
<italic>n</italic>
is the number of Bernoulli trials.</p>
<disp-formula id="pone.0149815.e001">
<alternatives>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0149815.e001.jpg" id="pone.0149815.e001g" mimetype="image" position="anchor" orientation="portrait"></graphic>
<mml:math id="M1">
<mml:mrow>
<mml:mi>S</mml:mi>
<mml:mi>E</mml:mi>
<mml:mi>x</mml:mi>
<mml:mo>=</mml:mo>
<mml:msqrt>
<mml:mrow>
<mml:mfrac>
<mml:mrow>
<mml:mi>p</mml:mi>
<mml:mi>q</mml:mi>
</mml:mrow>
<mml:mi>n</mml:mi>
</mml:mfrac>
</mml:mrow>
</mml:msqrt>
</mml:mrow>
</mml:math>
</alternatives>
</disp-formula>
<p>Latency data from the compound conditioning experiment were compared using a generalized linear model with a Gaussian distribution. The model was simplified following a stepwise deletion to remove insignificant interactions (α > 0.10). Only latency data from single stimulus versus control tests were included in those analyses. Latency data were not reported for single stimulus conditioning experiments because the sample size was too small to provide adequate statistical power.</p>
<p>At least two replicates, and as many as eight, were performed for each experiment. The number of individuals tested per replicate was dependent on the variation in response and the number of individuals required for statistical power for that particular experiment. For the analyses described above, the data from all replicates within a given experiment were pooled. All analyses were run in R (Version 3.1.3; the R Foundation for statistical software R; Vienna, Austria).</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec sec-type="results" id="sec009">
<title>Results</title>
<sec id="sec010">
<title>Experiment 1: Host Plant Preference</title>
<p>Between group comparisons of the overall response of sour orange-experienced psyllids and orange jasmine-experienced psyllids suggests that host plant preference was significantly affected by natal host plant type (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 7.89, df = 1, p = 0.005) (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g002">Fig 2</xref>
). Within group comparisons showed that female
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
reared on sour orange plants preferred sour orange to orange jasmine leaf volatiles (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 4, df = 1, p = 0.045). Conversely, female psyllids reared on orange jasmine significantly preferred orange jasmine odor as compared with sour orange (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 3.9, df = 1, p = 0.048).</p>
<fig id="pone.0149815.g002" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0149815.g002</object-id>
<label>Fig 2</label>
<caption>
<title>Developmental host plant preference.</title>
<p>Differences in female
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
preference based on natal host plant species. Asterisks within bars indicate statistically significant differences within groups, while asterisks associated with a bracket indicate differences between groups (χ
<sup>2</sup>
test, *: < 0.05, **: < 0.01).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0149815.g002"></graphic>
</fig>
<p>However, when insects reared on sour orange were transferred to orange jasmine plants as adults, host plant preference shifted towards orange jasmine 48 hrs post-transfer (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g003">Fig 3a</xref>
) (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 5.76, df = 1, p = 0.016). Similarly, a transfer of adult female
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
from the developmental host plant species, orange jasmine, to the alternative host plant species, sour orange, changed those preferences in favor of the most recent host plant that was experienced (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g003">Fig 3b</xref>
). By 48 hrs post host plant transfer,
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
significantly preferred sour orange compared with
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
that had constant exposure to the developmental host plant, which maintained preference for orange jasmine (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 5.32, df = 1, p = 0.021).</p>
<fig id="pone.0149815.g003" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0149815.g003</object-id>
<label>Fig 3</label>
<caption>
<title>Experience dependent host plant preference.</title>
<p>Shifts in preference after short-term adult experience feeding on the alternative host plant species. The labels “Kept on SO” (sour orange) or “Kept on OJ” (orange jasmine) represent the control groups—
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
maintained on a single host plant species for the duration of the study. The experimental groups are labeled as “Moved to SO” or “Moved to OJ.” Significant differences between groups at each time point are indicated by different letters (χ
<sup>2</sup>
test, α≤0.05).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0149815.g003"></graphic>
</fig>
</sec>
<sec id="sec011">
<title>Experiment 2: Single Stimulus Conditioning</title>
<p>There was a significant effect of treatment in the single stimulus learning experiments, with experienced insects showing greater response to the test stimulus than naïve insects. Feeding experience on vanillin-baited plants for 72 hrs was sufficient to produce a significant change in response to that volatile in adult
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
(
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g004">Fig 4a</xref>
). Compared to naïve insects, experienced females (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 7.57, df = 1, p = 0.006) and males (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 3.99, df = 1, p = 0.045) showed significantly greater selection of vanillin. Similarly, feeding experience on plants illuminated with blue light induced a significant change in response compared with naïve insects in both females (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 9.99, df = 1, p = 0.002) and males (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 4.84, df = 1, p = 0.027) (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g004">Fig 4b</xref>
).</p>
<fig id="pone.0149815.g004" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0149815.g004</object-id>
<label>Fig 4</label>
<caption>
<title>Learned response to a novel olfactory stimulus (a) and a visual stimulus (b).</title>
<p>(a) Light gray bars indicate responses to a novel olfactory stimulus (vanillin odor). (b) Blue bars indicate the responses to a novel visual stimulus (blue light). (a, b) White bars indicate responses to the blank control. Asterisks (*) within bars indicate statistically significant differences within groups selecting arm A or arm B, while asterisks (*) outside bars indicate differences between naïve and experienced insects (χ
<sup>2</sup>
test, *: < 0.05, **: < 0.01, ***: <0.001).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0149815.g004"></graphic>
</fig>
</sec>
<sec id="sec012">
<title>Experiment 3: Compound Conditioning</title>
<p>Experience with a bimodal compound stimulus (vanilla odor + blue light) resulted in significantly different responses in female
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
to each stimulus when presented individually (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g005">Fig 5a and 5b</xref>
). In a naïve state, female
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
oriented towards vanillin at the same rate as the unscented control arm, meaning that response to vanillin was neutral. However, after experience, this percentage increased by approximately 19%. Between group comparisons showed that this difference was statistically significant (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 3.81, df = 1, p≤0.05). Naïve female response to blue light increased similarly after experience, from 19 to 45% (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 8.96, df = 1, p = 0.003). Males did not show increases in response to each stimulus when presented individually.</p>
<fig id="pone.0149815.g005" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0149815.g005</object-id>
<label>Fig 5</label>
<caption>
<title>
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
responses after compound conditioning to six test conditions (a-f).</title>
<p>Gray bars represent the olfactory stimulus (vanillin odor), blue bars represent the visual stimulus (blue light), striped blue and gray bars represent the compound stimulus (vanillin odor + blue light), and white bars indicated a blank control. Asterisks (*) within the bars indicate statistically significant differences within a group selecting arm A or arm B (χ
<sup>2</sup>
test, *: < 0.05, **: < 0.01, ***: < 0.001), ****: <0.0001. Asterisks (*) outside the bars indicate differences between a set of naïve and experienced insects for a given test condition (χ
<sup>2</sup>
test).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0149815.g005"></graphic>
</fig>
<p>When the two stimuli were presented on either end of the Y-tube simultaneously, preference shifted as a result of experience (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g005">Fig 5c</xref>
