Serveur d'exploration Tamazight

Attention, ce site est en cours de développement !
Attention, site généré par des moyens informatiques à partir de corpus bruts.
Les informations ne sont donc pas validées.
***** Acces problem to record *****\

Identifieur interne : 000186 ( Pmc/Corpus ); précédent : 0001859; suivant : 0001870 ***** probable Xml problem with record *****

Links to Exploration step


Le document en format XML

<record>
<TEI>
<teiHeader>
<fileDesc>
<titleStmt>
<title xml:lang="en">Order of the major constituents in sign languages: implications for all language</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Napoli, Donna Jo" sort="Napoli, Donna Jo" uniqKey="Napoli D" first="Donna Jo" last="Napoli">Donna Jo Napoli</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="aff1">
<institution>Department of Linguistics, Swarthmore College</institution>
<country>Swarthmore, PA, USA</country>
</nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sutton Spence, Rachel" sort="Sutton Spence, Rachel" uniqKey="Sutton Spence R" first="Rachel" last="Sutton-Spence">Rachel Sutton-Spence</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="aff2">
<institution>Post-Graduate Department of Translation, Federal University of Santa Catarina</institution>
<country>Florianopolis, Brazil</country>
</nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
</titleStmt>
<publicationStmt>
<idno type="wicri:source">PMC</idno>
<idno type="pmid">24860523</idno>
<idno type="pmc">4026690</idno>
<idno type="url">http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4026690</idno>
<idno type="RBID">PMC:4026690</idno>
<idno type="doi">10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00376</idno>
<date when="2014">2014</date>
<idno type="wicri:Area/Pmc/Corpus">000186</idno>
<idno type="wicri:explorRef" wicri:stream="Pmc" wicri:step="Corpus" wicri:corpus="PMC">000186</idno>
</publicationStmt>
<sourceDesc>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<title xml:lang="en" level="a" type="main">Order of the major constituents in sign languages: implications for all language</title>
<author>
<name sortKey="Napoli, Donna Jo" sort="Napoli, Donna Jo" uniqKey="Napoli D" first="Donna Jo" last="Napoli">Donna Jo Napoli</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="aff1">
<institution>Department of Linguistics, Swarthmore College</institution>
<country>Swarthmore, PA, USA</country>
</nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sutton Spence, Rachel" sort="Sutton Spence, Rachel" uniqKey="Sutton Spence R" first="Rachel" last="Sutton-Spence">Rachel Sutton-Spence</name>
<affiliation>
<nlm:aff id="aff2">
<institution>Post-Graduate Department of Translation, Federal University of Santa Catarina</institution>
<country>Florianopolis, Brazil</country>
</nlm:aff>
</affiliation>
</author>
</analytic>
<series>
<title level="j">Frontiers in Psychology</title>
<idno type="eISSN">1664-1078</idno>
<imprint>
<date when="2014">2014</date>
</imprint>
</series>
</biblStruct>
</sourceDesc>
</fileDesc>
<profileDesc>
<textClass></textClass>
</profileDesc>
</teiHeader>
<front>
<div type="abstract" xml:lang="en">
<p>A survey of reports of sign order from 42 sign languages leads to a handful of generalizations. Two accounts emerge, one amodal and the other modal. We argue that universal pressures are at work with respect to some generalizations, but that pressure from the visual modality is at work with respect to others. Together, these pressures conspire to make all sign languages order their major constituents SOV or SVO. This study leads us to the conclusion that the order of S with regard to verb phrase (VP) may be driven by sensorimotor system concerns that feed universal grammar.</p>
</div>
</front>
<back>
<div1 type="bibliography">
<listBibl>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aboh, E" uniqKey="Aboh E">E. Aboh</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pfau, R" uniqKey="Pfau R">R. Pfau</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zeshan, U" uniqKey="Zeshan U">U. Zeshan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Akach, P A O" uniqKey="Akach P">P. A. O. Akach</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Alexiadou, A" uniqKey="Alexiadou A">A. Alexiadou</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Anagnostopoulou, E" uniqKey="Anagnostopoulou E">E. Anagnostopoulou</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Antinucci, F" uniqKey="Antinucci F">F. Antinucci</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Duranti, A" uniqKey="Duranti A">A. Duranti</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gebert, L" uniqKey="Gebert L">L. Gebert</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aoun, J" uniqKey="Aoun J">J. Aoun</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Benmamoun, E" uniqKey="Benmamoun E">E. Benmamoun</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sportiche, D" uniqKey="Sportiche D">D. Sportiche</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aronoff, M" uniqKey="Aronoff M">M. Aronoff</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meir, I" uniqKey="Meir I">I. Meir</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Padden, C" uniqKey="Padden C">C. Padden</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sandler, W" uniqKey="Sandler W">W. Sandler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bahan, B" uniqKey="Bahan B">B. Bahan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Baker, C" uniqKey="Baker C">C. Baker</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cokely, D" uniqKey="Cokely D">D. Cokely</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Baker, M C" uniqKey="Baker M">M. C. Baker</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Baron, I" uniqKey="Baron I">I. Baron</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bergman, B" uniqKey="Bergman B">B. Bergman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wallin, L" uniqKey="Wallin L">L. Wallin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bickerton, D" uniqKey="Bickerton D">D. Bickerton</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bickerton, D" uniqKey="Bickerton D">D. Bickerton</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bobaljik, J D" uniqKey="Bobaljik J">J. D. Bobaljik</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Carnie, A H" uniqKey="Carnie A">A. H. Carnie</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bobillo Garcia, N" uniqKey="Bobillo Garcia N">N. Bobillo-García</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Morales L Pez, E" uniqKey="Morales L Pez E">E. Morales-López</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Reigosa Varela, C" uniqKey="Reigosa Varela C">C. Reigosa-Varela</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Freire Rodriguez, C" uniqKey="Freire Rodriguez C">C. Freire-Rodríguez</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Perez Casanova, C" uniqKey="Perez Casanova C">C. Pérez-Casanova</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bogaerde, E M" uniqKey="Bogaerde E">E. M. Bogaerde</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Den" uniqKey="Van Den">van den</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mills, A E" uniqKey="Mills A">A. E. Mills</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Borsley, R D" uniqKey="Borsley R">R. D. Borsley</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Roberts, I" uniqKey="Roberts I">I. Roberts</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bouchard, D" uniqKey="Bouchard D">D. Bouchard</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bouchard, D" uniqKey="Bouchard D">D. Bouchard</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dubuisson, C" uniqKey="Dubuisson C">C. Dubuisson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Boyes Braem, P" uniqKey="Boyes Braem P">P. Boyes Braem</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fournier, M L" uniqKey="Fournier M">M.-L. Fournier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rickli, F" uniqKey="Rickli F">F. Rickli</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Corrazza, S" uniqKey="Corrazza S">S. Corrazza</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Franchi, M L" uniqKey="Franchi M">M.-L. Franchi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Volterra, V" uniqKey="Volterra V">V. Volterra</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Boyes Braem, P" uniqKey="Boyes Braem P">P. Boyes Braem</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pizzuto, E" uniqKey="Pizzuto E">E. Pizzuto</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Volterra, V" uniqKey="Volterra V">V. Volterra</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Branchini, C" uniqKey="Branchini C">C. Branchini</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Geraci, C" uniqKey="Geraci C">C. Geraci</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Giudice, S" uniqKey="Giudice S">S. Giudice</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mereghetti, E" uniqKey="Mereghetti E">E. Mereghetti</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Braze, D" uniqKey="Braze D">D. Braze</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brennan, M" uniqKey="Brennan M">M. Brennan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brennan, M" uniqKey="Brennan M">M. Brennan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brew, C" uniqKey="Brew C">C. Brew</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schulte Im Walde, S" uniqKey="Schulte Im Walde S">S. Schulte im Walde</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Canale, M" uniqKey="Canale M">M. Canale</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Carnie, A" uniqKey="Carnie A">A. Carnie</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Guilfoyle, E" uniqKey="Guilfoyle E">E. Guilfoyle</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Carston, R" uniqKey="Carston R">R. Carston</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cecchetto, C" uniqKey="Cecchetto C">C. Cecchetto</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Geraci, C" uniqKey="Geraci C">C. Geraci</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zucchi, S" uniqKey="Zucchi S">S. Zucchi</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chen Pichler, D" uniqKey="Chen Pichler D">D. Chen Pichler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chen Pichler, D" uniqKey="Chen Pichler D">D. Chen Pichler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schalber, K" uniqKey="Schalber K">K. Schalber</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hochgesang, J" uniqKey="Hochgesang J">J. Hochgesang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Milkovic, M" uniqKey="Milkovic M">M. Milkovic</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wilbur, R" uniqKey="Wilbur R">R. Wilbur</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vulje, M" uniqKey="Vulje M">M. Vulje</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Choe, H S" uniqKey="Choe H">H. S. Choe</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Chomsky, N" uniqKey="Chomsky N">N. Chomsky</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Christensen, P" uniqKey="Christensen P">P. Christensen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tylen, K" uniqKey="Tylen K">K. Tylén</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Clark, A" uniqKey="Clark A">A. Clark</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Clark, E V" uniqKey="Clark E">E. V. Clark</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Coerts, J" uniqKey="Coerts J">J. Coerts</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Corrazza, S" uniqKey="Corrazza S">S. Corrazza</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Radutzky, E" uniqKey="Radutzky E">E. Radutzky</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Santarelli, B" uniqKey="Santarelli B">B. Santarelli</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Verdirosi, M L" uniqKey="Verdirosi M">M. L. Verdirosi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Volterra, V" uniqKey="Volterra V">V. Volterra</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zingarini, A" uniqKey="Zingarini A">A. Zingarini</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Craig, C G" uniqKey="Craig C">C. G. Craig</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Crasborn, O" uniqKey="Crasborn O">O. Crasborn</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Crasborn, O" uniqKey="Crasborn O">O. Crasborn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Der Kooija, E" uniqKey="Van Der Kooija E">E. van der Kooija</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Waters, D" uniqKey="Waters D">D. Waters</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Woll, B" uniqKey="Woll B">B. Woll</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mesch, J" uniqKey="Mesch J">J. Mesch</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Crasborn, O" uniqKey="Crasborn O">O. Crasborn</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zwitserlood, I" uniqKey="Zwitserlood I">I. Zwitserlood</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cowie, F" uniqKey="Cowie F">F. Cowie</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Crowley, T" uniqKey="Crowley T">T. Crowley</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Curtiss, S" uniqKey="Curtiss S">S. Curtiss</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Curtiss, S" uniqKey="Curtiss S">S. Curtiss</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Krashen, S D" uniqKey="Krashen S">S. D. Krashen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fromkin, V A" uniqKey="Fromkin V">V. A. Fromkin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rigler, D" uniqKey="Rigler D">D. Rigler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rigler, M" uniqKey="Rigler M">M. Rigler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cuxac, C" uniqKey="Cuxac C">C. Cuxac</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cuxac, C" uniqKey="Cuxac C">C. Cuxac</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sallandre, M A" uniqKey="Sallandre M">M-A. Sallandre</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Degraff, M" uniqKey="Degraff M">M. DeGraff</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Degraff, M" uniqKey="Degraff M">M. DeGraff</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Langhe, O" uniqKey="De Langhe O">O. De Langhe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Guitteny, P" uniqKey="Guitteny P">P. Guitteny</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Portine, H" uniqKey="Portine H">H. Portine</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Retore, C" uniqKey="Retore C">C. Retoré</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Delgado Martins, M R" uniqKey="Delgado Martins M">M. R. Delgado-Martins</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Coutinho, A" uniqKey="Coutinho A">A. Coutinho</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Amaral, M A" uniqKey="Amaral M">M. A. Amaral</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Delsing, L O" uniqKey="Delsing L">L.-O. Delsing</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Desimone, R" uniqKey="Desimone R">R. Desimone</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Duncan, J" uniqKey="Duncan J">J. Duncan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Swart, H" uniqKey="De Swart H">H. de Swart</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Deuchar, M" uniqKey="Deuchar M">M. Deuchar</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dryer, M S" uniqKey="Dryer M">M. S. Dryer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dryer, M S" uniqKey="Dryer M">M. S. Dryer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Efthimiou, E" uniqKey="Efthimiou E">E. Efthimiou</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fotinea, S E" uniqKey="Fotinea S">S-E. Fotinea</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Emmorey, K" uniqKey="Emmorey K">K. Emmorey</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Engberg Pedersen, E" uniqKey="Engberg Pedersen E">E. Engberg-Pedersen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Farris, M" uniqKey="Farris M">M. Farris</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fedorenko, E" uniqKey="Fedorenko E">E. Fedorenko</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Duncan, J" uniqKey="Duncan J">J. Duncan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kanwisher, N" uniqKey="Kanwisher N">N. Kanwisher</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fischer, S" uniqKey="Fischer S">S. Fischer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fischer, S D" uniqKey="Fischer S">S. D. Fischer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fischer, S D" uniqKey="Fischer S">S. D. Fischer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fischer, S D" uniqKey="Fischer S">S. D. Fischer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Janis, W" uniqKey="Janis W">W. Janis</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Flack Potts, K" uniqKey="Flack Potts K">K. Flack Potts</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Friedman, L" uniqKey="Friedman L">L. Friedman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fromkin, V" uniqKey="Fromkin V">V. Fromkin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Krashen, S" uniqKey="Krashen S">S. Krashen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Curtiss, S" uniqKey="Curtiss S">S. Curtiss</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rigler, D" uniqKey="Rigler D">D. Rigler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rigler, M" uniqKey="Rigler M">M. Rigler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gallese, V" uniqKey="Gallese V">V. Gallese</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gernsbacher, M A" uniqKey="Gernsbacher M">M. A. Gernsbacher</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Heyes, C" uniqKey="Heyes C">C. Heyes</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hickok, G" uniqKey="Hickok G">G. Hickok</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Iacoboni, M" uniqKey="Iacoboni M">M. Iacoboni</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gell Mann, M" uniqKey="Gell Mann M">M. Gell-Mann</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ruhlen, M" uniqKey="Ruhlen M">M. Ruhlen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gershkoff Stowe, L" uniqKey="Gershkoff Stowe L">L. Gershkoff-Stowe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Goldin Meadow, S" uniqKey="Goldin Meadow S">S. Goldin-Meadow</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gibson, E" uniqKey="Gibson E">E. Gibson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fedorenko, E" uniqKey="Fedorenko E">E. Fedorenko</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gibson, E" uniqKey="Gibson E">E. Gibson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Piantadosi, S T" uniqKey="Piantadosi S">S. T. Piantadosi</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Brink, K" uniqKey="Brink K">K. Brink</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bergen, L" uniqKey="Bergen L">L. Bergen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lim, E" uniqKey="Lim E">E. Lim</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Saxe, R" uniqKey="Saxe R">R. Saxe</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Giles, J" uniqKey="Giles J">J. Giles</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Giv N, T" uniqKey="Giv N T">T. Givón</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Giv N, T" uniqKey="Giv N T">T. Givón</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Giv N, T" uniqKey="Giv N T">T. Givón</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gluck, S" uniqKey="Gluck S">S. Glück</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pfau, R" uniqKey="Pfau R">R. Pfau</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Goldberg, A" uniqKey="Goldberg A">A. Goldberg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Goldberg, A" uniqKey="Goldberg A">A. Goldberg</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Goldin Meadow, S" uniqKey="Goldin Meadow S">S. Goldin-Meadow</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Goldin Meadow, S" uniqKey="Goldin Meadow S">S. Goldin-Meadow</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Goldin Meadow, S" uniqKey="Goldin Meadow S">S. Goldin-Meadow</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="So, W C" uniqKey="So W">W. C. So</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ozyurek, A" uniqKey="Ozyurek