). Naïve males (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 5.67, df = 1, p = 0.02) and females (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 9.28, df = 1, p = 0.002) preferred the arm emitting the olfactory stimulus, while insects that had experienced a combination of vanilla odor + blue light selected the colored and scented arms equally.</p>
<p>When the compound stimulus was presented opposite of the blank control, there were no differences between groups (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g005">Fig 5d</xref>
). However, there was a significant difference in naïve female selection, with preference shifted towards the control arm (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 11.27, df = 1, p<0.0008). This difference was abolished after experience—experienced females showed no preference for either the blank arm or the arm with the compound stimulus. The opposite trend occurred for males; naïve males showed no preference between the compound stimulus and the control; whereas experienced males preferred the control (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 4.59, df = 1, p = 0.03).</p>
<p>The final two tests involved presenting the compound stimulus opposite each of the stimuli individually (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g005">Fig 5e and 5f</xref>
). When the compound stimulus was presented opposite of the olfactory stimulus alone, experienced males showed an increase in response to the compound stimulus (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 4.85, df = 1, p = 0.03) (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g005">Fig 5e</xref>
). There was no difference in female response, (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 2.88, df = 1, p = 0.09). When the compound stimulus was presented opposite of the visual stimulus alone (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g005">Fig 5f</xref>
), there was no statistically significant difference between naïve and experienced insects, although experienced females showed an 18% increase in response to the compound stimulus (χ
<sup>2</sup>
= 3.55, df = 1, p = 0.059).</p>
<p>In addition to behavioral choice data, we also collected latency data. These results revealed differences in response time when making a selection in the 2-choice olfactometer (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g006">Fig 6</xref>
). Comparing the latency to selection for all
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
during single stimulus tests using a GLM, there was a significant effect of sex (females compared to males) and treatment (naïve insects compared to experienced insects) on the time needed for selection (
<xref ref-type="table" rid="pone.0149815.t002">Table 2</xref>
). All of the interactions among sex, treatment, and the sensory modality of the tests (olfactory tasks compared to visual tasks) were insignificant at α < 0.10 and were consequently removed from the model. Time to selection was significantly greater in female
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
as compared to males (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g006">Fig 6a and 6b</xref>
). Additionally, experience was associated with a small but significant decrease in response time compared to naïve insects (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g006">Fig 6b</xref>
). No significant differences were observed between the olfactory and visual tasks (
<xref ref-type="fig" rid="pone.0149815.g006">Fig 6a</xref>
).</p>
<table-wrap id="pone.0149815.t002" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0149815.t002</object-id>
<label>Table 2</label>
<caption>
<title>Results from the GLM with Gaussian distribution.</title>
<p>Results based on the latency data associated with the single stimulus tests in the compound conditioning experiment. All non-significant interactions were removed (α >0.10).</p>
</caption>
<alternatives>
<graphic id="pone.0149815.t002g" xlink:href="pone.0149815.t002"></graphic>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<colgroup span="1">
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
<col align="left" valign="middle" span="1"></col>
</colgroup>
<thead>
<tr>
<th align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Factor</th>
<th align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Df</th>
<th align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">F</th>
<th align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">P-value</th>
</tr>
</thead>
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Sex</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">1, 402</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">22.32</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1"><0.0001</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Treatment</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">1, 402</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">3.94</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.048</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Test</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">1, 402</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">2.36</td>
<td align="char" char="." rowspan="1" colspan="1">0.125</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</alternatives>
</table-wrap>
<fig id="pone.0149815.g006" orientation="portrait" position="float">
<object-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1371/journal.pone.0149815.g006</object-id>
<label>Fig 6</label>
<caption>
<title>Latency to selection for
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
.</title>
<p>Differences in selection time in female (gray bars) and male (white bars) psyllids based on (a) the stimulus modality of the test (olfactory or visual test) and (b) the learning treatment applied to the insects (naïve or experienced). Asterisks (*) indicate significant differences *: < 0.05, **: < 0.01, ***: < 0.001).</p>
</caption>
<graphic xlink:href="pone.0149815.g006"></graphic>
</fig>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec sec-type="conclusions" id="sec013">
<title>Discussion</title>
<p>The host plant preference tests in Experiment 1 compared the behavioral response of
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
to familiar or novel host plant species. We found that female
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
displayed host plant preferences based on experience, such that these insects initially preferred the host plant species on which they were reared. Similar cases of induced host preference have been well documented in
<italic>Manduca sexta</italic>
[
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref054" ref-type="bibr">54</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref055" ref-type="bibr">55</xref>
] and
<italic>Heliothis zea</italic>
[
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref056" ref-type="bibr">56</xref>
], where moth larvae reared from egg on a specific host species display feeding preferences for the species on which they were reared. However, the results of a second experiment showed that those initial developmental preferences were subject to change after adult
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
were moved to an alternative host plant species.
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
preferences shifted to the alternative host plant species within 24–48 hrs following transfer. This suggests that although developmental experience may influence mature adult insect behavior, adult experiences may be more significant in affecting
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
host selection, such that the most recently fed upon plant is preferred.</p>
<p>The single stimulus conditioning tests in Experiment 2 investigated the parameters surrounding learning in
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
in terms of environment-host plant associations, and showed that male and female
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
can associatively learn cues in both olfactory and visual contexts. This reinforces the hypothesis that both olfactory and visual stimuli are important in
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
host selection [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref041" ref-type="bibr">41</xref>
] and confirms previous findings of multimodal learning in an artificial feeding assay [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref051" ref-type="bibr">51</xref>
]. Research on multimodal sensory perception in the hawkmoth,
<italic>Manduca sexta</italic>
, suggests that both forms of sensory information act synergistically, likely aiding in communication between the environment and the foraging insect such that visual cues in combination with olfactory stimuli may provide contextual information [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref057" ref-type="bibr">57</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref059" ref-type="bibr">59</xref>
]. In
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
, the capacity for visual and olfactory learning may increase the likelihood of detecting suitable hosts when migration and dispersal is required.