A">A. Özyürek</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mylander, C" uniqKey="Mylander C">C. Mylander</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hale, K" uniqKey="Hale K">K. Hale</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hall, M L" uniqKey="Hall M">M. L. Hall</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mayberry, R I" uniqKey="Mayberry R">R. I. Mayberry</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ferreira, V S" uniqKey="Ferreira V">V. S. Ferreira</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="H Nel, B" uniqKey="H Nel B">B. Hänel</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hanke, T" uniqKey="Hanke T">T. Hanke</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Konig, L" uniqKey="Konig L">L. König</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wagner, S" uniqKey="Wagner S">S. Wagner</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Matthes, S" uniqKey="Matthes S">S. Matthes</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Happ, D" uniqKey="Happ D">D. Happ</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Verkoper, M O" uniqKey="Verkoper M">M. O. Verköper</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Haviland, J B" uniqKey="Haviland J">J. B. Haviland</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Haywood, S" uniqKey="Haywood S">S. Haywood</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pickering, M" uniqKey="Pickering M">M. Pickering</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Branigan, H" uniqKey="Branigan H">H. Branigan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Herring, S C" uniqKey="Herring S">S. C. Herring</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Paolillo, J C" uniqKey="Paolillo J">J. C. Paolillo</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hoekstra, T" uniqKey="Hoekstra T">T. Hoekstra</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mulder, R" uniqKey="Mulder R">R. Mulder</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hohenberger, A" uniqKey="Hohenberger A">A. Hohenberger</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Hr Arsd Ttir, " uniqKey="Hr Arsd Ttir ">Þ. Hróarsdóttir</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jantunen, T" uniqKey="Jantunen T">T. Jantunen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jefwa, G J" uniqKey="Jefwa G">G. J. Jefwa</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnson, J S" uniqKey="Johnson J">J. S. Johnson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Newport, E" uniqKey="Newport E">E. Newport</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnston, T" uniqKey="Johnston T">T. Johnston</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnston, T" uniqKey="Johnston T">T. Johnston</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnston, T" uniqKey="Johnston T">T. Johnston</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnston, T" uniqKey="Johnston T">T. Johnston</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schembri, A" uniqKey="Schembri A">A. Schembri</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnston, T" uniqKey="Johnston T">T. Johnston</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schembri, A" uniqKey="Schembri A">A. Schembri</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnston, T" uniqKey="Johnston T">T. Johnston</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vermeerbergen, M" uniqKey="Vermeerbergen M">M. Vermeerbergen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schembri, A" uniqKey="Schembri A">A. Schembri</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leeson, L" uniqKey="Leeson L">L. Leeson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Julien, M" uniqKey="Julien M">M. Julien</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kegl, J" uniqKey="Kegl J">J. Kegl</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kemmerer, D" uniqKey="Kemmerer D">D. Kemmerer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Khanittanan, W" uniqKey="Khanittanan W">W. Khanittanan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kimmelman, V" uniqKey="Kimmelman V">V. Kimmelman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Klima, E" uniqKey="Klima E">E. Klima</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bellugi, U" uniqKey="Bellugi U">U. Bellugi</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Konrad, R" uniqKey="Konrad R">R. Konrad</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Koutsoudas, A" uniqKey="Koutsoudas A">A. Koutsoudas</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kristoffersen, J H" uniqKey="Kristoffersen J">J. H. Kristoffersen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kuhn, J" uniqKey="Kuhn J">J. Kuhn</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Langus, A" uniqKey="Langus A">A. Langus</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nespor, M" uniqKey="Nespor M">M. Nespor</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lee, H" uniqKey="Lee H">H. Lee</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leeson, L" uniqKey="Leeson L">L. Leeson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Saeed, J" uniqKey="Saeed J">J. Saeed</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leibowitz, H" uniqKey="Leibowitz H">H. Leibowitz</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Leuninger, H" uniqKey="Leuninger H">H. Leuninger</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Levy, R" uniqKey="Levy R">R. Levy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Levy, R" uniqKey="Levy R">R. Levy</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Jaeger, T F" uniqKey="Jaeger T">T. F. Jaeger</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Liddell, S" uniqKey="Liddell S">S. Liddell</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Liddell, S" uniqKey="Liddell S">S. Liddell</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Johnson, R" uniqKey="Johnson R">R. Johnson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lillo Martin, D" uniqKey="Lillo Martin D">D. Lillo-Martin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lillo Martin, D" uniqKey="Lillo Martin D">D. Lillo-Martin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lillo Martin, D" uniqKey="Lillo Martin D">D. Lillo-Martin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Berk, S" uniqKey="Berk S">S. Berk</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Massone, M I" uniqKey="Massone M">M. I. Massone</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Curiel, M" uniqKey="Curiel M">M. Curiel</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Matsuoka, K" uniqKey="Matsuoka K">K. Matsuoka</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcdonald, B" uniqKey="Mcdonald B">B. McDonald</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcenery, T" uniqKey="Mcenery T">T. McEnery</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wilson, A" uniqKey="Wilson A">A. Wilson</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcintire, M" uniqKey="Mcintire M">M. McIntire</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mckee, R" uniqKey="Mckee R">R. McKee</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kennedy, G" uniqKey="Kennedy G">G. Kennedy</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Mcwhorter, J H" uniqKey="Mcwhorter J">J. H. McWhorter</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meier, R" uniqKey="Meier R">R. Meier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Cormier, K" uniqKey="Cormier K">K. Cormier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Quinto Pozos, D" uniqKey="Quinto Pozos D">D. Quinto-Pozos</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meir, I" uniqKey="Meir I">I. Meir</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meir, I" uniqKey="Meir I">I. Meir</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meir, I" uniqKey="Meir I">I. Meir</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lifshitz, A" uniqKey="Lifshitz A">A. Lifshitz</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ilkbasaran, D" uniqKey="Ilkbasaran D">D. Ilkbasaran</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Padden, C" uniqKey="Padden C">C. Padden</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meir, I" uniqKey="Meir I">I. Meir</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Padden, C" uniqKey="Padden C">C. Padden</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aronoff, M" uniqKey="Aronoff M">M. Aronoff</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sandler, W" uniqKey="Sandler W">W. Sandler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meir, I" uniqKey="Meir I">I. Meir</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sandler, W" uniqKey="Sandler W">W. Sandler</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meir, I" uniqKey="Meir I">I. Meir</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sandler, W" uniqKey="Sandler W">W. Sandler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Padden, P" uniqKey="Padden P">P. Padden</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aronoff, M" uniqKey="Aronoff M">M. Aronoff</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meurant, L" uniqKey="Meurant L">L. Meurant</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sinte, A" uniqKey="Sinte A">A. Sinte</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Miljan, M" uniqKey="Miljan M">M. Miljan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Milkovic, M" uniqKey="Milkovic M">M. Milkovic</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bradaric Joncic, S" uniqKey="Bradaric Joncic S">S. Bradaric-Joncic</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wilbur, R" uniqKey="Wilbur R">R. Wilbur</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Milkovic, M" uniqKey="Milkovic M">M. Milkovic</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bradaric Joncic, S" uniqKey="Bradaric Joncic S">S. Bradaric-Joncic</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wilbur, R" uniqKey="Wilbur R">R. Wilbur</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Minoura, N" uniqKey="Minoura N">N. Minoura</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Morales L Pez, E" uniqKey="Morales L Pez E">E. Morales-López</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Boldu Menasanch, R M" uniqKey="Boldu Menasanch R">R. M. Boldú-Menasanch</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Alonso Rodriguez, J A" uniqKey="Alonso Rodriguez J">J. A. Alonso-Rodríguez</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gras Ferrer, V" uniqKey="Gras Ferrer V">V. Gras-Ferrer</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rodriguez Gonzalez, M A" uniqKey="Rodriguez Gonzalez M">M. A. Rodríguez-González</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Morales L Pez, E" uniqKey="Morales L Pez E">E. Morales-López</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Reigosa Varela, C" uniqKey="Reigosa Varela C">C. Reigosa-Varela</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bobillo Garcia, N" uniqKey="Bobillo Garcia N">N. Bobillo-García</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Moravcsik, E" uniqKey="Moravcsik E">E. Moravcsik</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nadeau, M" uniqKey="Nadeau M">M. Nadeau</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Desouvrey, L" uniqKey="Desouvrey L">L. Desouvrey</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nakanashi, K" uniqKey="Nakanashi K">K. Nakanashi</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Napoli, D J" uniqKey="Napoli D">D. J. Napoli</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sutton Spence, R" uniqKey="Sutton Spence R">R. Sutton-Spence</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Napoli, D J" uniqKey="Napoli D">D. J. Napoli</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sutton Spence, R" uniqKey="Sutton Spence R">R. Sutton-Spence</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Neidle, C" uniqKey="Neidle C">C. Neidle</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Kegl, J" uniqKey="Kegl J">J. Kegl</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Maclaughlin, D" uniqKey="Maclaughlin D">D. MacLaughlin</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bahan, B" uniqKey="Bahan B">B. Bahan</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lee, R" uniqKey="Lee R">R. Lee</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Newmeyer, F" uniqKey="Newmeyer F">F. Newmeyer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Newmeyer, F" uniqKey="Newmeyer F">F. Newmeyer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Newport, E" uniqKey="Newport E">E. Newport</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Newport, E" uniqKey="Newport E">E. Newport</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Newport, E" uniqKey="Newport E">E. Newport</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Bavelier, D" uniqKey="Bavelier D">D. Bavelier</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Neville, H J" uniqKey="Neville H">H. J. Neville</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nyst, V" uniqKey="Nyst V">V. Nyst</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ochs, E" uniqKey="Ochs E">E. Ochs</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Oviedo, A" uniqKey="Oviedo A">A. Oviedo</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Padden, C" uniqKey="Padden C">C. Padden</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Padden, C" uniqKey="Padden C">C. Padden</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Padden, C" uniqKey="Padden C">C. Padden</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meir, I" uniqKey="Meir I">I. Meir</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sandler, W" uniqKey="Sandler W">W. Sandler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aronoff, M" uniqKey="Aronoff M">M. Aronoff</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pakhomova, A S" uniqKey="Pakhomova A">A. S. Pakhomova</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Parodi, C" uniqKey="Parodi C">C. Parodi</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Petitto, L A" uniqKey="Petitto L">L. A. Petitto</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zatorre, R J" uniqKey="Zatorre R">R. J. Zatorre</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gauna, K" uniqKey="Gauna K">K. Gauna</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Nikelski, E J" uniqKey="Nikelski E">E. J. Nikelski</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Dostie, D" uniqKey="Dostie D">D. Dostie</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Evans, A C" uniqKey="Evans A">A. C. Evans</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Petronio, K" uniqKey="Petronio K">K. Petronio</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Plaza Pust, C" uniqKey="Plaza Pust C">C. Plaza-Pust</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Plaza Pust, C" uniqKey="Plaza Pust C">C. Plaza-Pust</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weinmeister, K" uniqKey="Weinmeister K">K. Weinmeister</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Quadros, R M" uniqKey="Quadros R">R. M. Quadros</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="De" uniqKey="De">de</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Quadros, R M" uniqKey="Quadros R">R. M. Quadros</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="De" uniqKey="De">de</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Lillo Martin, D" uniqKey="Lillo Martin D">D. Lillo-Martin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Quadros, R M" uniqKey="Quadros R">R. M. Quadros</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="De" uniqKey="De">de</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Quer, J" uniqKey="Quer J">J. Quer</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Quinto, D" uniqKey="Quinto D">D. Quinto</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Raney, R" uniqKey="Raney R">R. Raney</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Roberge, P T" uniqKey="Roberge P">P. T. Roberge</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rosenbaum, D A" uniqKey="Rosenbaum D">D. A. Rosenbaum</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rosenbaum, D A" uniqKey="Rosenbaum D">D. A. Rosenbaum</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Carlson, R A" uniqKey="Carlson R">R. A. Carlson</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gilmore, R O" uniqKey="Gilmore R">R. O. Gilmore</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Rosenstein, O" uniqKey="Rosenstein O">O. Rosenstein</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Ross, M" uniqKey="Ross M">M. Ross</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Russo, T" uniqKey="Russo T">T. Russo</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sallandre, M" uniqKey="Sallandre M">M. Sallandre</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sandler, W" uniqKey="Sandler W">W. Sandler</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Meir, I" uniqKey="Meir I">I. Meir</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Padden, C" uniqKey="Padden C">C. Padden</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Aronoff, M" uniqKey="Aronoff M">M. Aronoff</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schembri, A" uniqKey="Schembri A">A. Schembri</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schouwstra, M" uniqKey="Schouwstra M">M. Schouwstra</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Schuit, J" uniqKey="Schuit J">J. Schuit</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Baker, A" uniqKey="Baker A">A. Baker</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Pfau, R" uniqKey="Pfau R">R. Pfau</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Senghas, A" uniqKey="Senghas A">A. Senghas</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Coppola, M" uniqKey="Coppola M">M. Coppola</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Newport, E L" uniqKey="Newport E">E. L. Newport</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Supalla, T" uniqKey="Supalla T">T. Supalla</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Shannon, C" uniqKey="Shannon C">C. Shannon</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Slobin, D I" uniqKey="Slobin D">D. I. Slobin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Smith, W" uniqKey="Smith W">W. Smith</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="So, W C" uniqKey="So W">W. C. So</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Coppola, M" uniqKey="Coppola M">M. Coppola</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Licciardello, V" uniqKey="Licciardello V">V. Licciardello</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Goldin Meadow, S" uniqKey="Goldin Meadow S">S. Goldin-Meadow</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sprouse, J" uniqKey="Sprouse J">J. Sprouse</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sundquist, J D" uniqKey="Sundquist J">J. D. Sundquist</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sutton Spence, R" uniqKey="Sutton Spence R">R. Sutton-Spence</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Woll, B" uniqKey="Woll B">B. Woll</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Sze, Y B F" uniqKey="Sze Y">Y. B. F. Sze</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tallerman, M" uniqKey="Tallerman M">M. Tallerman</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Taub, S" uniqKey="Taub S">S. Taub</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Taylor, A" uniqKey="Taylor A">A. Taylor</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tettamanti, M" uniqKey="Tettamanti M">M. Tettamanti</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Buccino, G" uniqKey="Buccino G">G. Buccino</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Saccuman, M C" uniqKey="Saccuman M">M. C. Saccuman</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Gallese, V" uniqKey="Gallese V">V. Gallese</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Danna, M" uniqKey="Danna M">M. Danna</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Scifo, P" uniqKey="Scifo P">P. Scifo</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Timm, L A" uniqKey="Timm L">L. A. Timm</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tomlin, R" uniqKey="Tomlin R">R. Tomlin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tsay, J" uniqKey="Tsay J">J. Tsay</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Myers, J" uniqKey="Myers J">J. Myers</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Tomasello, M" uniqKey="Tomasello M">M. Tomasello</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Gelderen, E" uniqKey="Van Gelderen E">E. Van Gelderen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Varin, A" uniqKey="Varin A">A. Varin</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vermeerbergen, M" uniqKey="Vermeerbergen M">M. Vermeerbergen</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vermeerbergen, M" uniqKey="Vermeerbergen M">M. Vermeerbergen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Herreweghe, M" uniqKey="Van Herreweghe M">M. Van Herreweghe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Akach, P" uniqKey="Akach P">P. Akach</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vermeerbergen, M" uniqKey="Vermeerbergen M">M. Vermeerbergen</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Van Herreweghe, M" uniqKey="Van Herreweghe M">M. Van Herreweghe</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Akach, P" uniqKey="Akach P">P. Akach</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Matabane, E" uniqKey="Matabane E">E. Matabane</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Volterra, V" uniqKey="Volterra V">V. Volterra</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Laudanna, A" uniqKey="Laudanna A">A. Laudanna</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Corazza, S" uniqKey="Corazza S">S. Corazza</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Radutzky, E" uniqKey="Radutzky E">E. Radutzky</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Natale, F" uniqKey="Natale F">F. Natale</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Vulanovi, R" uniqKey="Vulanovi R">R. Vulanoviæ</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Washabaugh, W" uniqKey="Washabaugh W">W. Washabaugh</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Woodward, J" uniqKey="Woodward J">J. Woodward</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="De Santis, S" uniqKey="De Santis S">S. De Santis</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Weskott, T" uniqKey="Weskott T">T. Weskott</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Fanselow, G" uniqKey="Fanselow G">G. Fanselow</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wilbur, R" uniqKey="Wilbur R">R. Wilbur</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wilbur, R" uniqKey="Wilbur R">R. Wilbur</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wilbur, R" uniqKey="Wilbur R">R. Wilbur</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Willis, D W E" uniqKey="Willis D">D. W. E. Willis</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wojda, P" uniqKey="Wojda P">P. Wojda</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Woll, B" uniqKey="Woll B">B. Woll</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wood, S K" uniqKey="Wood S">S. K. Wood</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Wood, S K" uniqKey="Wood S">S. K. Wood</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Woodward, J" uniqKey="Woodward J">J. Woodward</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Woodward, J" uniqKey="Woodward J">J. Woodward</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Yau, S C" uniqKey="Yau S">S. C. Yau</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zeshan, U" uniqKey="Zeshan U">U. Zeshan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zeshan, U" uniqKey="Zeshan U">U. Zeshan</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zhang, N N" uniqKey="Zhang N">N. N. Zhang</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
<biblStruct>
<analytic>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zhang, X" uniqKey="Zhang X">X. Zhang</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Han, J" uniqKey="Han J">J. Han</name>
</author>
<author>
<name sortKey="Zhao, G" uniqKey="Zhao G">G. Zhao</name>
</author>
</analytic>
</biblStruct>
</listBibl>
</div1>
</back>
</TEI>
<pmc article-type="research-article">
<pmc-dir>properties open_access</pmc-dir>
<front>
<journal-meta>
<journal-id journal-id-type="nlm-ta">Front Psychol</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="iso-abbrev">Front Psychol</journal-id>
<journal-id journal-id-type="publisher-id">Front. Psychol.</journal-id>
<journal-title-group>
<journal-title>Frontiers in Psychology</journal-title>
</journal-title-group>
<issn pub-type="epub">1664-1078</issn>
<publisher>
<publisher-name>Frontiers Media S.A.</publisher-name>
</publisher>
</journal-meta>
<article-meta>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmid">24860523</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="pmc">4026690</article-id>
<article-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00376</article-id>
<article-categories>
<subj-group subj-group-type="heading">
<subject>Psychology</subject>
<subj-group>
<subject>Original Research Article</subject>
</subj-group>
</subj-group>
</article-categories>
<title-group>
<article-title>Order of the major constituents in sign languages: implications for all language</article-title>
</title-group>
<contrib-group>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Napoli</surname>
<given-names>Donna Jo</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff1">
<sup>1</sup>
</xref>
<xref ref-type="author-notes" rid="fn001">
<sup>*</sup>
</xref>
<uri xlink:type="simple" xlink:href="http://community.frontiersin.org/people/u/106441"></uri>
</contrib>
<contrib contrib-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sutton-Spence</surname>
<given-names>Rachel</given-names>
</name>
<xref ref-type="aff" rid="aff2">
<sup>2</sup>
</xref>
<uri xlink:type="simple" xlink:href="http://community.frontiersin.org/people/u/156339"></uri>
</contrib>
</contrib-group>
<aff id="aff1">
<sup>1</sup>
<institution>Department of Linguistics, Swarthmore College</institution>
<country>Swarthmore, PA, USA</country>
</aff>
<aff id="aff2">
<sup>2</sup>
<institution>Post-Graduate Department of Translation, Federal University of Santa Catarina</institution>
<country>Florianopolis, Brazil</country>
</aff>
<author-notes>
<fn fn-type="edited-by">
<p>Edited by: Iris Berent, Northeastern University, USA; Susan Goldin-Meadow, University of Chicago, USA</p>
</fn>
<fn fn-type="edited-by">
<p>Reviewed by: Cristiano Chesi, IUSS Pavia, Italy; Edward Gibson, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, USA; Carol Padden, University of California, San Diego, USA</p>
</fn>
<corresp id="fn001">*Correspondence: Donna Jo Napoli, Department of Linguistics, Swarthmore College, 500 College Ave., Swarthmore, PA 19081, USA e-mail:
<email xlink:type="simple">donnajonapoli@gmail.com</email>
</corresp>
<fn fn-type="other" id="fn002">
<p>This article was submitted to Language Sciences, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology.</p>
</fn>
</author-notes>
<pub-date pub-type="epub">
<day>12</day>
<month>5</month>
<year>2014</year>
</pub-date>
<pub-date pub-type="collection">
<year>2014</year>
</pub-date>
<volume>5</volume>
<elocation-id>376</elocation-id>
<history>
<date date-type="received">
<day>28</day>
<month>10</month>
<year>2013</year>
</date>
<date date-type="accepted">
<day>09</day>
<month>4</month>
<year>2014</year>
</date>
</history>
<permissions>
<copyright-statement>Copyright © 2014 Napoli and Sutton-Spence.</copyright-statement>
<copyright-year>2014</copyright-year>
<license license-type="open-access" xlink:href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/">
<license-p>This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.</license-p>
</license>
</permissions>
<abstract>
<p>A survey of reports of sign order from 42 sign languages leads to a handful of generalizations. Two accounts emerge, one amodal and the other modal. We argue that universal pressures are at work with respect to some generalizations, but that pressure from the visual modality is at work with respect to others. Together, these pressures conspire to make all sign languages order their major constituents SOV or SVO. This study leads us to the conclusion that the order of S with regard to verb phrase (VP) may be driven by sensorimotor system concerns that feed universal grammar.</p>
</abstract>
<kwd-group>
<kwd>sign languages</kwd>
<kwd>word order</kwd>
<kwd>vision</kwd>
<kwd>syntax</kwd>
<kwd>sensorimotor systems and language</kwd>
<kwd>gesture</kwd>
</kwd-group>
<counts>
<fig-count count="0"></fig-count>
<table-count count="1"></table-count>
<equation-count count="0"></equation-count>
<ref-count count="229"></ref-count>
<page-count count="18"></page-count>
<word-count count="18253"></word-count>
</counts>
</article-meta>
</front>
<body>
<sec sec-type="intro" id="s1">
<title>Introduction</title>
<p>In the initial period of linguistic analysis of sign languages, scholars tended to stay away from examining phenomena that were modality bound in favor of those that were more universal, in order to establish that sign languages were bona fide languages (see Woll,
<xref rid="B220" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
for an overview). Since the mid-1980s, however, scholars have turned their attention to the importance of modality (Bergman and Wallin,
<xref rid="B11" ref-type="bibr">1985</xref>
; Sze,
<xref rid="B197" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
; also see Meier et al.,
<xref rid="B136" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
).</p>
<p>We focus attention on the issue of sentence-level sign order in sign languages, looking at subject, object and verb. Research on 42 sign languages (see Table
<xref ref-type="table" rid="T1">1</xref>
), taken as a whole, coupled with our own observations leads to generalizations about order that contrast to varying degrees with word order in spoken languages. We consider two hypotheses: (1) that our generalizations are due to universal pressures on language, ones which are seen most strongly in young languages, and (2) that our generalizations are due to modality; that is, the patterns for sign order in sign languages are determined by what makes sense visually. We conclude that the first hypothesis carries us quite far, but consideration of visual pressures allows us to account for all the observed tendencies in our study. We conclude that all sign languages should order their constituents SOV and SVO in most declaratives. Importantly, this does not preclude the possibility that languages may impose language-specific constraints on order within a phrase (see work on noun phrases in Estonian Sign Language, Miljan,
<xref rid="B144" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
, and Taiwanese Sign Language, Zhang,
<xref rid="B228" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
).</p>
<table-wrap id="T1" position="float">
<label>Table 1</label>
<caption>
<p>
<bold>Sign Languages</bold>
.</p>
</caption>
<table frame="hsides" rules="groups">
<tbody>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Adamorobe</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Nyst,
<xref rid="B161" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Al-Sayyid Bedouin</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Sandler et al.,
<xref rid="B185" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
; Padden et al.,
<xref rid="B166" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">American</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Fischer,
<xref rid="B66" ref-type="bibr">1975</xref>
,
<xref rid="B67" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
; Friedman,
<xref rid="B70" ref-type="bibr">1976</xref>
; Klima and Bellugi,
<xref rid="B112" ref-type="bibr">1979</xref>
; Baker and Cokely,
<xref rid="B8" ref-type="bibr">1980</xref>
; McIntire,
<xref rid="B133" ref-type="bibr">1980</xref>
; Woodward,
<xref rid="B223" ref-type="bibr">1980</xref>
; Liddell and Johnson,
<xref rid="B125" ref-type="bibr">1986</xref>
; Lillo-Martin,
<xref rid="B126" ref-type="bibr">1986</xref>
,
<xref rid="B127" ref-type="bibr">1991</xref>
; Wilbur,
<xref rid="B215" ref-type="bibr">1987</xref>
,
<xref rid="B216" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
,
<xref rid="B217" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
; Padden,
<xref rid="B164" ref-type="bibr">1988</xref>
,
<xref rid="B165" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
; Fischer and Janis,
<xref rid="B68" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
; Kegl,
<xref rid="B108" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
; Liddell,
<xref rid="B124" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
; Petronio,
<xref rid="B170" ref-type="bibr">1993</xref>
; Bahan,
<xref rid="B7" ref-type="bibr">1996</xref>
; Matsuoka,
<xref rid="B130" ref-type="bibr">1997</xref>
; Neidle et al.,
<xref rid="B155" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
; Taub,
<xref rid="B199" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
; Aronoff et al.,
<xref rid="B6" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
; Chen Pichler et al.,
<xref rid="B32" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Argentine</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Massone and Curiel,
<xref rid="B129" ref-type="bibr">2004</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Australian</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Johnston and Schembri,
<xref rid="B104" ref-type="bibr">2007a</xref>
,
<xref rid="B105" ref-type="bibr">b</xref>
; Johnston et al.,
<xref rid="B106" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Austrian</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Wilbur,
<xref rid="B216" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
; Chen Pichler et al.,
<xref rid="B32" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Brazilian</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Quadros,
<xref rid="B173" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
; Quadros and Quer,
<xref rid="B175" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">British</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Deuchar,
<xref rid="B57" ref-type="bibr">1983</xref>
; Sutton-Spence and Woll,
<xref rid="B196" ref-type="bibr">1999</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Catalan</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Morales-López et al.,
<xref rid="B148" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
; Quadros and Quer,
<xref rid="B175" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Chinese</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Yau,
<xref rid="B225" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Colombian</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Oviedo,
<xref rid="B163" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Croatian</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Milkovic et al.,
<xref rid="B145" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
,
<xref rid="B146" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
; Chen Pichler et al.,
<xref rid="B32" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Danish</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Engberg-Pedersen,
<xref rid="B62" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
; Kristoffersen,
<xref rid="B115" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Estonian</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Miljan,
<xref rid="B144" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Finnish</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Jantunen,
<xref rid="B98" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Flemish</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Vermeerbergen,
<xref rid="B208" ref-type="bibr">1996</xref>
; Johnston et al.,
<xref rid="B106" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
; Vermeerbergen et al.,
<xref rid="B209" ref-type="bibr">2007a</xref>
,
<xref rid="B210" ref-type="bibr">b</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">French</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Baron,
<xref rid="B10" ref-type="bibr">1998</xref>
; Cuxac,
<xref rid="B48" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
; De Langhe et al.,
<xref rid="B52" ref-type="bibr">2004</xref>
; Cuxac and Sallandre,
<xref rid="B49" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">French Swiss</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Boyes Braem et al.,
<xref rid="B20" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">German</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Glück and Pfau,
<xref rid="B81" ref-type="bibr">1998</xref>
; Leuninger,
<xref rid="B121" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
; Hänel,
<xref rid="B89" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
; Happ and Verköper,
<xref rid="B91" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
; Plaza-Pust,
<xref rid="B171" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
; Plaza-Pust and Weinmeister,
<xref rid="B172" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Greek</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Efthimiou and Fotinea,
<xref rid="B60" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Hong Kong</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Sze,
<xref rid="B197" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Indian</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Zeshan,
<xref rid="B226" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
; Aboh et al.,
<xref rid="B1" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Inuit</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Schuit et al.,
<xref rid="B188" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Irish</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Johnston et al.,
<xref rid="B106" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Israeli</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Meir,
<xref rid="B137" ref-type="bibr">1995</xref>
; Aronoff et al.,
<xref rid="B6" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
; Rosenstein,
<xref rid="B181" ref-type="bibr">2004</xref>
; Meir and Sandler,
<xref rid="B141" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
; Meir et al.,
<xref rid="B140" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
,
<xref rid="B139" ref-type="bibr">2010a</xref>
,
<xref rid="B142" ref-type="bibr">b</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Italian</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Volterra et al.,
<xref rid="B211" ref-type="bibr">1984</xref>
; Corrazza et al.,
<xref rid="B39" ref-type="bibr">1985</xref>
; Boyes Braem et al.,
<xref rid="B20" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Japanese</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Nakanashi,
<xref rid="B152" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Kenyan</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Akach,
<xref rid="B2" ref-type="bibr">1992</xref>
; Jefwa,
<xref rid="B99" ref-type="bibr">2009</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Malagasy</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Minoura,
<xref rid="B147" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Mexican</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Quinto,
<xref rid="B176" ref-type="bibr">1999</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">New Zealand</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">McKee and Kennedy,
<xref rid="B134" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Polish</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Farris,
<xref rid="B63" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
; Wojda,
<xref rid="B219" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Portuguese</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Delgado-Martins et al.,
<xref rid="B53" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Providence Island Language</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Washabaugh et al.,
<xref rid="B213" ref-type="bibr">1978</xref>
; Woodward,
<xref rid="B224" ref-type="bibr">1987</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Quebec</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Nadeau and Desouvrey,
<xref rid="B151" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Russian</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Kimmelman,
<xref rid="B111" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">The Netherlands</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Bogaerde and Mills,
<xref rid="B16" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
; Coerts,
<xref rid="B38" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">South African</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Vermeerbergen et al.,