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
are known to disperse up to 2 kilometers, particularly in search of flushing host plants suitable for oviposition [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref060" ref-type="bibr">60</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref062" ref-type="bibr">62</xref>
].</p>
<p>In Experiment 3, we investigated the relative associative strength of olfactory and visual stimuli in terms of biological relevance to
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
host plant selection behavior. We predicted that the more salient stimulus would show the greatest rate and proportion of learning [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref063" ref-type="bibr">63</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref064" ref-type="bibr">64</xref>
]. The results suggest that female
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
learn the individual components of the compound stimulus separately, as well as the compound stimulus as a whole. In addition, while naïve females display strong preference for the olfactory stimulus compared to the visual stimulus, after conditioning, the two stimuli appear to share similar strength of attraction. These findings suggest that female
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
actively acquire visual and olfactory information about their host plants, similar to that found in previous work [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref041" ref-type="bibr">41</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref051" ref-type="bibr">51</xref>
]. Interestingly, after the compound presentation of the stimuli, we were unable to demonstrate learning in male
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
. Unlike the females, males appeared to show lower capability of learning each stimulus individually and some degree of learned aversion to the compound stimulus. The reasons for this finding are unclear, but it may indicate that the biological significance of the information, or perhaps the way information is stored neurologically, varies depending on the sex of the insect. More experiments are required to better understand these findings and explore those hypotheses.</p>
<p>Along with choice, Experiment 3 also examined the latency prior to behavioral response, which is often used as a measure of decision-making, and in some cases, its own measure of learning [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref065" ref-type="bibr">65</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref066" ref-type="bibr">66</xref>
]. We found that experienced
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
make target selections faster than naïve insects. The most significant difference was dependent on the sex of the insect, with males making more rapid selections than females regardless of task or experience. This may reflect differences in overall selection strategy between the sexes and may suggest that male psyllids have weaker discriminatory abilities than females, which may help explain the sex differences in learned response rate. This is not surprising considering the reproductive role of females and the importance of oviposition site selection for successful nymph development.</p>
<p>Our investigation was not without some limitations. While our olfactory stimulus, vanillin, was truly novel and is not found in association with the
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
host environment, our visual stimulus was not novel. Blue light appears to provoke a strong innate repellent response [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref038" ref-type="bibr">38</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref039" ref-type="bibr">39</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref050" ref-type="bibr">50</xref>
]. In addition, some
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
possess a “blue-green” abdominal color-morph and there is increasing evidence that abdominal color is associated with important behavioral differences among psyllids with respect to dispersal and reproduction [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref060" ref-type="bibr">60</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref067" ref-type="bibr">67</xref>
]. However, we avoided strongly attractive colors such as yellow, orange, red, or green [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref039" ref-type="bibr">39</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref040" ref-type="bibr">40</xref>
], since previous work has indicated that there is little learned increase in
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
behavior when the innate response to the stimulus is positive [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref051" ref-type="bibr">51</xref>
]. Therefore, although the blue light stimulus used in this study does not conform to the characteristics of traditional stimuli used in most learning experiments, we believe it was the appropriate choice for our subject animal. In fact, it revealed a rather interesting result—
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
appeared capable of significantly changing their responses, with blue no longer acting as a repellent after conditioning. This suggests that while psyllids may possess intrinsic aversions to certain stimuli, likely evolutionarily selected to help them avoid non-hosts, those aversions are not so hardwired as to be immutable [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref068" ref-type="bibr">68</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref070" ref-type="bibr">70</xref>
]. The data shown here suggest that short-term exposure to those stimuli within the context of a reinforcing stimulus, such as food, may override the innate responses of these insects, allowing them to maximally benefit from a complex environment of potential hosts, even those which are on the fringes of their oligophagous range.</p>
<p>Based on these data, we conclude that
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
not only learn, but learning may be partially responsible for local host plant preference phenomena [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref038" ref-type="bibr">38</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref046" ref-type="bibr">46</xref>
,
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref071" ref-type="bibr">71</xref>
]. To our knowledge, the findings presented here represent the most extensive study of learning in a sternorrhynchan species. This is significant because sternorrhynchans are an important group of disease vectors and understanding the behavioral ecology involved in host plant preference and selection in these insects is potentially beneficial in the design and implementation of pest monitoring and management programs. Although this work is limited to fairly fundamental questions regarding learning in
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
(eg., what types of stimuli are learned, how long memories are retained), and may at this point in time have limited application, we believe that it lays the foundation for further study of learning of this insect and other related sternorrhynchans. It appears likely that
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
acquire olfactory and visual information about the host plants with which they have experience. Similar to other psyllid species, visual information appears to be as biologically relevant to
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
as olfactory cues in terms of orientation towards a target and association with host plants [
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref072" ref-type="bibr">72</xref>
<xref rid="pone.0149815.ref073" ref-type="bibr">73</xref>
]. Given the observed plasticity in response to visual stimuli seen in these experiments, even brief visual experience with a novel, non-traditional, and initially deterrent host may increase the likelihood of future selection for that species, despite repellant visual properties. This could facilitate vector dispersal by providing temporary refuge for psyllids during migration, or even reduce intraspecific competition by introducing novel host plant options; however, further investigation is needed to fully test this hypothesis. For that reason, future
<italic>D</italic>
.
<italic>citri</italic>
traps may benefit from increased attention to the visual aspects of design, possibly tailoring color, as well as semiochemical lures to the most prevalent host plant varieties within each citrus growing region.</p>
</sec>
<sec sec-type="supplementary-material" id="sec014">
<title>Supporting Information</title>
<supplementary-material content-type="local-data" id="pone.0149815.s001">
<label>S1 Dataset</label>
<caption>
<title>Original data for Experiments 1–3.</title>
<p>Each data file contains those data used for the experiments as described in the text. Each experiment is shown in a separate tab. The latency data collected from experiment 3 is shown in a separate tab as well.</p>
<p>(XLSX)</p>
</caption>
<media xlink:href="pone.0149815.s001.xlsx">
<caption>
<p>Click here for additional data file.</p>
</caption>
</media>
</supplementary-material>
</sec>
</body>
<back>
<ack>
<p>We would like to thank Angel Hoyte, Wendy Meyer, and Karen Addison for their technical assistance. We would also like to thank Holly Shugart, Alex Arp, and Dennis Willett for their helpful advice while preparing the manuscript.</p>
</ack>
<ref-list>
<title>References</title>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref001">
<label>1</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Mori</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>n-Hexacosanol and n-octacosanol; feeding stimulants for larvae of the silkworm,
<italic>Bombyx mori</italic>
</article-title>
.