<xref rid="B209" ref-type="bibr">2007a</xref>
,
<xref rid="B210" ref-type="bibr">b</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Spanish</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Bobillo-García et al.,
<xref rid="B15" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
; Morales-López et al.,
<xref rid="B149" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Swedish</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Bergman and Wallin,
<xref rid="B11" ref-type="bibr">1985</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Taiwanese</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Smith,
<xref rid="B192" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
; Zhang,
<xref rid="B228" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
; Tsay and Myers,
<xref rid="B204" ref-type="bibr">2009</xref>
</td>
</tr>
<tr>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Turkish</td>
<td align="left" rowspan="1" colspan="1">Zeshan,
<xref rid="B227" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
</td>
</tr>
</tbody>
</table>
</table-wrap>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Terminology regarding predicates and nominals</title>
<p>We use V throughout to indicate predicates of any category. We use S and O to refer to the arguments of a V, but these labels are problematic, since what is referred to as S in the literature is typically agent and what is referred to as O is typically any other argument. We do not include discussion of non-argument nominals.</p>
<p>As for nominals, to understand the generalizations here we must pay attention to articulation. Referents can be manually articulated via a lexical NP (including fingerspelling) or via finger pointing to an object within sight. These are two typical ways of introducing referents (what we here call players) into the discourse (what we here call the conversational scene).</p>
<p>Once a player is on the scene, it is commonly assigned a spatial index and subsequently this index is pointed to Johnston (
<xref rid="B103" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
). Many behaviors fall under the rubric “pointing”: referential spatial indexes can be pointed to by finger, gaze, lip, chin, head-tilt, among others. Further, already introduced arguments can be incorporated into a V (Wilbur,
<xref rid="B217" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
), or indicated by body shift (Bahan,
<xref rid="B7" ref-type="bibr">1996</xref>
) and/or embodiment by the signer (Meir et al.,
<xref rid="B140" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
). For justification of including all these mechanisms as ways to indicate arguments, see Neidle et al. (
<xref rid="B155" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
). Still, null arguments are possible (Lillo-Martin,
<xref rid="B126" ref-type="bibr">1986</xref>
). Where a sentence appears to have an “omitted” argument (i.e., no articulatory realization, manual or non-manual), we take such an argument to be expressed earlier in the discourse or to be understood through context, otherwise the sentence would be incomprehensible (Bergman and Wallin,
<xref rid="B11" ref-type="bibr">1985</xref>
, p. 220). Argument omission is typical with a series of verbs that have the same argument (often S), where that argument has already been established (McIntire,
<xref rid="B133" ref-type="bibr">1980</xref>
; Padden,
<xref rid="B164" ref-type="bibr">1988</xref>
). Note that “I” and “you” are always on the scene, since they are participants in the sign/speech act.</p>
<p>Since ways of referring to old-information referents are, with one exception, layered (i.e., built into the V or indicated by the non-manuals), one cannot talk about their order with respect to the V: they are expressed simultaneously. We understand these referents in the context of the discourse and of our knowledge of who the signer/speaker is, and what the signer/speaker might be trying to communicate; this is a general practice in language comprehension (Carston,
<xref rid="B29" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
, among others).</p>
<p>The only exception is manual (i.e., finger or hand) pointing; this is generally not simultaneously articulated with the V. Many of our sources do not indicate manual pointing, but we use any information they do present. We categorize lexical NPs and NPs indicated by manual pointing together under the rubric “manually-expressed NPs,” and we use the abbreviation MNP.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>A word on data</title>
<p>We surveyed articles on 42 sign languages, as shown in Table
<xref ref-type="table" rid="T1">1</xref>
, where language names are given in English (cited studies tell which varieties of language are gathered under these rubrics). We draw upon data collected and analyzed in these works as well as cite insights of others, without necessarily adopting the authors' analyses.</p>
<p>While some conclusions in these works seem resilient within the study of a given language and sometimes across languages, many are fragile in that they do not find corroboration in other studies. Brennan (
<xref rid="B25" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
) points out that American Sign Language, for example, has been analyzed as SVO (Fischer,
<xref rid="B66" ref-type="bibr">1975</xref>
), V-final (Friedman,
<xref rid="B70" ref-type="bibr">1976</xref>
), and topic-comment (Baker and Cokely,
<xref rid="B8" ref-type="bibr">1980</xref>
). We add that ASL has been analyzed as varying between SVO and SOV depending on sociolinguistic factors (Woodward,
<xref rid="B223" ref-type="bibr">1980</xref>
). Further, sometimes no constraints on word order emerge; in Malagasy Sign Language all possible permutations of S, O, and V occur (Minoura,
<xref rid="B147" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
). Bouchard and Dubuisson (
<xref rid="B19" ref-type="bibr">1995</xref>
) and Bouchard (
<xref rid="B18" ref-type="bibr">1996</xref>
) argue that there is no base order in sign languages (and they say spoken language has this option, as well), looking at ASL and Quebec Sign Language.</p>
<p>Unfortunately, much of the confusion in the literature results from how the various studies were carried out. While replication of results is a revered principle in science, many times the best we can hope for is corroboration (Giles,
<xref rid="B77" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
). But often not even corroboration is found on sign order. Johnston et al. (
<xref rid="B106" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
; see also Coerts,
<xref rid="B38" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
) point out that attempts at comparing studies are confounded by the range of methodologies adopted in data collection, varying from elicitation based on drawings, to translations of sentences in a written language, to seeking grammaticality judgments of constructed sentences, to examining spontaneous or naturalistic data (monologs or dialogs).</p>
<p>Reliance on these methods, rather than on a large corpus of naturally occurring data gathered with no aim other than general linguistic study, is problematic (McEnery and Wilson,
<xref rid="B132" ref-type="bibr">1996</xref>
). Such methods' reliability is even more doubtful for sign language study, where often the number of native signers consulted is small (Johnston and Schembri,
<xref rid="B104" ref-type="bibr">2007a</xref>
). The sociolinguistics of Deaf communities complicates the issue further. Sign language communities are small minority communities whose language is young and without well-developed community-based standards of correctness and which have few true native signers (Johnston,
<xref rid="B103" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
). Concerns about basing analyses of any language on very limited data and about what we can conclude from different methods of data collection abound (Sprouse,
<xref rid="B194" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
; Weskott and Fanselow,
<xref rid="B214" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
; Gibson and Fedorenko,
<xref rid="B75" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
) and lead to the conclusion that methodological options in accumulating evidence for syntactic analysis should be expanded.</p>
<p>With regard to sign order studies, Johnston et al. (
<xref rid="B106" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
) point out further that often information about the linguistic consultants that might be pertinent to language variation is not given, and that issues as fundamental as having consistent criteria (or even any explicit criteria) for what counts as a clause or a complete sentence remain unresolved (and see Crasborn,
<xref rid="B41" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
; Jantunen,
<xref rid="B98" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
). Here we take the relevant unit for discussion to be predicates and their constellations of arguments, regardless of repetition of various parts (as in V sandwiching/doubling, see Fischer and Janis,
<xref rid="B68" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
; Kegl,
<xref rid="B108" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
; Matsuoka,
<xref rid="B130" ref-type="bibr">1997</xref>
). We take a light V and the main V it supports to be one predicate, an unproblematic analysis since no arguments intervene between the two in the data observed (as in signing GIVE plus HUG, rather than simply HUG—a rare construction, reported for Flemish Sign Language, but which might reveal spoken language influence, see Johnston et al.,
<xref rid="B106" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
).</p>
<p>The variety of theoretical approaches used, from syntactically based ones to semantically-pragmatically based ones, is another complicating factor (Johnston et al.,
<xref rid="B106" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
). Theoretical biases impose themselves in fundamental ways. Simply transcribing sign languages with a morpheme-by-morpheme gloss and then a translation into a spoken language can obscure the information (lexical and functional) in a sign and how it is packaged (Slobin,
<xref rid="B191" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
); there is no way to represent linguistic data that is theory-neutral (Ochs,
<xref rid="B162" ref-type="bibr">1979</xref>
). Thus, in any given study we may not know exactly what data are under consideration and, hence, exactly what we can conclude. Further, many of the findings in the various studies consist of generalizations often in the form of tables that give numbers of occurrences of templates such as OV, SOV, SVO, etc., but few actual examples, so that various comparisons we wanted to make were precluded. Given this lack of information, we have no choice but to transcribe sign streams in the way our sources do, rather than in a consistent transcription system that might be better suited for sign languages (such as the Berkeley Transcription System in Slobin,
<xref rid="B191" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
). While inconsistent coding inhibits comparison, one advantage of using the form presented in our sources is that sometimes this form is given in the ambient spoken language, and thus may relate articulatory information, since mouthing is common (Crasborn et al.,
<xref rid="B42" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
).</p>
<p>Sign languages can allow variety in order for the same range of reasons spoken languages do, including stylistic and grammatical concerns. So the murky issue of a so-called unmarked word order arises (Leeson and Saeed,
<xref rid="B119" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
). We have chosen to be inclusive for fear of excluding relevant data. Still, we restrict ourselves to declaratives (as do most works in our survey and as do studies of spoken languages). A handful of our sources focus on interrogatives, so that few examples from them are of use to us. Importantly, even when a study is on some issue other than sign order, the data presented support our claims here (as, for example, with Inuit Sign Language, in Schuit et al. (
<xref rid="B188" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
), where they explicitly set aside order as an issue they will not address).</p>
<p>Further, we are leery of relying on data not taken from spontaneous conversation, given confounding influences of the laboratory situation itself. This concern is of particular weight for sign languages since Deaf linguistic consultants can be influenced by perceived researchers' expectations based on grammatical properties of the ambient spoken language (Deuchar,
<xref rid="B57" ref-type="bibr">1983</xref>
, p. 76; Coerts,
<xref rid="B38" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
). Nevertheless, we use data from all 42 languages regardless of how it was collected.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Generalizations in the data</title>
<p>Here we list the generalizations we have found in the literature, augmented by our own observations of ASL and BSL conversations. These generalizations concern only MNPs, since all other nominals are expressed simultaneously to the V, precluding statements of linear ordering with respect to the V. So when we say S precedes V, we mean an S that is an MNP precedes the V, and so on. With the exception of the first, these generalizations are tendencies. The section A Comparison to Two Accounts discusses two accounts of these generalizations along with data that run counter to them.</p>
<sec>
<title>Generalization one</title>
<p>SOV is grammatical in all sign languages.</p>
<p>Yau (
<xref rid="B225" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
) makes this claim and our survey confirms it. We offer a typical example from Finnish Sign Language (Jantunen,
<xref rid="B98" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
, p. 99):</p>
<disp-quote>
<p>BOY APPLE BUY ‘(The) boy buys an apple.’</p>
</disp-quote>
<p>If there are three MNPs in the sentence (which is uncommon in conversation) and all are arguments, then all can precede the V, as in this example from Israeli Sign Language (Meir et al.,
<xref rid="B142" ref-type="bibr">2010b</xref>
, p. 276):</p>
<disp-quote>
<p>WOMAN BOX TABLE PUT-ON ‘The woman puts the box on the table.’</p>
</disp-quote>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Generalization two</title>
<p>If an argument affects the phonological shape of the V, it precedes V.</p>
<p>This includes classifier predicates (Emmorey,
<xref rid="B61" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
), agreeing verbs (Wilbur,
<xref rid="B215" ref-type="bibr">1987</xref>
; Liddell,
<xref rid="B124" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
), pointing verbs (De Langhe et al.,
<xref rid="B52" ref-type="bibr">2004</xref>
), spatial verbs (Padden,
<xref rid="B164" ref-type="bibr">1988</xref>
; and see Liddell,
<xref rid="B124" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
), and argument-sensitive verbs (Klima and Bellugi,
<xref rid="B112" ref-type="bibr">1979</xref>
; Volterra et al.,
<xref rid="B211" ref-type="bibr">1984</xref>
, p. 33; a.k.a. “imitating” predicates, in Vermeerbergen et al.,
<xref rid="B210" ref-type="bibr">2007b</xref>
). (All types of V in this paper are discussed in Padden,
<xref rid="B164" ref-type="bibr">1988</xref>
,
<xref rid="B165" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
; Quadros and Quer,
<xref rid="B175" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
; Padden et al.,
<xref rid="B166" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
.) Only plain Vs are exempt. Evidence comes from explicit statements by scholars in the surveyed articles and our own observations.</p>
<p>Many studies exhibit only SOV sentences and explicitly claim that V must come finally. Others exhibit only SOV sentences but claim that the order is topic-comment (as in McKee and Kennedy,
<xref rid="B134" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
, on New Zealand Sign Language). Others explicitly claim that if the V is a classifier, it must come finally, while still others say a classifier predicate usually comes finally.</p>
<p>Many studies note sentences with the structure SVOV, the template of V sandwiches, where the two Vs indicate the same action. Whether we have two clauses here or only one is a tricky matter, but not one we need to resolve. What matters for us is that the first V is typically a simple form, whereas the second shows variable phonological shape, sometimes with aspectual marking on it, but often with more iconic information than the basic form, some of which may be affected by the arguments. (Many have noted for ASL that if a V is aspectually marked, its O precedes it even in single-V clauses, where the explanation involves raising the marked V to a right-branching functional projection, leaving the O in pre-verbal position, as in Chen Pichler,
<xref rid="B31" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
; Fischer and Janis,
<xref rid="B68" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
; Matsuoka,
<xref rid="B130" ref-type="bibr">1997</xref>
; Braze,
<xref rid="B23" ref-type="bibr">2004</xref>
.)</p>
<p>Here we see a V sandwich from Russian Sign Language where the second instance of the V is accompanied by a non-manual adverbial morpheme (Kimmelman,
<xref rid="B111" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
, example 47):</p>
<disp-quote>
<p>                           face: doubtfully</p>
<p>LOOK G-R-U-Š-A          LOOK        ‘[He] looked at the pear</p>
<p>                                                        doubtfully’</p>
</disp-quote>
<p>Several studies explicitly mention that agreeing Vs come in final position. In Brazilian Sign Language, SVO is argued to be the unmarked order (Quadros,
<xref rid="B173" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
) but agreeing verbs can also come in final position, with SOV order (see also Quadros and Lillo-Martin,
<xref rid="B174" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
). If pointing verbs are discussed at all, they are typically mixed into the discussion of agreeing verbs.</p>
<p>We turn now to argument-sensitive verbs. The studies we consulted that offer evidence about argument-sensitive verbs (whether they note it or not) show that MNPs precede argument-sensitive Vs. For example, Johnston et al. (
<xref rid="B106" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
) discuss sentences containing HUG in Irish Sign Language, Flemish Sign Language, and Auslan. Sometimes the first appearance of an argument of HUG is an MNP which follows the V, as in this example from Auslan (Johnston et al.,
<xref rid="B106" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
, p. 192):</p>
<disp-quote>
<p>BOY MEET HUG
<sub>
<italic>p</italic>
</sub>
GRANDMOTHER</p>
</disp-quote>
<p>We analyze the above as two clauses (as do the study authors), but significantly the first appearance of the O of HUG follows it (that is, GRANDMOTHER). And here the articulatory shape of HUG has not been adjusted to match the arguments. We indicate this fact with the subscript “p,” showing this is a plain V. (Argument-sensitive Vs, unlike most agreeing Vs, only optionally incorporate their arguments.) However, a V sandwich example from Irish Sign Language has two instances of HUG, the first without phonological adjustment for the arguments (HUG
<sub>
<italic>p</italic>
</sub>
) and the second with such adjustment (HUG
<sub>s</sub>
, where the subscript “s” indicates this is an argument-sensitive realization of the V). We find that the MNPs representing the relevant arguments (the hugger and the hugged) precede the second instance of HUG (HUG
<sub>s</sub>
) and, further, that the S precedes the O in this Irish Sign Language sentence (Johnston et al.,
<xref rid="B106" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
, p. 192):</p>
<disp-quote>
<p>BOY HUG
<sub>
<italic>p</italic>
</sub>
WITH OLD-GRANDMOTHER HUG
<sub>
<italic>s</italic>
</sub>
</p>
</disp-quote>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Generalization three</title>
<p>The most common sentence type has only one new argument, which precedes the V.</p>
<p>We offer a typical example from Indian Sign Language (Aboh et al.,
<xref rid="B1" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
, p. 22) in the completive aspect (COMPL):</p>
<disp-quote>
<p>YESTERDAY FATHER DIE COMPL ‘Yesterday (my) father died.’</p>