<source>J. Insect Physiol</source>
.
<year>1982</year>
;
<volume>28</volume>
:
<fpage>969</fpage>
<lpage>973</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref002">
<label>2</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Bernays</surname>
<given-names>EA</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Woodhead</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Haines</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Climbing by newly hatched larvae of the spotted stalk borer
<italic>Chilo partellus</italic>
to the top of sorghum plants</article-title>
.
<source>Entomol. Exp. Appl</source>
.
<year>1985</year>
;
<volume>39</volume>
:
<fpage>73</fpage>
<lpage>79.3</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref003">
<label>3</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Weiss</surname>
<given-names>MR</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Innate colour preferences and flexible colour learning in the pipevine swallowtail</article-title>
.
<source>Anim. Behav</source>
.
<year>1997</year>
;
<volume>53</volume>
(
<issue>5</issue>
):
<fpage>1043</fpage>
<lpage>1052</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref004">
<label>4</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Kelber</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Pattern discrimination in a hawkmoth: innate preferences, learning performance and ecology</article-title>
.
<source>Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B: Biol Sciences</source>
.
<year>2002</year>
;
<volume>269</volume>
(
<issue>1509</issue>
):
<fpage>2573</fpage>
<lpage>2577</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref005">
<label>5</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Riffell</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Alarcón</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Abrell</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Davidowitz</surname>
<given-names>G</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Bronstein</surname>
<given-names>JL</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Hildebrand</surname>
<given-names>JG</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Behavioral consequences of innate preferences and olfactory learning in hawkmoth–flower interactions</article-title>
.
<source>Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci</source>
.
<year>2008</year>
;
<volume>105</volume>
(
<issue>9</issue>
):
<fpage>3404</fpage>
<lpage>3409</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0709811105">10.1073/pnas.0709811105</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18305169</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref006">
<label>6</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Temple</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Bonini</surname>
<given-names>N</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Dawson</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Quinn</surname>
<given-names>W</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Reward learning in normal and mutant
<italic>Drosophila</italic>
</article-title>
.
<source>Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci</source>
.
<year>1983</year>
;
<volume>80</volume>
:
<fpage>1482</fpage>
<lpage>1486</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">6572401</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref007">
<label>7</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Quinn</surname>
<given-names>W</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Harris</surname>
<given-names>W</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Benzer</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Conditioned behavior in
<italic>Drosophila melanogaster</italic>
</article-title>
.
<source>Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A</source>
.
<year>1974</year>
;
<volume>71</volume>
(
<issue>3</issue>
):
<fpage>708</fpage>
<lpage>712</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">4207071</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref008">
<label>8</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Simpson</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Raubenheimer</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>The hungry locust</article-title>
.
<source>Adv. Study of Behav</source>
.
<year>2000</year>
;
<volume>29</volume>
:
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>44</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref009">
<label>9</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Dukas</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Bernays</surname>
<given-names>EA</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Learning improves growth rate in grasshoppers</article-title>
.
<source>Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci</source>
.
<year>2000</year>
;
<volume>97</volume>
(
<issue>6</issue>
):
<fpage>2637</fpage>
<lpage>2640</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">10706621</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref010">
<label>10</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<name>
<surname>von Frisch</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
.
<source>The dance language and orientation of bees</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Harvard University Press</publisher-name>
;
<year>1967</year>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref011">
<label>11</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Menzel</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Giurfa</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Cognitive architecture of a mini-brain: the honeybee</article-title>
.
<source>Trends Cog. Sci</source>
.
<year>2001</year>
;
<volume>5</volume>
(
<issue>2</issue>
):
<fpage>62</fpage>
<lpage>71</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref012">
<label>12</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Menzel</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Muller</surname>
<given-names>U</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Learning and memory in honeybees: from behavior to neural substrates</article-title>
.
<source>Annu. Rev. Neurosci</source>
.
<year>1996</year>
;
<volume>19</volume>
(
<issue>1</issue>
):
<fpage>379</fpage>
<lpage>404</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">8833448</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref013">
<label>13</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Dyer</surname>
<given-names>F</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>The biology of the dance language</article-title>
.
<source>Annu. Rev. Entomol</source>
.
<year>2002</year>
;
<volume>47</volume>
:
<fpage>917</fpage>
<lpage>949</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11729095</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref014">
<label>14</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Carter</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Resource-tracking by bumble bees: responses to plant level differences in quality</article-title>
.
<source>Ecology</source>
.
<year>2004</year>
;
<volume>85</volume>
(
<issue>10</issue>
):
<fpage>2764</fpage>
<lpage>2771</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref015">
<label>15</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Laverty</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Bumble bee learning and flower morphology</article-title>
.
<source>Anim. Behav</source>
.
<year>1994</year>
;
<volume>47</volume>
(
<issue>3</issue>
):
<fpage>531</fpage>
<lpage>545</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref016">
<label>16</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Leadbeater</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Chittka</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>The dynamics of social learning in an insect model, the bumblebee (
<italic>Bombus terrestris</italic>
)</article-title>
.
<source>Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol</source>
.
<year>2007</year>
;
<volume>61</volume>
(
<issue>11</issue>
):
<fpage>1789</fpage>
<lpage>1796</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref017">
<label>17</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Dawson</surname>
<given-names>EH</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Avarguès-Weber</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Chittka</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Leadbeater</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Learning by observation emerges from simple associations in an insect model</article-title>
.
<source>Curr. Biol</source>
.
<year>2013</year>
;
<volume>23</volume>
(
<issue>8</issue>
):
<fpage>727</fpage>
<lpage>730</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.03.035">10.1016/j.cub.2013.03.035</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23562271</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref018">
<label>18</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Hunt</surname>
<given-names>KL</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Chittka</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Merging of long-term memories in an insect</article-title>
.
<source>Curr. Biol</source>
.
<year>2015</year>
;
<volume>25</volume>
(
<issue>6</issue>
):
<fpage>741</fpage>
<lpage>745</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.023">10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.023</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">25728692</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref019">
<label>19</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Githeko</surname>
<given-names>AK</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Lindsay</surname>
<given-names>SW</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Confalonieri</surname>
<given-names>UE</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Patz</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Climate change and vector-borne diseases: a regional analysis</article-title>
.
<source>Bull. WHO</source>
.
<year>2000</year>
;
<volume>78</volume>
(
<issue>9</issue>
):
<fpage>1136</fpage>
<lpage>1147</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11019462</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref020">
<label>20</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Parmesan</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Ecological and evolutionary responses to recent climate change</article-title>
.
<source>Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst</source>
.
<year>2006</year>
;
<volume>37</volume>
:
<fpage>637</fpage>
<lpage>669</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref021">
<label>21</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<name>
<surname>Watson</surname>
<given-names>RT</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Zinyowera</surname>
<given-names>MC</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Moss</surname>
<given-names>RH</given-names>
</name>
.
<source>Climate Change 1995 impacts, adaptations and mitigation of climate change: Scientific-technical analysis</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
;
<year>1996</year>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref022">
<label>22</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Patt</surname>
<given-names>JM</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Sétamou</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Recognition of novel volatile cues by the nymphs of the glassy-winged sharpshooter,
<italic>Homalodisca vitripennis</italic>
(Cicadellidae)</article-title>
.
<source>J. Insect Behav</source>
.
<year>2010</year>
;
<volume>23</volume>
(
<issue>4</issue>
):
<fpage>290</fpage>
<lpage>302</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref023">
<label>23</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Henaut</surname>
<given-names>Y</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Alauzet</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Dargagnon</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Lambin</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Visual learning in larval
<italic>Orius majusculus</italic>
a polyphagous predator</article-title>
.
<source>Entomol. Exp. Appl</source>
.
<year>1999</year>
;
<volume>90</volume>
(
<issue>1</issue>
):
<fpage>103</fpage>
<lpage>107</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref024">
<label>24</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Aldana</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Abramson</surname>
<given-names>CI</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Lizano</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Vegas</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Sulbaran-Romero</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Learning and orientation to odor in the bug
<italic>Rhodnius prolixus</italic>
Stal 1859 under laboratory conditions</article-title>
.
<source>Parasitol. Res</source>
.
<year>2008</year>
;
<volume>103</volume>
(
<issue>3</issue>
):
<fpage>587</fpage>
<lpage>594</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00436-008-1014-4">10.1007/s00436-008-1014-4</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18509679</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref025">
<label>25</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Abramson</surname>
<given-names>CI</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Frasca</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Fehr</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Romero</surname>
<given-names>ES</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Lizano</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Aldana</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Psychology of learning: a new approach to study behavior of
<italic>Rhodnius prolixus</italic>
stal under laboratory conditions 1, 2</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Rep</source>
.
<year>2005</year>
;
<volume>97</volume>
(
<issue>3</issue>
):
<fpage>721</fpage>
<lpage>731</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16512287</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref026">
<label>26</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Vinauger</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Buratti</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Lazzari</surname>
<given-names>CR</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Learning the way to blood: first evidence of dual olfactory conditioning in a blood-sucking insect,
<italic>Rhodnius prolixus</italic>
. I. Appetitive learning</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Biol</source>
.
<year>2011</year>
;
<volume>214</volume>
(
<issue>18</issue>
):
<fpage>3032</fpage>
<lpage>3038</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21865515</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref027">
<label>27</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Vinauger</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Buratti</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Lazzari</surname>
<given-names>CR</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Learning the way to blood: first evidence of dual olfactory conditioning in a blood-sucking insect,
<italic>Rhodnius prolixus</italic>
. II. Aversive learning</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Biol</source>
.
<year>2011</year>
;
<volume>214</volume>
(
<issue>18</issue>
):
<fpage>3039</fpage>
<lpage>3045</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21865516</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref028">
<label>28</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Vinauger</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Pereira</surname>
<given-names>MH</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Lazzari</surname>
<given-names>CR</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Learned host preference in a Chagas disease vector,
<italic>Rhodnius prolixus</italic>
</article-title>
.
<source>Acta tropica</source>
.
<year>2012</year>
;
<volume>122</volume>
(
<issue>1</issue>
):
<fpage>24</fpage>
<lpage>28</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actatropica.2011.11.007">10.1016/j.actatropica.2011.11.007</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22138145</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref029">
<label>29</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<name>
<surname>Prokopy</surname>
<given-names>RJ</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Lewis</surname>
<given-names>JW</given-names>
</name>
.
<chapter-title>Application of learning to pest management</chapter-title>
In
<name>
<surname>Papaj</surname>
<given-names>DR</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Lewis</surname>
<given-names>AC</given-names>
</name>
, editors.
<source>Insect Learning: Ecology and Evolutionary Perspectives</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
,
<publisher-name>Chapman and Hall</publisher-name>
;
<year>1993</year>
pp.
<fpage>308</fpage>
<lpage>342</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref030">
<label>30</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Halbert</surname>
<given-names>SE</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Manjunath</surname>
<given-names>KL</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Asian citrus psyllids (Sternorrhyncha: Psyllidae) and greening disease of citrus: A literature review and assessment of risk in Florida</article-title>
.
<source>Fla. Entomol</source>
.
<year>2004</year>
;
<volume>87</volume>
(
<issue>3</issue>
):
<fpage>330</fpage>
<lpage>353</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref031">
<label>31</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Bové</surname>
<given-names>JM</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Huanglongbing: a destructive, newly-emerging, century-old disease of citrus</article-title>
.
<source>J. Plant Path</source>
.
<year>2006</year>
;
<volume>88</volume>
(
<issue>1</issue>
):
<fpage>7</fpage>
<lpage>37</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref032">
<label>32</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Grafton-Cardwell</surname>
<given-names>EE</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Stelinski</surname>
<given-names>LL</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Stansly</surname>
<given-names>PA</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Biology and management of Asian citrus psyllid, vector of the huanglongbing pathogens</article-title>
.
<source>Annu. Rev. Entomol</source>
.
<year>2013</year>
;
<volume>58</volume>
:
<fpage>413</fpage>
<lpage>432</lpage>
,
<fpage>31</fpage>
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-ento-120811-153542">10.1146/annurev-ento-120811-153542</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23317046</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref033">
<label>33</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<name>
<surname>Hodges</surname>
<given-names>AW</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Spreen</surname>
<given-names>TH</given-names>
</name>
.
<chapter-title>Economic impacts of citrus greening (HLB) in Florida, 2006/07–2010/11</chapter-title>
<source>In Electronic Data Information Source (EDIS) FE903</source>
(p.
<fpage>32611</fpage>
).
<publisher-name>University of Florida</publisher-name>
<publisher-loc>Gainesville, FL</publisher-loc>
<year>2012</year>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref034">
<label>34</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Tiwari</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Mann</surname>
<given-names>RS</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Rogers</surname>
<given-names>ME</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Stelinski</surname>
<given-names>LL</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Insecticide resistance in field populations of Asian citrus psyllid in Florida</article-title>
.
<source>Pest Manage. Sci</source>
.