</disp-quote>
<p>In fact, V S is generally unfound except when the V's sense introduces a player (which can be an event) onto the scene. Evidence for this generalization comes from explicit statements by scholars and our own observations. Additionally, we present evidence from so called split-sentence constructions.</p>
<sec>
<title>Claims in the literature and our observations</title>
<p>First, sign languages usually express at most one MNP in a sentence, a fact some authors explicitly note. Many studies exhibit no V-initial sentences, again an observation often explicitly noted (and predicted in Minoura,
<xref rid="B147" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
, p. 49, an idea proposed to her in personal correspondence by Susan Fischer). Other studies do have V-initial sentences, but the Vs function precisely to present or introduce a new argument, such as the existential verbs “seem,” “exist,” and the presentational verb “happen,” as in this example from Kenyan Sign Language (Jefwa,
<xref rid="B99" ref-type="bibr">2009</xref>
, p. 167):</p>
<disp-quote>
<p>HAPPEN ONE MZUNGU COME KENYA ‘It happened one European came to Kenya.’</p>
</disp-quote>
<p>or possessives (some of which are presentational, see Kristoffersen,
<xref rid="B115" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
; Johnston et al.,
<xref rid="B106" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
), as in this example from Swedish Sign Language (Bergman and Wallin,
<xref rid="B11" ref-type="bibr">1985</xref>
, p. 219):</p>
<disp-quote>
<p>HAVE CAR I ‘I have a car.’</p>
</disp-quote>
<p>Still, in Malagasy Sign Language several V-initial assertions with other types of verbs are reported, an example being (Minoura,
<xref rid="B147" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
, p. 52 and ff.):</p>
<disp-quote>
<p>MANDRARAKA KAMIÖ VATO ‘scatter truck rock’ ‘The truck scatters rocks.’</p>
</disp-quote>
<p>Minoura suggests the order in such examples is an influence from written Malagasy. (For remarks on the influence of written language order on sign order, see Fischer,
<xref rid="B66" ref-type="bibr">1975</xref>
; Bogaerde and Mills,
<xref rid="B16" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
; De Langhe et al.,
<xref rid="B52" ref-type="bibr">2004</xref>
; Milkovic et al.,
<xref rid="B146" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
; Yau,
<xref rid="B225" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
; Wojda,
<xref rid="B219" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
, who argues that this factor makes it impossible to determine the unmarked word order of Polish Sign Language.)</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Split-sentence constructions</title>
<p>When one conveys a proposition in which the predicate has two arguments, and the referents of both are new to the conversation, a common tendency is to employ two clauses. The first introduces one MNP with a predicate that locates it or otherwise gives an identifying characteristic of it. That is, the first has a monadic V. The second clause introduces the other MNP with a dyadic V, that is, a V that takes two arguments. In the second clause the argument of the dyadic V that was introduced in the earlier clause is now not manually expressed.</p>
<p>In the first clause the MNP is the S of its clause per force. In the second clause, the MNP is typically the S. Very often, this second clause tells what the referent of the MNP in the second clause does to the referent of the MNP in the first clause. That is, the MNP in the first clause is coreferential with the O of the second clause (which is not manually expressed). This construction is known as “the split-sentence construction,” and has been characterized as S
<sub>1</sub>
V S
<sub>2</sub>
V, since each subject precedes its predicate, as exemplified here in Italian Sign Language (Volterra et al.,
<xref rid="B211" ref-type="bibr">1984</xref>
, p. 32):</p>
<disp-quote>
<p>BAMBINO SEDUTO MAMMA PETTINARE</p>
<p>child           seated       mother     comb</p>
<p>‘The child sits and the mother combs (his) hair.’</p>
</disp-quote>
<p>This signing stream conveys that the mother combs the hair of the seated child. The point for us is that instead of signing this proposition in a single clause with two MNPs, the choice is to have two clauses with only one MNP per clause, where that MNP is the S of the predicate and precedes it.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Generalization four</title>
<p>When two MNPs occur in a locational expression that forms a single clause, larger more immobile objects tend to precede smaller more mobile ones, regardless of theta role or grammatical function.</p>
<p>However, animacy complicates the situation (see remarks in the section Order and the Visual Modality). We are appealing here to properties of the referents of the signs, not to properties of the signs themselves.</p>
<p>This fact is explicitly remarked on by many, and it is subsumed under the figure-ground principle (Happ and Verköper,
<xref rid="B91" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
). An example from German Sign Language is seen here (the example is from Leuninger,
<xref rid="B121" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
, p. 238; the translation is from Plaza-Pust,
<xref rid="B171" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
, p. 85):</p>
<disp-quote>
<p>WAND
<sub>1</sub>
JACKE ICH HANG_AN
<sub>1</sub>
‘I hang up the jacket on the</p>
<p>wall        jacket   I      hang-on                                        wall.’</p>
</disp-quote>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Generalization five</title>
<p>O is immediately adjacent to V.</p>
<p>Evidence for this comes from the order observed in the vast majority of examples in our survey. Certainly the order OSV occurs often in sign languages, but the literature overwhelmingly analyzes this as topicalization of O (indicated typically by prosodic cues and/or discourse contexts; Padden,
<xref rid="B164" ref-type="bibr">1988</xref>
; Lillo-Martin,
<xref rid="B127" ref-type="bibr">1991</xref>
; Petronio,
<xref rid="B170" ref-type="bibr">1993</xref>
). This generalization supports the idea that there is a verb phrase (VP) in sign languages.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Generalization six</title>
<p>In reversible sentences with plain verbs, SVO is favored.</p>
<p>Several studies note this tendency, regardless of the word order a language exhibits in non-reversible sentences. This tendency is noted so often that when a language does not exhibit it, the authors typically explicitly say that (as for Sign Language of the Netherlands, Coerts,
<xref rid="B38" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
). Surprisingly, a study of Flemish Sign Language found more variation in word order in reversible sentences (where we find SOV and OSV) than in non-reversible (where we find only SOV) (Vermeerbergen,
<xref rid="B208" ref-type="bibr">1996</xref>
). For the languages that favor SVO with plain verbs in reversible sentences, it would seem that NP
<sub>1</sub>
V NP
<sub>2</sub>
order is not ambiguous (interpreted only as SVO), whereas NP
<sub>1</sub>
NP
<sub>2</sub>
V order is open to the readings SOV and OSV (and see Fischer,
<xref rid="B66" ref-type="bibr">1975</xref>
). In contrast, Kimmelman (
<xref rid="B111" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
) points out for Russian Sign Language, that, since OSV is marked, the cues that go with topicalization of the O should eliminate ambiguity in reversible sentences. The observation captured in generalization six remains, and we return to discussion of possible motivation in sections An Amodal Account and A Modal Account.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>A comparison to two accounts</title>
<p>We list the generalizations here for easy reference:</p>
<list list-type="simple">
<list-item>
<p>
<bold>Generalization One</bold>
. SOV is grammatical in all sign languages.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>
<bold>Generalization Two</bold>
. If an argument affects the phonological shape of the V, it precedes V.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>
<bold>Generalization Three</bold>
. The most common sentence type has only one new argument, which precedes the V.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>
<bold>Generalization Four</bold>
. When two MNPs occur in a locational expression that forms a single clause, the larger more immobile objects tend to precede smaller more mobile ones, regardless of theta role or grammatical function.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>
<bold>Generalization Five</bold>
. O is immediately adjacent to V.</p>
</list-item>
<list-item>
<p>
<bold>Generalization Six</bold>
. In reversible sentences with plain verbs, SVO is favored.</p>
</list-item>
</list>
<p>Taken together, we arrive at the generalization that SV is the order we find in most intransitive sign language sentences, and SOV and SVO are the orders for transitive sentences. Further, the choice between SOV and SVO is frequently determined by phonological considerations, where most of the time SOV should be preferred.</p>
<sec>
<title>An amodal account</title>
<p>One possible account of these generalizations is amodal: perhaps there are universal pressures on language that favor these patterns.</p>
<p>Consider generalization one. If we categorize languages by the six possible string permutations of S, O, and V, we find that together SOV and SVO characterize around 76% of spoken languages (Dryer,
<xref rid="B59" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
), where SOV is dominant and SVO is not far behind. (For a current count, see Dryer's ongoing site
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://wals.info/chapter/81">http://wals.info/chapter/81</ext-link>
. There, 41% of the 1377 spoken languages considered are SOV, and 35% are SVO.) Further, many V-initial languages also have an alternate word order with the S preceding the V, as in Arabic and Berber, in contrast to SOV languages, which tend to be strictly V-final in unmarked sentences (Tomlin,
<xref rid="B203" ref-type="bibr">1986</xref>
; Herring and Paolillo,
<xref rid="B94" ref-type="bibr">1995</xref>
; among many). We might therefore want to conclude that SOV or SVO is possible in all languages. The biggest problem for this conclusion is the Celtic family. Celtic languages have been claimed to be rigidly VSO except for main clauses in Breton and Cornish (Tallerman,
<xref rid="B198" ref-type="bibr">1998</xref>
). There is not complete agreement on this, however. A drift toward SVO has been documented for Breton and Welsh (Raney,
<xref rid="B177" ref-type="bibr">1984</xref>
; but see Willis,
<xref rid="B218" ref-type="bibr">1998</xref>
for Welsh), and a claim made that SVO is more frequent in modern Breton than VSO (Varin,
<xref rid="B207" ref-type="bibr">1979</xref>
; but see Timm,
<xref rid="B202" ref-type="bibr">1989</xref>
). We conclude that, on the whole, languages in general favor SOV, not just sign languages, and languages in general favor adjacency of V and O.</p>
<p>But the tendency for SOV is stronger in sign languages. Why? Some linguists argue that SOV is the default order for human language (including Givón,
<xref rid="B80" ref-type="bibr">1979</xref>
; Newmeyer,
<xref rid="B156" ref-type="bibr">2000a</xref>
). Newmeyer (
<xref rid="B157" ref-type="bibr">2000b</xref>
), in fact, claims SOV was the order in proto-language. Sign languages are young, so perhaps the acceptability of SOV in all sign languages follows. Indeed, could all the generalizations we noted in the immediately preceding section hold of young languages in general?</p>
<p>Many languages are known to have changed diachronically from SOV to SVO. In Indo-European, this is the case with English (Canale,
<xref rid="B27" ref-type="bibr">1978</xref>
, among many), Greek (Taylor,
<xref rid="B200" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
), Swedish (Delsing,
<xref rid="B54" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
), Icelandic (Hróarsdóttir,
<xref rid="B97" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
: p. 60), Norwegian (Sundquist,
<xref rid="B195" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
), Spanish (Parodi,
<xref rid="B168" ref-type="bibr">1995</xref>
), and Italian (Antinucci et al.,
<xref rid="B4" ref-type="bibr">1979</xref>
). (And see Fischer,
<xref rid="B65" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
for discussion of word order change in general, with a focus on Indo-European languages.) In Sino-Tibetan, this is the case with Bai, the Karen languages of Thailand and Burma, and may be responsible for a number of complex word order facts in languages of China (Dryer,
<xref rid="B58" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
). In Niger-Congo, this is the case with Bantu languages (Givón,
<xref rid="B78" ref-type="bibr">1975</xref>
). And the list continues. Rarely, however, do we find diachronic change in the opposite direction (Gell-Mann and Ruhlen,
<xref rid="B73" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
). Some exceptions are the Austronesian language Motu (Crowley,
<xref rid="B45" ref-type="bibr">1992</xref>
), the Western Oceanic language Takia (Ross,
<xref rid="B182" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
), the Tai-Kadai language Kamti Tai (Khanittanan,
<xref rid="B110" ref-type="bibr">1986</xref>
), and a few others, where that change is argued to be an influence from contact with an SOV language. (For overview and citations see Van Gelderen,
<xref rid="B206" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
). Further, emerging sign languages favor SOV strongly (Meir et al.,
<xref rid="B142" ref-type="bibr">2010b</xref>
).</p>
<p>With respect to generalization three, while there is an enormous literature on (in)transitivity, trying to estimate the prevalence of different valencies is far from obvious (as in Brew and Schulte im Walde,
<xref rid="B26" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
). In the substantial literature on creoles, no one, to our knowledge, discusses the relative prevalence of intransitive to transitive sentences (see, for example, McWhorter,
<xref rid="B135" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
). And we are aware of no literature on any spoken language that claims that a particular language or language family has a tendency toward having only one fully referential NP (that is, an NP that is not a pronoun or an anaphor) in a clause or about young languages having such a tendency.</p>
<p>With respect to generalization four and spoken languages, again there is considerable literature on locational, existential, and possessive expressions, which have a number of semantic similarities. But much of that literature is concerned precisely with those semantic properties (for example, Hoekstra and Mulder,
<xref rid="B95" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
). Some of the literature, however, addresses word order. Clark (
<xref rid="B37" ref-type="bibr">1978</xref>
, p. 88), for example, notes that “roughly speaking” definite NPs precede indefinite ones in English and French sentences of this type. However, we know of no claims to the effect that the size or mobility of the referent of an NP matters in the determination of word order in spoken languages.</p>
<p>One can also look to word order in spoken creoles with respect to the claim that young languages favor adjacency of V and O—that is, to support the claim that generalization five is true of young languages, since creoles are by and large young languages. DeGraff (
<xref rid="B50" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
,
<xref rid="B51" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
) surveys a number of creoles and shows that, despite claims to the contrary (as in Bickerton,
<xref rid="B12" ref-type="bibr">1981</xref>
,
<xref rid="B13" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
and following), creoles are not an exceptional kind of language morphologically and syntactically. In particular, SVO is not the (near) universal word order for creoles. Instead, creole VPs can be OV or VO. Still, it appears that many more creoles are SVO than SOV (Julien,
<xref rid="B107" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
). So the evidence from creoles is not compelling with respect to the claim that young languages favor SOV (generalization one).</p>
<p>With respect to generalization six, while many languages allow a wide range of ambiguities, word order can be sensitive to situations of potential ambiguity with regard to grammatical functions (particularly S and O); indeed, sometimes in potential-ambiguity contexts in spoken language we do not find the otherwise expected word orders (Craig,
<xref rid="B40" ref-type="bibr">1977</xref>
for Jacaltec, Kuhn,
<xref rid="B116" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
for German, Lee,
<xref rid="B118" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
for Hindi and Korean, Vulanoviæ,
<xref rid="B212" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
and Flack Potts,
<xref rid="B69" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
for Japanese). Speakers of English adjust their word order to avoid ambiguity when the visual context is the source of the potential ambiguity (Haywood et al.,
<xref rid="B93" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
). While we have found no mention that this tendency is stronger in young languages, it certainly appears to be evidence of a natural language principle.</p>
<p>The only remaining generalization to be addressed with respect to spoken languages, number two, calls for a more complex discussion. The situation in spoken languages is interestingly complex, and we restrict the discussion here to the tense-carrying V (not to participles, which enter into a different paradigm). In general, for an argument to affect the phonological shape of the V (an effect that is arbitrary with respect to meaning for spoken languages—we return to this point in the section Order and the Visual Modality, when we discuss generalization two), there must be agreement between the two. Most commonly, if there is agreement, the V agrees with the S alone. Since S precedes V in most languages, this is no problem for our generalization. However, nearly 9% of spoken languages are V-initial (conflating the VSO and VOS examples on the site
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://wals.info/chapter/81">http://wals.info/chapter/81</ext-link>
), among them the Celtic languages. In all the Celtic languages, the V does not agree with an S that is a fully referential NP, but it might agree with a pronominal S (whether overt or “pro”), as happens in Welsh (Borsley and Roberts,
<xref rid="B17" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
, p. 40). But the very conditions for a pronominal S are that the referent already be present on the conversational scene. This is consistent with our motivation for generalization two. On the other hand, various varieties of Arabic allow both VSO and SVO order, but the V still agrees with the S even when the S follows the V, although interesting complications arise. In particular, in Standard Arabic (as opposed to Lebanese or Moroccan Arabic) when the S follows the V we find agreement for gender only, not for person and number, but when the S precedes the V, we find agreement for the full range of features (Aoun et al.,
<xref rid="B5" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
; Alexiadou and Anagnostopoulou,
<xref rid="B3" ref-type="bibr">1998</xref>
).</p>
<p>Further, some languages allow agreement of a V with O, either direct object (as with Hungarian, Ge'ez, and Eastern Aramaic) or both direct and indirect object (as with Amharic, Swahili, and Lebanese Arabic), where O might well-follow V. Again, we find interesting complications. In Lebanese Arabic, where O follows the V, V can agree with an O only if it is definite (Koutsoudas,
<xref rid="B114" ref-type="bibr">1969</xref>
). The same is true of Swahili (Givón,
<xref rid="B79" ref-type="bibr">1976</xref>
). Since definite NPs are used when the referent is already on the conversational scene, generalization two seems to loom in the background again. On the other hand, in Amharic a definite O triggers agreement on the V, while an indefinite O does not (Baker,
<xref rid="B9" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