<year>2011</year>
;
<volume>67</volume>
(
<issue>10</issue>
):
<fpage>1258</fpage>
<lpage>1268</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref035">
<label>35</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Martini</surname>
<given-names>X</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Pelz-Stelinski</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Stelinski</surname>
<given-names>LL</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Absence of windbreaks, and solid set plantings increase density of Asian citrus psyllid</article-title>
.
<source>Agric. Ecosyst. Environ</source>
.
<year>2015</year>
;
<volume>212</volume>
:
<fpage>168</fpage>
<lpage>174</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref036">
<label>36</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Croxton</surname>
<given-names>SD</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Stansly</surname>
<given-names>PA</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Metalized polyethylene mulch to repel Asian citrus psyllid, slow spread of huanglongbing and improve growth of new citrus plantings Pest Manage</article-title>
.
<source>Sci</source>
.
<year>2014</year>
;
<volume>70</volume>
(
<issue>2</issue>
):
<fpage>318</fpage>
<lpage>323</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref037">
<label>37</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Cook</surname>
<given-names>SM</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Khan</surname>
<given-names>ZR</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Pickett</surname>
<given-names>JA</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>The use of push-pull strategies in integrated pest management</article-title>
.
<source>Annu. Rev. Entomol</source>
.
<year>2006</year>
;
<volume>52</volume>
(
<issue>1</issue>
):
<fpage>375</fpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16968206</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref038">
<label>38</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Hall</surname>
<given-names>DG</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Hentz</surname>
<given-names>MG</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Ciomperlik</surname>
<given-names>MA</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>A comparison of traps and stem tap sampling for monitoring adult Asian citrus psyllid (Hemiptera: Psyllidae) in citrus</article-title>
.
<source>Fla. Entomol</source>
.
<year>2007</year>
;
<volume>90</volume>
(
<issue>2</issue>
):
<fpage>327</fpage>
<lpage>334</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref039">
<label>39</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Paris</surname>
<given-names>TM</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Croxton</surname>
<given-names>SD</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Stansly</surname>
<given-names>PA</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Allan</surname>
<given-names>SA</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Temporal response and attraction of
<italic>Diaphorina citri</italic>
to visual stimuli</article-title>
.
<source>Entomol. Exp. Appl</source>
.
<year>2015</year>
;
<volume>155</volume>
(
<issue>2</issue>
):
<fpage>137</fpage>
<lpage>147</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref040">
<label>40</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Sétamou</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Sanchez</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Saldaña</surname>
<given-names>RR</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Patt</surname>
<given-names>JM</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Summy</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Visual responses of adult Asian citrus psyllid (Hemiptera: Liviidae) to colored sticky traps on citrus trees</article-title>
.
<source>J. Insect Behav</source>
.
<year>2014</year>
;
<volume>27</volume>
(
<issue>4</issue>
):
<fpage>540</fpage>
<lpage>553</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref041">
<label>41</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Patt</surname>
<given-names>JM</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Meikle</surname>
<given-names>WG</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Mafra-Neto</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Sétamou</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Mangan</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Yang</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
,
<etal>et al</etal>
<article-title>Multimodal cues drive host-plant assessment in Asian citrus psyllid (
<italic>Diaphorina citri</italic>
)</article-title>
.
<source>Environ. Entomol</source>
.
<year>2011</year>
;
<volume>40</volume>
(
<issue>6</issue>
):
<fpage>1494</fpage>
<lpage>1502</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1603/EN11149">10.1603/EN11149</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22217766</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref042">
<label>42</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Mankin</surname>
<given-names>RW</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Rohde</surname>
<given-names>BB</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Mcneill</surname>
<given-names>SA</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Paris</surname>
<given-names>TM</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Zagvazdina</surname>
<given-names>NI</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Greenfeder</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>
<italic>Diaphorina citri</italic>
(Hemiptera: Liviidae) responses to microcontroller-buzzer communication signals of potential use in vibration traps</article-title>
.
<source>Fla. Entomol</source>
.
<year>2013</year>
;
<volume>96</volume>
(
<issue>4</issue>
):
<fpage>1546</fpage>
<lpage>1555</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref043">
<label>43</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Rohde</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Paris</surname>
<given-names>TM</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Heatherington</surname>
<given-names>EM</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Hall</surname>
<given-names>DG</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Mankin</surname>
<given-names>RW</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Responses of
<italic>Diaphorina citri</italic>
(Hemiptera: Psyllidae) to conspecific vibrational signals and synthetic mimics</article-title>
.
<source>Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am</source>
.
<year>2013</year>
;
<volume>106</volume>
:
<fpage>392</fpage>
<lpage>399</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref044">
<label>44</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Mann</surname>
<given-names>RS</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Ali</surname>
<given-names>JG</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Hermann</surname>
<given-names>SL</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Tiwari</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Pelz-Stelinski</surname>
<given-names>KS</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Alborn</surname>
<given-names>HT</given-names>
</name>
,
<etal>et al</etal>
<article-title>Induced release of a plant-defense volatile ‘deceptively’attracts insect vectors to plants infected with a bacterial pathogen</article-title>
.
<source>PLoS Pathog</source>
.
<year>2012</year>
;
<volume>8</volume>
(
<issue>3</issue>
):
<fpage>e1002610</fpage>
<lpage>e1002610</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1002610">10.1371/journal.ppat.1002610</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">22457628</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref045">
<label>45</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Martini</surname>
<given-names>X</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Kuhns</surname>
<given-names>EH</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Hoyte</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Stelinski</surname>
<given-names>LL</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Plant volatiles and density-dependent conspecific female odors are used by Asian citrus psyllid to evaluate host suitability on a spatial scale</article-title>
.
<source>Arthropod Plant Interact</source>
.
<year>2014</year>
;
<volume>8</volume>
(
<issue>5</issue>
):
<fpage>453</fpage>
<lpage>460</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref046">
<label>46</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Patt</surname>
<given-names>JM</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Setamou</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Responses of the Asian citrus psyllid to volatiles emitted by the flushing shoots of its rutaceous host plants</article-title>
.
<source>Environ. Entomol</source>
.
<year>2010b</year>
;
<volume>39</volume>
(
<issue>2</issue>
):
<fpage>618</fpage>
<lpage>624</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20388295</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref047">
<label>47</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Aksenov</surname>
<given-names>AA</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Martini</surname>
<given-names>X</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Zhao</surname>
<given-names>W</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Stelinski</surname>
<given-names>LL</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Davis</surname>
<given-names>CE</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Synthetic blends of volatile, phytopathogen-induced odorants can be used to manipulate vector behavior</article-title>
.
<source>Front. Ecol. Evol</source>
.
<year>2014</year>
;
<volume>2</volume>
:
<fpage>78</fpage>
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2014.00078">10.3389/fevo.2014.00078</ext-link>
</comment>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref048">
<label>48</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Mann</surname>
<given-names>RS</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Rouseff</surname>
<given-names>RL</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Smoot</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Rao</surname>
<given-names>N</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Meyer</surname>
<given-names>WL</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Lapointe</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
,
<etal>et al</etal>
<article-title>Chemical and behavioral analysis of the cuticular hydrocarbons from Asian citrus psyllid,
<italic>Diaphorina citri</italic>
</article-title>
.
<source>Insect Sci</source>
.
<year>2013</year>
;
<volume>20</volume>
(
<issue>3</issue>
):
<fpage>367</fpage>
<lpage>378</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-7917.2012.01541.x">10.1111/j.1744-7917.2012.01541.x</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23955888</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref049">
<label>49</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Godfrey</surname>
<given-names>KE</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Galindo</surname>
<given-names>C</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Patt</surname>
<given-names>JM</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Luque-Williams</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Evaluation of Color and Scent Attractants Used to Trap and Detect Asian Citrus Psyllid (Hemiptera: Liviidae) in Urban Environments</article-title>
.
<source>Fla. Entomol</source>
.
<year>2013</year>
;
<volume>96</volume>
(
<issue>4</issue>
):
<fpage>1406</fpage>
<lpage>1416</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref050">
<label>50</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Coutinho-Abreu</surname>
<given-names>IV</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>McInally</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Forster</surname>
<given-names>L</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Luck</surname>
<given-names>R</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Ray</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Odor coding in a disease-transmitting herbivorous insect, the Asian citrus psyllid</article-title>
.
<source>Chem. Senses</source>
.
<year>2014</year>
;
<volume>39</volume>
(
<issue>6</issue>
):
<fpage>539</fpage>
<lpage>549</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/chemse/bju023">10.1093/chemse/bju023</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">24904081</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref051">
<label>51</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Patt</surname>
<given-names>JM</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Stockton</surname>
<given-names>D</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Meikle</surname>
<given-names>WG</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Sétamou</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Mafra-Neto</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Adamczyk</surname>
<given-names>JJ</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Innate and conditioned responses to chemosensory and visual cues in Asian citrus psyllid,
<italic>Diaphorina citri</italic>
(Hemiptera: Liviidae), vector of Huanglongbing pathogens</article-title>
.
<source>Insects</source>
.
<year>2014</year>
;
<volume>5</volume>
(
<issue>4</issue>
):
<fpage>921</fpage>
<lpage>941</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/insects5040921">10.3390/insects5040921</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">26462949</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref052">
<label>52</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Stelinski</surname>
<given-names>LL</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Tiwari</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Vertical T-maze choice assay for arthropod response to odorants</article-title>
.
<source>J Vis Exp</source>
.
<year>2013</year>
;
<volume>72</volume>
:
<fpage>e50229</fpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref053">
<label>53</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Mann</surname>
<given-names>RS</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Rouseff</surname>
<given-names>RL</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Smoot</surname>
<given-names>JM</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Castle</surname>
<given-names>WS</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Stelinski</surname>
<given-names>LL</given-names>
</name>
. (
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Sulfur volatiles from Allium spp. affect Asian citrus psyllid,
<italic>Diaphorina citri</italic>
Kuwayama (Hemiptera: Psyllidae), response to citrus volatiles</article-title>
.
<source>Bull Entomol Res</source>
. 2011;
<volume>101</volume>
(
<issue>01</issue>
):
<fpage>89</fpage>
<lpage>97</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20609277</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref054">
<label>54</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Yamamoto</surname>
<given-names>RT</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Fraenkel</surname>
<given-names>GS</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>The specificity of the tobacco hornworm,
<italic>Protoparce sexta</italic>
, to solanaceous plants</article-title>
.
<source>Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am</source>
.
<year>1960</year>
;
<volume>53</volume>
(
<issue>4</issue>
):
<fpage>503</fpage>
<lpage>507</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref055">
<label>55</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Städler</surname>
<given-names>E</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Hanson</surname>
<given-names>FE</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Food discrimination and induction of preference for artificial diets in the tobacco hornworm,
<italic>Manduca sexta</italic>
</article-title>
.
<source>Physiol. Entomol</source>
.
<year>1978</year>
;
<volume>3</volume>
(
<issue>2</issue>
):
<fpage>121</fpage>
<lpage>133</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref056">
<label>56</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Jermy</surname>
<given-names>T</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Hanson</surname>
<given-names>FE</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Dethier</surname>
<given-names>VG</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Induction of specific food preference in lepidopterous larvae</article-title>
.
<source>Entomol. Exp. Appl</source>
.
<year>1968</year>
;
<volume>11</volume>
(
<issue>2</issue>
):
<fpage>211</fpage>
<lpage>230</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref057">
<label>57</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Raguso</surname>
<given-names>RA</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Willis</surname>
<given-names>MA</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Synergy between visual and olfactory cues in nectar feeding by naıve hawkmoths,
<italic>Manduca sexta</italic>
</article-title>
.
<source>Anim. Behav</source>
.
<year>2002</year>
;
<volume>64</volume>
(
<issue>5</issue>
):
<fpage>685</fpage>
<lpage>695</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref058">
<label>58</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Couvillon</surname>
<given-names>PA</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Bitterman</surname>
<given-names>ME</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Compound-component and conditional discrimination of colors and odors by honeybees; further tests of a continuity model</article-title>
.
<source>Anim. Learn. Behav</source>
.
<year>1998</year>
;
<volume>16</volume>
:
<fpage>67</fpage>
<lpage>74</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref059">
<label>59</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Goyret</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Pfaff</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Raguso</surname>
<given-names>RA</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Kelber</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Why do
<italic>Manduca sexta</italic>
feed from white flowers? Innate and learnt colour preferences in a hawkmoth</article-title>
.
<source>Naturwissenschaften</source>
.
<year>2008</year>
;
<volume>95</volume>
(
<issue>6</issue>
):
<fpage>569</fpage>
<lpage>576</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00114-008-0350-7">10.1007/s00114-008-0350-7</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18288469</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref060">
<label>60</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Martini</surname>
<given-names>X</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Hoyte</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Stelinski</surname>
<given-names>LL</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Abdominal color of the Asian citrus psyllid (Hemiptera: Liviidae) is associated with flight capabilities</article-title>
.
<source>Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am</source>
.