), going exactly counter to our expectations if generalization two holds of spoken languages.</p>
<p>We have not done a survey of agreement facts in general, and agreement is remarkably messy (see Moravcsik,
<xref rid="B150" ref-type="bibr">1988</xref>
). However, it seems clear that generalization two is not true of spoken languages, young or not, especially since we have found no typologists' claims to this effect.</p>
<p>In sum, an amodal account explains the preference for SOV, for the adjacency of V and O, and for word order to resolve potential ambiguities that arise in reversible sentences. But it does not account for the preference for clauses with only one fully referential NP, for word order in existentials and presentational sentences, nor for the phonological and semantic factors that affect word order in sign languages (i.e., generalizations two through four).</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>A modal account</title>
<p>The alternative account we now consider is that these generalizations are a result of the modality of sign languages.</p>
<p>With respect to generalization one, a number of studies of gesture conclude that SOV is the default order in visual communication. In one study, Gershkoff-Stowe and Goldin-Meadow (
<xref rid="B74" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
) had English speakers describe scenes solely with gesture, and in another they presented speakers with pictures and asked them to order them in a way that would communicate a given scene. In both, people presented scenes in the order SMA—
<bold>s</bold>
tationary entity, moving entity, action. Importantly, the order of stationary before mobile entity is exactly what we find in sign languages, expressed in generalization four.</p>
<p>So et al. (
<xref rid="B193" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
) asked English speakers to describe vignettes in speech accompanied by gestures created on the spot as well as solely in gestures. When using gestures alone, the hands exploited space for reference and coreference more often than when speech was also used, and the types of entities the gestures represented differed. Most gestures accompanying speech concerned action, but gestures alone also concerned entities. From the data given, it appears that the order of “constituents” in gesture-only propositions resembles that in sign languages. For example, this is the description of a man communicating “man gives woman basket” with gestures (So et al.,
<xref rid="B193" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
, p. 1032):</p>
<disp-quote>
<p>He first set up one person (man) on his body [G1] and a second person (woman) on his right [G2]. He then produced a GIVE gesture moving from a location in front of him (later identified as basket) to the location to his right (woman) [G3, which was coreferential with G2]. After producing a gesture for basket in the location in front of him [G4, which was coreferential with G3], he again produced a GIVE gesture moving from the basket location to the woman location [G5, which was coreferential with G2, G3, and G4].</p>
</disp-quote>
<p>We see clearly the strategy of setting up participants in an action, then expressing the action. And, when relevant, we see the strategy of setting up the S before other participants. Importantly, we see that the action gesture, whose articulatory shape is affected by the participants, appears after those participants, just as in sign languages (see generalization two).</p>
<p>Goldin-Meadow et al. (
<xref rid="B86" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
) likewise find that SOV recurs in non-verbal communication. They had native speakers of languages with varying word orders (English, Turkish, Spanish, Chinese) perform studies like those in Gershkoff-Stowe and Goldin-Meadow (
<xref rid="B74" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
)—using wholly gestures in one study and arranging pictures in another, but now the scenes involved actions from an agent onto a patient (like transitive verbs) rather than intransitive changing-location actions. The order of constituents in speakers' native languages did not influence the order in these visual tasks. They conclude that SOV is the “natural order that we impose on events when describing and reconstructing them non-verbally and exploit when constructing language anew” (Goldin-Meadow et al.,
<xref rid="B86" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
, p. 9163).</p>
<p>Langus and Nespor (
<xref rid="B117" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
) replicated Goldin-Meadow et al.s' (
<xref rid="B86" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
) experiments with speakers of Italian and Turkish. Their results led them to a similar conclusion about the early stages of an emerging language: SOV is the preferred order in “simple improvised communication” (Langus and Nespor,
<xref rid="B117" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
, p. 293). In another experiment they concluded that improvised communication does not organize its constituents hierarchically, in contrast to natural language. In a third experiment, they tested speech comprehension of sentences with prosodically flat words, where S, O, and V appeared in all possible orders and concluded that, while speakers understand best sentences whose order conforms to that of their native language (SVO for Italian; SOV for Turkish), compared reaction time in recognition of the meaning of speech strings with varying order shows a preference for V to precede O. They conclude that the computational system of grammar prefers SVO, whereas the preference for SOV in improvisational communication demonstrates “a direct link between the sensory-motor and the conceptual systems that prevails in gesture production” (Langus and Nespor,
<xref rid="B117" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
, p. 308). In other words, SVO is the preferred syntactic order, with SOV being the natural conceptual order.</p>
<p>Gibson et al. (
<xref rid="B76" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
), in a gesture-production task with speakers of English, Japanese, and Korean, conclude that SOV is, indeed, the preferred order in gestural communication, but SVO arises when communication needs demand it, as in reversible events. The same is true in emerging sign languages; when asked to use gesture to describe reversible events in which both participants are animate (“girl kicks fireman”/“fireman kicks girl”), people prefer SVO (Meir et al.,
<xref rid="B139" ref-type="bibr">2010a</xref>
). This echoes the behavior of many sign languages, as stated in generalization six.</p>
<p>Gibson and colleagues tie this to works on language proper that claim SOV is the default order for human language. Their explanation for this shift to SVO in reversible events is based on the “noisy-channel” hypothesis (Shannon,
<xref rid="B190" ref-type="bibr">1948</xref>
; Levy,
<xref rid="B122" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
; Levy and Jaeger,
<xref rid="B123" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
; the quote here is from Gibson et al.,
<xref rid="B76" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
, p. 1081).</p>
<disp-quote>
<p>A speaker wishes to convey a meaning m and chooses an utterance u to do so. This utterance is conveyed across a channel that may corrupt u in some way, resulting in a received utterance ũ. The noise may result from errors on the side of the producer, external noise, or errors on the side of the listener. The listener must use ũ to determine the intended meaning m. The best strategy for a speaker is thus to choose an utterance u that will maximize the listener's ability to recover the meaning given the noise process.</p>
</disp-quote>
<p>Languages need to be robust against this omnipresent noise. Essentially, a representation of an event with an animate patient is more robust to noise when the agent and patient are separated by the action (V). Spoken languages with SOV order can be robust against interfering noise by using case-marking, and they point out that case-marking is prevalent in SOV languages but almost absent in SVO languages.</p>
<p>Since languages are known to have changed diachronically from SOV to SVO, as discussed in section An Amodal Account, the idea that a noisy channel might be the impetus for such change arises. Hall et al. (
<xref rid="B88" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
) address this issue; they asked speakers of English to describe in pantomime both reversible and non-reversible transitive events. Critically, speakers always took on the role of actor, and Hall and colleagues noted what they call a “role conflict” in reversible events (Hall et al.,
<xref rid="B88" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
, p. 5):</p>
<disp-quote>
<p>To describe a non-reversible event (e.g., a woman lifting a box) using SOV order, participants would generally adopt the role of the agent (long hair), then produce a gesture for the box without adopting any role. In this case, the participant does not need to do anything special to re-inhabit the role of agent in time to produce the action gesture. In contrast, using SOV for reversible events (e.g., a man lifting a woman) is likely to entail a role conflict between O and V. For example, if a participant described a reversible event using SOV order, she or he would first adopt the role of the agent (flexing muscles), then the patient (long hair). The participant is now in the patient role but is ready to produce the action, which requires him or her to be in the agent role. If the participant were to produce an action gesture without first doing something to switch back into the agent role, it may “feel” to him or her as if it is the patient and not the agent that is carrying out the action. It is this that we refer to as role conflict.</p>
</disp-quote>
<p>They suggest that the preference for SVO in reversible events is due to a desire to avoid role conflict. And they note that when speakers do produce SOV order in reversible events, they find ways to get around the potential role conflict, either by not embodying the role of the patient (perhaps simply tracing it in space) or by establishing a spatial location for agent and another for patient and then shifting appropriately between them when they pantomime the action. (Spatial marking is also observed in Gibson et al.,
<xref rid="B76" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
, who compare it to case marking in spoken languages.)</p>
<p>Schouwstra (
<xref rid="B187" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
) also addresses the issue of a natural word order by looking at gesture in an improvised communication experiment. Many of her findings echo those of earlier scholars. Her work differs, however, in arguing that constituent ordering is influenced not only by the cognitive abilities involved in making an analogy between language meaning and cognitive representations (and see de Swart,
<xref rid="B56" ref-type="bibr">2009</xref>
), but also by the communicative needs involved in public expression, where the conventional nature of language imposes itself (Roberge,
<xref rid="B178" ref-type="bibr">2009</xref>
). Participants view an event on a screen. Then they use gesture to describe it. The process of transitioning from the simultaneity of the picture to the linearization of the gesture string forces participants to consciously choose the order in which they present things. This choice can be made on grounds of communicative needs. Schouwstra makes a distinction between “motion events,” which involve extensional predicates (that create transparent contexts), such as
<italic>carries</italic>
in “princess carries vase,” and “intensional events,” which involve intensional predicates (that create opaque contexts), such as
<italic>think of</italic>
in “cook thinks of sock”. Both Turkish and Dutch speakers strongly preferred SOV order in their gestural representation of motion events, but SVO order (though less strongly) in their gestural representation of intensional events. Schouwstra then looked at order in events involving a subset of intensional predicates, the creation verbs. She found that the tendency toward SVO was smaller with creation verbs than with other intensional verbs but was still the preferred order. (Indeed, we found evidence of pressure toward SVO with creation verbs in our study, but nothing conclusive.) There is no doubt that semantics influences word order in these experiments. As Schouwstra (
<xref rid="B187" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
, p. 148) says, “When making a sequence of the different elements, they [the participants] are forced to impose an order on the information. So it is only in making the information public, in being involved in communication, that ordering plays a role.” Likewise, she found that when people interpret gesturing of others, SOV strings are interpreted more often as motion events than SVO strings are, and SVO strings are interpreted more often as intensional events than are SOV strings. “This shows that in emerging communication systems, meaning and structure have more to do with each other than previously thought. Moreover, it suggests that ordering information in utterances in these systems is quite an active process, rather than simply a reproduction of how information is represented mentally” (Schouwstra,
<xref rid="B187" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
, p. 148).</p>
<p>Christensen and Tylén (
<xref rid="B35" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
) offer another gestural communication experiment which uses an interactional paradigm instead of an elicitation task. Participants communicate to a passive experimenter or a camcorder, thus participating in proper bidirectional communication, where dyads are dependent on mutual comprehension of the gestural systems that evolve during the experiment sessions. They followed up on Schouwstra's work, contrasting “object manipulation events” to “construction events,” the latter of which involve effective verbs. The former consistently yielded SOV order, while the latter yielded SVO order, as we also found for sign languages, but with far too few examples to base a generalization upon. Again, we see that event structure rather than a cognitively natural order influences order in these gestural strings.</p>
<p>So the data on gestural communication is consistent with all the generalizations of section Generalizations in the Data.</p>
<p>Further, homesigners often produce strings of V plus one argument, where they place the V finally (that is, SV or OV) (Goldin-Meadow,
<xref rid="B85" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
). And studies of young sign languages, still with a relatively unstable grammar, reveal a tendency for utterances to consist of SV, OV, and SOV (Senghas et al.,
<xref rid="B189" ref-type="bibr">1997</xref>
; Sandler et al.,
<xref rid="B185" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
; Haviland,
<xref rid="B92" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
). These findings are, so far as they go, consistent with the generalizations of section Generalizations in the Data.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Conclusion</title>
<p>The amodal account covers some of our generalizations; the modal account covers all. One might then conclude that our observations on sign languages are evidence of a natural visual order. That is, we know vision is at play in both producing and receiving gestural strings and sign languages, so if one is to claim some other cognitive ability is at play, the burden of proof lies on them.</p>
<p>Nevertheless, the fact that visual communication (gesture and sign languages) and spoken languages, particularly young languages, share important tendencies in order of constituents should make us wary of such a conclusion. It seems unlikely that totally independent pressures on sign languages and spoken languages would happen to produce such similarities. Two logical possibilities come up. One is that the pressures evidenced in the generalizations about order in sign languages really do hold of language in general, but that over time evidence for several of them has been lost as these pressures yield to competing pressures (whatever they might be), or several of them are simply gapped in spoken language. This possibility is not open to testing, unfortunately, but the speculation remains (and see Hale,
<xref rid="B87" ref-type="bibr">1975</xref>
for discussion of gaps in universals).</p>
<p>The other possibility is that the word order generalizations for sign languages reveal universal pressures augmented by visual pressures. As Chomsky (
<xref rid="B34" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
, p. 35) says, “… each language incorporates a mechanism that determines an infinite array of hierarchically structured expressions that are transferred for interpretation to two interfaces: the sensorymotor system SM for externalization and the conceptual-intentional system CI, for thought (broadly understood).” The structured expressions in spoken and sign languages are transferred to different sensorimotor systems—leading to different realizations.</p>
<p>At this point one might be led to the reasonable position that the universal pressures on word order discussed in this paper are grammatical in nature, while the pressures that apply only to sign language word order are visual in nature. Still, there is a way to see a coherence in the two sets of pressures. If, in fact, pressures of both the auditory and visual systems are behind the universal pressures on word order, we can view the sensorimotor pressures as motivating this particular part of universal grammar, which is apparent in both spoken and sign languages. Certainly, biological sources as a foundation for universal grammar should be seriously examined. After all, the innate language faculty, which serves for both spoken and sign languages, evolved somehow.</p>
<p>Given that language is embedded in the neuronal circuitry of the brain, and given that motor, cognitive, and perceptual systems are implicated in language learning and language use, we may assume that the language faculty should have come from pre-existing competencies, which initially were unrelated to language (Cowie,
<xref rid="B44" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
; and, for compatible remarks, see even non-nativists, such as Tomasello,
<xref rid="B205" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
). Certainly, finding evidence today that bears on human cognitive evolution is a daunting job, but our findings here suggest that comparative studies of languages in different modalities may offer new ways to approach the issue (and see Napoli and Sutton-Spence,
<xref rid="B154" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
). Whatever the truth about language evolution may turn out to be, the birth of the language faculty will have been complex and, if we are correct, will involve many other competencies that developed earlier and were then adapted to language, with the sensorimotor systems playing a significant role.</p>
<p>The idea that shared language properties may follow from shared pressures of the visual and auditory sensorimotor systems seems to be gaining strength in the neuroscience field. Tettamanti and colleagues argue (2005, p. 273), “… listening to sentences that describe actions engages the visuomotor circuits which subserve action execution and observation” (but see Mortan Ann Gernsbacher's remarks in Gallese et al.,
<xref rid="B72" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
). Further, the prevalence of SOV and SVO accords well with the representation of action in Broca's area (Kemmerer,
<xref rid="B109" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
; but for arguments that Broca's area does not have a unified function, see Fedorenko et al.,
<xref rid="B64" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
). Additionally, neural tissue involved in language processing involves polymodal neural activity, so the idea that the different sensorimotor systems would share properties may follow from a cooperation of these neural activities (Petitto et al.,
<xref rid="B169" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
). And, finally, there is evidence that intellectual and perceptual-motor skills involve hierarchical unpacking of chunks of knowledge (Rosenbaum et al.,
<xref rid="B180" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
; Rosenbaum,
<xref rid="B179" ref-type="bibr">2009</xref>
; Clark,
<xref rid="B36" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
), thus sensorimotor-system pressures may even motivate the hierarchical nature of universal grammar.</p>
<p>Further, if this sensorimotor hypothesis about word order can be supported, it is the more interesting one since it calls for a reassessment of how to approach the issues of the order of the major constituents in language in general. Let us assume that the grammar of all languages organizes words into phrases, including VP. That means that OV and VO are both generated, depending on whether phrases in the language are head-initial or head-final. So the potential orders we can expect the relevant sensorimotor systems to produce in both spoken and sign languages for transitive sentences are SOV, OVS, SVO, and VOS. The fact that SOV and SVO occur so frequently in spoken language and so overwhelmingly in sign languages suggests that pressures of the sensorimotor systems favor S preceding VP. This accounts for the infrequency of spoken languages with unmarked word order being OVS (under 0.8%; 11 out of 1377) and VOS (under 2%; 25 out of 1377); they are bucking the sensorimotor system pressures. This also leads to the conclusion that OSV will be the result of topicalization from either SOV or SVO. That is, OSV should be a marked order in language, calling for contexts in which we are somehow highlighting the O. In fact, only 4 spoken languages out of 1377 have been claimed to have OSV as unmarked order (under 0.3%).</p>
<p>Finally, let's consider VSO. An immediate problem is that V and O are not adjacent. Further, we see no evidence of pressure from sensorimotor systems to favor V in initial position. As we discussed, VSO is (almost) non-existent in sign languages and is rare as an unmarked order in spoken languages (under 7%, 95 out of 1377). Importantly, as also discussed, VSO in spoken languages often has SVO as an alternate order. The strong consensus in the literature is that VSO arises from SVO via V-raising in order to satisfy requirements of the grammar (Choe,
<xref rid="B33" ref-type="bibr">1987</xref>
; Carnie and Guilfoyle,
<xref rid="B28" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
; for example), even for Irish (Bobaljik and Carnie,
<xref rid="B14" ref-type="bibr">1992</xref>
). (For details on the analysis, see Alexiadou and Anagnostopoulou,
<xref rid="B3" ref-type="bibr">1998</xref>
).</p>
<p>The sensorimotor hypothesis, then, says that S precedes VP as a fundamental strategy in language. This conclusion finds support in the language of people who are linguistically deprived in the sense that they were not exposed to accessible language during the early years of life. Such people generally manage to use appropriate word order in most situations, whereas many other properties of language are problematic for them. This is true of Genie, an abused girl who was not rescued until the age of around thirteen (Curtiss et al.,
<xref rid="B47" ref-type="bibr">1973</xref>
; Fromkin et al.,
<xref rid="B71" ref-type="bibr">1974</xref>
; Curtiss,
<xref rid="B46" ref-type="bibr">1977</xref>
; Goldin-Meadow,
<xref rid="B84" ref-type="bibr">1978</xref>
) and of deaf “late learners” (Johnson and Newport,
<xref rid="B100" ref-type="bibr">1989</xref>
; Newport,
<xref rid="B158" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
; Newport et al.,
<xref rid="B160" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
; Wood,
<xref rid="B222" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
). In fact, deaf children first exposed to ASL after the age of 6 do not produce appropriate variations in word order that native signers produce (even as young as 2 year olds), instead using SVO heavily (Lillo-Martin and Berk,
<xref rid="B128" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
). That it is SVO rather than SOV that these late learners produce is consistent with the fact that their morphology is underdeveloped, thus their verbs exhibit fewer instances of phonological shape affected by arguments (that is, fewer instances of the situations that call for SOV, see discussion in section Generalization Two) than verbs of native signers (Newport,
<xref rid="B159" ref-type="bibr">1991</xref>
). Thus, the sensorimotor hypothesis accounts for why some characteristics of language are “resilient” and others are “fragile” (Wood,
<xref rid="B221" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
,
<xref rid="B222" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
); the resilient ones are dependent upon sensorimotor pressures that exist regardless of language and that motivate certain parts of the grammar, while the fragile ones are not. In other words, late learners look at the world visually and their language is sensitive to visual pressures. On the other hand, they have trouble producing those grammatical structures that are not motivated by sensorimotor pressures, but are arbitrary to the particular language.</p>
<p>Given this explanatory force of the sensorimotor hypothesis, it is worth taking a closer look at what some of these pressures might be. The sensorimotor account of word order amounts to saying there are universal pressures driving the order similarities among sign and spoken languages, pressures that are imposed by factors that the visual and auditory sensorimotor systems have in common, and there are modality-specific pressures resulting in the order differences between sign and spoken languages, pressures imposed by the visual (-manual) sensorimotor system. In the next section we explore the relevant visual pressures on sign languages, and one suggestion of a pressure imposed by the manual articulators.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Order and the visual modality</title>
<p>Here we consider the generalizations that hold of sign languages but not of (young) spoken languages (i.e., generalizations two through five), and we argue each follows from visual needs or principles. Some of our accounts rely on coherence and iconicity; they turn upon the construction of a visual image, making testable predictions. There is no doubt that iconicity plays a role in sign language order. As De Langhe et al. (
<xref rid="B52" ref-type="bibr">2004</xref>
, p. 117) say (in our own translation), “… the most important thing for constructing sign expressions is iconicity… one must find the image that represents the subject and as soon as an image is formed in the mind, the translation into sign language becomes clear and easier.” Thus, there is pressure for temporal and spatial organization to work together coherently at every level of grammar, maximizing comprehensibility.</p>
<sec>
<title>Generalization two</title>
<p>If an argument affects the phonological shape of the V, it precedes V.</p>
<p>Why should sign languages but not spoken languages share this generalization? In a spoken language, the relationship between phonological features and meaning is (to a huge extent) arbitrary. In a sign language, that relationship is not arbitrary. Instead, the phonological shape of classifier predicates, agreeing verbs, pointing verbs, and argument-sensitive verbs will vary in non-arbitrary ways according to meaningful properties of their arguments, such as their spatial index and their size, shape, and general category (human, animal, small round object, and so on). For example, agreeing Vs involve a transfer of something (abstract or concrete) from one location to another. If visual perceptibility matters to the order of arguments, then we might expect an alignment such that the visual representation of transfer should involve a path that moves in the direction of the transfer. That is, the spatial-temporal organization should be coherent with the semantics of the utterance. This means that the point of initiation of the movement should be spatially indexed with the argument that is the origin of the transfer, and the endpoint of the movement should be spatially indexed with the argument that is the goal of the transfer (Meir,
<xref rid="B137" ref-type="bibr">1995</xref>
; Aronoff et al.,
<xref rid="B6" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
). In most of the sign languages we have read about with respect to Vs of giving and taking, the verb GIVE moves from a point indexed with the giver to a point indexed with the receiver of the gift; whereas the verb TAKE moves from a point indexed with the one (or the place) from whom something is taken to a point indexed with the taker. In such examples as in classifier constructions, we find “mappings of envisioned mental spaces onto signing space” (Taub,
<xref rid="B199" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
, p. 163). If the addressee is to make sense out of the phonological shape of these predicates, including the direction of path movement, the relevant arguments should already be present in the discourse or be introduced within the sentence before the V (for a similar claim, but with more conditions on it, see Yau,
<xref rid="B225" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
, pp. 152–153).</p>
<p>With respect to classifier constructions, the non-arbitrariness of phonological features is rampant. To express that someone almost gave something to someone else, one might move only halfway along the path from one spatial index to another, for example (Quadros and Quer,
<xref rid="B175" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
), perhaps with a dynamics that portrays hesitancy. Thus, iconicity can be a motivation with respect to the order of elements and with respect to various factors of a predicate's movement (direction, length of path, and so on), as well as with respect to other phonological parameters (such as facing of the hands, as in Meir,
<xref rid="B138" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
). Syntactic structure is here a linguistic construction that itself conveys meaning (Goldberg,
<xref rid="B82" ref-type="bibr">1995</xref>
,
<xref rid="B83" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
).</p>
<p>As final evidence that generalization two reflects semantic concerns that are realized visually, we note that sign languages, like spoken languages, can exhibit phonological feature-spreading rules (as in compounding in ASL, see Liddell and Johnson,
<xref rid="B125" ref-type="bibr">1986</xref>
). Such rules are purely phonological; they are arbitrary with respect to semantics, and in these instances feature spreading can be anticipatory as well as perseverative. So when phonological shape is arbitrary, signs can be affected by what follows linearly. It is only when phonological shape is meaningful (as with classifier predicates, agreeing verbs, pointing verbs, and argument-sensitive verbs) that the element that influences the phonological shape appears beforehand as the unmarked order.</p>
<p>Certainly it is possible to articulate a predicate whose phonological shape is affected by an argument before introducing the relevant argument (Padden,
<xref rid="B164" ref-type="bibr">1988</xref>
), but this order is marked. The effect, according to the linguistic consultants we have asked, is like holding back information for dramatic impact and then revealing it, as in
<italic>And in walked…. her husband!</italic>
</p>
<p>An example from Inuit Sign Language makes the point nicely (Schuit et al.,
<xref rid="B188" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
, p. 21):</p>
<disp-quote>
<p>INDEX-LOC
<sub>3
<italic>a</italic>
</sub>
SCOOP DRILL-HOLE-WITH-AUGER FINISH.</p>
<p>
<sub>3
<italic>a</italic>
</sub>
WALK
<sub>1</sub>
     TAKE-LONG-ITEM     
<sub>1</sub>
WALK
<sub>3
<italic>a</italic>
</sub>
     WHITE-MAN</p>
<p>CHISEL
<sub>V</sub>
.    DROP     LONG-THIN-OBJECT   MOVES-BELOW-</p>
<p>SURFACE.</p>
<p>‘Over there they started a hole with a scoop, and then drilled it with an ice-auger. Someone walked from there towards me and took my chisel. The white man walked back (to the hole) and used the chisel. Then he dropped it, and it went all the way to the bottom (of the sea).’</p>
</disp-quote>
<p>(The translation is Schuit et al.'s, but the following comments are ours.) In the second sentence, “
<sub>3a</sub>
WALK
<sub>1</sub>
” indicates that someone walked from spatial location 3a (where the scoop and then the ice-auger were used) to spatial location 1 (which is the signer's location). “TAKE-LONG-ITEM” indicates a classifier predicate in which someone is taking hold of a long item. “
<sub>1</sub>
WALK
<sub>3a</sub>
” indicates that someone walked from spatial location 1 back to spatial location 3a. And only now are we told that the someone was a white man and that the long object he took was the signer's chisel. Here an unspecified NP is spatially indexed; we can't see who it is, all we see is that he picked up something long. Then we see it's a white man and we realize what he picked up is, in fact, a (the signer's) chisel (from how he used it). The word order reflects clarification after the fact. That is, the spatial index (3a) and the classifier predicate (TAKE-LONG-ITEM) precede the information about who was in that spatial index and what long item that classifier predicate involves. The MNPs come late for dramatic impact.</p>
<p>Russian Sign Language presents a (partial) exception to generalization two. SOV is found with classifier predicates, whereas SVO is found with plain verbs, as we expect. But SVO is also found with agreeing verbs (Kimmelman,
<xref rid="B111" ref-type="bibr">2012</xref>
). And Volterra et al. (
<xref rid="B211" ref-type="bibr">1984</xref>
) report for Italian Sign Language that in non-reversible sentences, SOV is used only if the V is a classifier or somehow else incorporates the O. However, they also say that SVO, the unmarked order, can occur under the same conditions (but see Cecchetto et al.,
<xref rid="B30" ref-type="bibr">2006</xref>
for the analysis of Italian Sign Language as SOV).</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Generalization three</title>
<p>The most common sentence type has only one new argument, which precedes the V.</p>
<p>The fact that the lone argument tends to precede the V is shared by (young) spoken languages. What's not shared is a particular strategy that sign languages often employ. Essentially, sign languages tend to put the relevant players on the stage one at a time, focusing our attention with a single spotlight on a single player, then moving that spotlight to a second player, and so forth. We saw that same strategy in gestural strings and in homesign (discussed in section A Modal Account).</p>
<p>Possibly, this is a visual strategy. While the retina can receive much information (our visual environment is typically cluttered), at a given time, only a small amount of that information can be processed. “Subjectively, giving attention to any one target leaves less available for others” (Desimone and Duncan,
<xref rid="B55" ref-type="bibr">1995</xref>
, p. 193). By introducing only one argument per predicate, we increase the chance that each argument will get attention, enhancing good communication of the event. Nevertheless, signers can convey information simultaneously with multiple articulators (both hands, various parts of the face). So we are not convinced this is a visual strategy.</p>
<p>More likely, it is a manual strategy. The manual articulators move slowly in comparison to the speech articulators, which means it takes time to set things up. So once we have the stage set, there's no need to keep doing something as uneconomical as repeating information everyone already knows.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Generalization four</title>
<p>When two MNPs occur in a locational expression that forms a single clause, the larger more immobile objects tend to precede smaller more mobile ones, regardless of theta role or grammatical function.</p>
<p>Among others, Volterra et al. (
<xref rid="B211" ref-type="bibr">1984</xref>
, pp. 35, 38) suggest this ordering is a direct result of the visual modality because larger objects are perceptually more important, a suggestion supported by a study on the order of gestures (not signs) in which participants consistently place a gesture for a larger stationary object before a gesture for a smaller moving one (Gershkoff-Stowe and Goldin-Meadow,
<xref rid="B74" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
). On the other hand, in both existential and locational sentences animate objects tend to precede inanimate ones (although see Coerts,
<xref rid="B38" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
and Kristoffersen,
<xref rid="B115" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
for complications), and sometimes these two principles conflict, which is the explanation these studies give for freer word order in existential/locational sentences, and which is the reason why we did not offer a separate generalization about word order in existential/locational sentences in particular.</p>
<p>A sign utterance that conveys relative spatial information about two objects creates that information spatially and, thus, evokes a cognitive representation of those objects in those spatial positions. It appears that, with respect to that evoked representation, sign languages are sensitive to the relevant visual principles. Studies show that perception of small objects (under 10 cm) differs from perception of large objects (Pakhomova,
<xref rid="B167" ref-type="bibr">2000</xref>
). Further we perceive small objects as moving more quickly than large objects even when they are moving at the same rate (Leibowitz,
<xref rid="B120" ref-type="bibr">1965</xref>
). So the fact that existential/ locational sentences tend to establish the location of large objects before they establish the location of small objects appears to follow from some property of visual perception.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Extra comment on generalization five</title>
<p>O is immediately adjacent to V.</p>
<p>Since this generalization holds of most spoken languages (which we expect, given the existence of VP) and of creoles (i.e., young languages) as well as sign languages, pressures common to all sensorimotor systems apply here. But Meir (
<xref rid="B138" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
) points out that in Israeli Sign Language a V can agree with its O without agreeing with the S, a situation not found in spoken languages. This suggests that the visual modality adds pressure for a visual unity or coherence of the V and O in sign languages.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Conclusions about visual and manual pressures</title>
<p>Sign languages are subject to the universal pressures on all languages. Some of those pressures are common to auditory and visual sensorimotor systems and, thus, we suggest they motivate parts of universal grammar. But sign languages are also subject to visual and perhaps manual pressures that set them apart from spoken languages. That sign languages should fall together typologically with respect to various aspects of grammar is not a new claim. For example, all sign languages use simultaneous expressions, a fact most often accounted for by the slowness of the manual articulators (Hohenberger,
<xref rid="B96" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
). By recognizing visual pressure on sign order, we can see that sign languages exploit simultaneity not simply because they can (given that spoken languages can, too—Napoli and Sutton-Spence,
<xref rid="B153" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
), nor totally because of the timing needs due to slow articulators, but because by exploiting it they can better align syntax and semantics with a visual coherence that is at the core of signing itself.</p>
<p>Our study argues that all sign languages will organize signs at the sentence level in a similar way partly because that's how all languages would do it, all else being equal, and partly because the visual modality entails creating pictures. Certainly these pictures are iconic in only the most abstract of ways and that iconicity is concentrated in the productive much more than in the frozen part of the lexicon (Klima and Bellugi,
<xref rid="B112" ref-type="bibr">1979</xref>
; McDonald,
<xref rid="B131" ref-type="bibr">1985</xref>
; Brennan,
<xref rid="B24" ref-type="bibr">1990</xref>
; Taub,
<xref rid="B199" ref-type="bibr">2001</xref>
; Russo,
<xref rid="B183" ref-type="bibr">2005</xref>