<year>2014</year>
;
<volume>107</volume>
(
<issue>4</issue>
):
<fpage>842</fpage>
<lpage>847</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref061">
<label>61</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Lewis-Rosenblum</surname>
<given-names>H</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Martini</surname>
<given-names>X</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Tiwari</surname>
<given-names>S</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Stelinski</surname>
<given-names>LL</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Seasonal movement patterns and long-range dispersal of Asian citrus psyllid in Florida citrus</article-title>
.
<source>J. Econ. Entomol</source>
.
<year>2015</year>
;
<volume>108</volume>
(
<issue>1</issue>
):
<fpage>3</fpage>
<lpage>10</lpage>
.
<comment>doi:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jee/tou008">10.1093/jee/tou008</ext-link>
</comment>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">26470097</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref062">
<label>62</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Hall</surname>
<given-names>DG</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Hentz</surname>
<given-names>MG</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Seasonal flight activity by the Asian citrus psyllid in east central Florida</article-title>
.
<source>Entomol. Exp. Appl</source>
.
<year>2011</year>
;
<volume>139</volume>
(
<issue>1</issue>
):
<fpage>75</fpage>
<lpage>85</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref063">
<label>63</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<name>
<surname>Rescorla</surname>
<given-names>RA</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Wagner</surname>
<given-names>AR</given-names>
</name>
.
<chapter-title>A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement</chapter-title>
In
<name>
<surname>Black</surname>
<given-names>AH</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Prokasy</surname>
<given-names>WF</given-names>
</name>
, Editors.
<source>Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Appleton-Century-Crofts</publisher-name>
<year>1972</year>
pp.
<fpage>64</fpage>
<lpage>99</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref064">
<label>64</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Miller</surname>
<given-names>RR</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Barnet</surname>
<given-names>RC</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Grahame</surname>
<given-names>NJ</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Assessment of the Rescorla-Wagner model</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Bull</source>
.
<year>1995</year>
;
<volume>117</volume>
(
<issue>3</issue>
):
<fpage>363</fpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">7777644</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref065">
<label>65</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Morris</surname>
<given-names>RG</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Spatial localization does not require the presence of local cues</article-title>
.
<source>Learn. Motiv</source>
.
<year>1981</year>
;
<volume>12</volume>
(
<issue>2</issue>
):
<fpage>239</fpage>
<lpage>260</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref066">
<label>66</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Wessnitzer</surname>
<given-names>J</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Mangan</surname>
<given-names>M</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Webb</surname>
<given-names>B</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Place memory in crickets</article-title>
.
<source>Proc. R. Soc. B. Biol. Sci</source>
.
<year>2008</year>
;
<volume>275</volume>
(
<issue>1637</issue>
):
<fpage>915</fpage>
<lpage>921</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref067">
<label>67</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Wenninger</surname>
<given-names>EJ</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Hall</surname>
<given-names>DG</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Daily and seasonal patterns in abdominal color in
<italic>Diaphorina citri</italic>
(Hemiptera: Psyllidae)</article-title>
.
<source>Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am</source>
.
<year>2008</year>
;
<volume>101</volume>
(
<issue>3</issue>
):
<fpage>585</fpage>
<lpage>592</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref068">
<label>68</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Huang</surname>
<given-names>XP</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Renwick</surname>
<given-names>JAA</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Cross habituation to feeding deterrents and acceptance of a marginal host plant by
<italic>Pieris rapae</italic>
larvae</article-title>
.
<source>Entomol. Exp. Appl</source>
.
<year>1995</year>
;
<volume>76</volume>
(
<issue>3</issue>
):
<fpage>295</fpage>
<lpage>302</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref069">
<label>69</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Szentesi</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Bernays</surname>
<given-names>EA</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>A study of behavioural habituation to a feeding deterrent in nymphs of
<italic>Schistocerca gregaria</italic>
</article-title>
.
<source>Physiol. Entomol</source>
.
<year>1984</year>
;
<volume>9</volume>
(
<issue>3</issue>
):
<fpage>329</fpage>
<lpage>340</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref070">
<label>70</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="other">Gill JS. Studies on insect feeding deterrents with special reference to the fruit extracts of the neem tree,
<italic>Azadirachta indiea</italic>
A. Juss. Ph.D. Thesis. University of London. 1972.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref071">
<label>71</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Chan</surname>
<given-names>STL</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Abang</surname>
<given-names>F</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Beattie</surname>
<given-names>A</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Kueh</surname>
<given-names>RJH</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Wong</surname>
<given-names>SK</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Influence of host plant species and flush growth stage on the Asian citrus psyllid, Diaphorina citri Kuwayama</article-title>
.
<source>Am. J. Agric. Biol. Sci</source>
.
<year>2011</year>
;
<volume>6</volume>
(
<issue>4</issue>
):
<fpage>536</fpage>
<lpage>543</lpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref072">
<label>72</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Farnier</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Dyer</surname>
<given-names>AG</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Steinbauer</surname>
<given-names>MJ</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Related but not alike: not all Hemiptera are attracted to yellow</article-title>
.
<source>Front. Ecol. Evol</source>
.
<year>2014</year>
;
<volume>2</volume>
:
<fpage>67</fpage>
.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="pone.0149815.ref073">
<label>73</label>
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<name>
<surname>Farnier</surname>
<given-names>K</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Dyer</surname>
<given-names>AG</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Taylor</surname>
<given-names>GS</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Peters</surname>
<given-names>RA</given-names>
</name>
,
<name>
<surname>Steinbauer</surname>
<given-names>MJ</given-names>
</name>
.
<article-title>Visual acuity trade-offs and microhabitat-driven adaptation of searching behaviour in psyllids (Hemiptera: Psylloidea: Aphalaridae)</article-title>
.
<source>J. Exp. Biol</source>
.
<year>2015</year>
;
<volume>218</volume>
(
<issue>10</issue>
):
<fpage>1564</fpage>
<lpage>1571</lpage>
.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">25827835</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</pmc>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Wicri/Bois/explor/OrangerV1/Data/Pmc/Corpus
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 000378 | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Corpus/biblio.hfd -nk 000378 | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Wicri/Bois
   |area=    OrangerV1
   |flux=    Pmc
   |étape=   Corpus
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     PMC:4773162
   |texte=   The Influence of Learning on Host Plant Preference in a Significant Phytopathogen Vector, Diaphorina citri
}}

Pour générer des pages wiki

HfdIndexSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Corpus/RBID.i   -Sk "pubmed:26930355" \
       | HfdSelect -Kh $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Corpus/biblio.hfd   \
       | NlmPubMed2Wicri -a OrangerV1 

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.25.
Data generation: Sat Dec 3 17:11:04 2016. Site generation: Wed Mar 6 18:18:32 2024