; Cuxac and Sallandre,
<xref rid="B49" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
; Sallandre,
<xref rid="B184" ref-type="bibr">2007</xref>
; Konrad,
<xref rid="B113" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
), otherwise any sighted person would be able to understand any sign language. Indeed, in the frozen lexicon, many signs are opaque in that their meanings are not guessable at all. And with respect to the others, signs whose meanings “are most directly interpreted from visibly present referents” or “can be shown by pantomimic expression” are more likely to be understood relatively accurately by people who do not know the given sign language than are signs whose meanings involve some kind of “metonymic association” or are “more culturally specific” (Boyes Braem et al.,
<xref rid="B21" ref-type="bibr">2002</xref>
, p. 187).</p>
<p>But once particular frozen lexical items are understood, and once one understands the nature of all the various types of predicates in sign languages, the organization of frozen and productive signs in the visual space and time of a sign sentence can be seen as largely iconic, where recognizing this iconicity calls for analogy, metaphor, metonymy, and other complex cognitive activities (Napoli and Sutton-Spence,
<xref rid="B154" ref-type="bibr">2011</xref>
). So the signed creation of pictures demands a visual coherency in order to be interpretable, and this demand for visual coherency should be equally high in any sign language.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Manual factors</title>
<p>A few of the studies we cite claim that manual considerations are relevant to word order. Nadeau and Desouvrey (
<xref rid="B151" ref-type="bibr">1994</xref>
, p. 156), in their study of Quebec Sign Language, suggest that SVO is favored for “mechanical” reasons, claiming that any other order would require additional transitional movements between the signs. Fischer (
<xref rid="B66" ref-type="bibr">1975</xref>
) mentions manual reasons for expecting the SOV order of ASL to change to OSV over time. Two studies point out that the O referred to in a handling classifier must immediately precede the classifier predicate (Jantunen,
<xref rid="B98" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
for Finnish Sign Language, Sze,
<xref rid="B197" ref-type="bibr">2003</xref>
for Hong Kong Sign Language in non-reversible sentences). We leave these remarks for future investigation.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Implications</title>
<p>Universal pressures and visual pressures conspire to bring about the generalizations we have found. We promoted the position here that those universal pressures follow from shared characteristics of the auditory and visual sensorimotor systems and we suggested that those shared characteristics are part of the motivation for universal grammar. Further, as visual pressures, in particular, play a stronger role in sign languages than in spoken languages, they mediate the emergence of the grammars of sign languages in such a way that sign languages tend to converge on a shared design that is, in the respects discussed in section Order and the Visual Modality, different from spoken languages.</p>
<p>We conclude that SOV and SVO should be the prevalent orders found in all declarative sentences in sign languages and that V-initial sentences should be restricted to presentational or existence sentences. In all of this, recall that we are talking only about the distribution of MNP arguments with respect to V. In fact, plain verbs are the only type that should show variation among languages in unmarked word order, specifically between SOV and SVO. That's because plain verbs are the only verbs whose phonological shape is not affected in an iconic way by their arguments. And, as it turns out, SOV and/or SVO are the unmarked orders for plain verbs across all the languages in the studies we examined (see remarks at the end of section Generalization Four under Order and the Visual Modality).</p>
<p>The account of sign order in sign languages that arises from our survey of the data in many studies needs to be tested through examination of a large video corpus, something that has not been possible for most linguists thus far. Fortunately, three major data corpora have recently been made available, for British Sign Language (BSL corpus project, discussed in Schembri,
<xref rid="B186" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
), Auslan (Johnston and Schembri,
<xref rid="B105" ref-type="bibr">2007b</xref>
; Johnston,
<xref rid="B101" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
,
<xref rid="B102" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
), and Sign Language of the Netherlands (Crasborn and Zwitserlood,
<xref rid="B43" ref-type="bibr">2008</xref>
). Similar databases are under construction, including for German Sign Language (Hanke et al.,
<xref rid="B90" ref-type="bibr">2010</xref>
), Italian Sign Language (Branchini et al.,
<xref rid="B22" ref-type="bibr">2009</xref>
), Chinese Sign Language (Zhang et al.,
<xref rid="B229" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
), and French Belgian Sign Language (Meurant and Sinte,
<xref rid="B143" ref-type="bibr">2013</xref>
). These databases can serve as a model for building databases for other sign languages, and they will enable researchers to make headway on linguistic analysis with confidence in the foundation upon which arguments are constructed and to both pose and answer questions regrettably infeasible without such a base. We offer our remarks here then, as a starting point for examining sign order with the goal of understanding better the sensorimotor system pressures affecting that order.</p>
</sec>
<sec>
<title>Author contributions</title>
<p>All parts of this work were done through collaboration of both authors.</p>
<sec>
<title>Conflict of interest statement</title>
<p>The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.</p>
</sec>
</sec>
</body>
<back>
<ack>
<p>Thanks to the Leverhulme Trust for awarding Donna Jo Napoli a Leverhulme Visiting Professorship in spring 2010, and to Swarthmore College for allowing her to accept. Thanks to the attendees at the linguistics research seminar at Newcastle University and at the University of Cambridge in spring 2010. Thanks to Trinity College Dublin for awarding Donna Jo Napoli a Long Room Hub Fellowship in summer 2012 to develop this research. Thanks to Swarthmore College for awarding Rachel Sutton-Spence the Cornell Visiting Professorship for 2011–2012, and to Bristol University for allowing her to accept. We thank Kearsy Cormier, Greg Carlson, Susan Goldin-Meadow, and our three anonymous referees for comments on earlier drafts. And our gratitude goes to Iris Berent, whose prodding questions helped us to see what we were actually trying to say and gave us the courage to say it.</p>
</ack>
<ref-list>
<title>References</title>
<ref id="B1">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Aboh</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pfau</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zeshan</surname>
<given-names>U.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>When a Wh-word is not a Wh-word: the case of Indian Sign Language</article-title>
, in
<source>The Yearbook of South Asian Languages and Linguistics</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Singh</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Berlin</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Walter de Gruyter</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>11</fpage>
<lpage>43</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B2">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Akach</surname>
<given-names>P. A. O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1992</year>
).
<article-title>Sentence formation in Kenyan Sign Language</article-title>
, in
<source>The East African Sign Language Seminar</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Akach</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mweri</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hempel</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Copenhagen</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Danish Deaf Association</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>45</fpage>
<lpage>51</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B3">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Alexiadou</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Anagnostopoulou</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1998</year>
).
<article-title>Parametrizing AGR: word order, V-movement and EPP-checking</article-title>
.
<source>Nat. Lang. Linguist. Theory</source>
<volume>16</volume>
,
<fpage>491</fpage>
<lpage>539</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1023/A:1006090432389</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B4">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Antinucci</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Duranti</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gebert</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1979</year>
).
<article-title>Relative clause structure, relative clause perception, and the change from SOV to SVO</article-title>
.
<source>Cognition</source>
<volume>7</volume>
,
<fpage>145</fpage>
<lpage>176</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0010-0277(79)90018-0</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">487727</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B5">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Aoun</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Benmamoun</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sportiche</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>Agreement, word order, and conjunction in some varieties of Arabic</article-title>
.
<source>Linguist. Inq</source>
.
<volume>25</volume>
,
<fpage>195</fpage>
<lpage>220</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B6">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Aronoff</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Meir</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Padden</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sandler</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Classifier constructions and morphology in two sign languages</article-title>
, in
<source>Perspectives on Classifier Constructions in Sign Languages</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Emmorey</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Mahwah, NJ</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Lawrence Erlbaum Associates</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>53</fpage>
<lpage>84</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B7">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bahan</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1996</year>
).
<source>Non-Manual Realization of Agreement in American Sign Language</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Boston, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Boston University</publisher-name>
Ph.D. dissertation. Available online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.bu.edu/asllrp/dissertatns.long.html">http://www.bu.edu/asllrp/dissertatns.long.html</ext-link>
(Accessed October 26, 2013).</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B8">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Baker</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cokely</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1980</year>
).
<source>American Sign Language: A Teacher's Resource Text on Grammar</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Silver Spring, MD</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>T.J. Publishers</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B9">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Baker</surname>
<given-names>M. C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>On the relationship of object agreement and accusative case: evidence from Amharic</article-title>
.
<source>Linguist. Inq</source>
.
<volume>43</volume>
,
<fpage>255</fpage>
<lpage>274</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1162/LING_a_00085</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B10">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Baron</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1998</year>
).
<article-title>Objet effettué et constructions nominales</article-title>
.
<source>Trav. Linguist</source>
.
<volume>35</volume>
,
<fpage>115</fpage>
<lpage>125</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B11">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bergman</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wallin</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1985</year>
).
<article-title>Sentence structure in Swedish Sign Language</article-title>
, in
<source>Sign Language Research '83</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Stokoe</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Volterra</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Silver Spring, MD</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Linstok Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>217</fpage>
<lpage>225</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B12">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bickerton</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1981</year>
).
<source>Roots of Language</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Ann Arbor, MI</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Karoma</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B13">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bickerton</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<source>Language and Species</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Chicago</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Chicago Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B14">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bobaljik</surname>
<given-names>J. D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Carnie</surname>
<given-names>A. H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1992</year>
).
<article-title>A Minimalist approach to some problems of irish word order</article-title>
, in
<source>Proceedings of the Harvard Celtic Colloquium</source>
, Vol. 12, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Hillers</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hunter</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Cambridge, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Department of Celtic Languages and Literatures, Harvard University</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>110</fpage>
<lpage>134</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B15">
<mixed-citation publication-type="webpage">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bobillo-García</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Morales-López</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Reigosa-Varela</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Freire-Rodríguez</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pérez-Casanova</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<source>El Orden de los Constituyentes de la Oración Declarative en la LSE</source>
. VI Congreso de Lingüística General, Universidad de Santiago de Compostela. Available online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.ruc.udc.es/dspace/bitstream/2183/644/1/OPCongrSantiago04.doc">http://www.ruc.udc.es/dspace/bitstream/2183/644/1/OPCongrSantiago04.doc</ext-link>
(Accessed October 26, 2013).</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B16">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bogaerde</surname>
<given-names>E. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>van den</surname>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mills</surname>
<given-names>A. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>Word order in language input to children: SLN or Dutch</article-title>
, in
<source>Word-order Issues in Sign Language</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Brennan</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Turner</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Durham</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>International Sign Linguistics Association</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>133</fpage>
<lpage>157</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B17">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Borsley</surname>
<given-names>R. D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Roberts</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(eds.). (
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Introduction</article-title>
in
<source>The Syntax of the Celtic Languages: A Comparative Perspective</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Borsley</surname>
<given-names>R. D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Roberts</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>52</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B18">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bouchard</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1996</year>
).
<article-title>Sign language and language universals: the status of order and position in grammar</article-title>
.
<source>Sign Lang. Stud</source>
.
<volume>91</volume>
,
<fpage>101</fpage>
<lpage>160</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1353/sls.1996.0018</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B19">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Bouchard</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dubuisson</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>Grammar, order and the position of wh-signs in Quebec Sign Language</article-title>
.
<source>Sign Lang. Stud</source>
.
<volume>87</volume>
,
<fpage>99</fpage>
<lpage>139</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1353/sls.1995.0007</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B20">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Boyes Braem</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fournier</surname>
<given-names>M.-L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rickli</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Corrazza</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Franchi</surname>
<given-names>M.-L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Volterra</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<article-title>A comparison of techniques for expressing semantic roles and locative relations in two different sign languages</article-title>
, in
<source>
<italic>SLR '87: Papers from the</italic>
Fourth
<italic>International Symposium on Sign Language Research</italic>
</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Edmondson</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Karlsson</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Hamburg</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Signum-Verlag</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>114</fpage>
<lpage>120</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B21">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Boyes Braem</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pizzuto</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Volterra</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>The interpretation of signs by (hearing and deaf) members of different cultures</article-title>
, in
<source>Progress in Sign Language Research/Fortschritte in der Gebärdensprachforschung</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Schulmeister</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Reinitzer</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Hamburg</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Signum Verlag</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>187</fpage>
<lpage>220</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B22">
<mixed-citation publication-type="webpage">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Branchini</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Geraci</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Giudice</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mereghetti</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Linguistic issues in building a corpus for LIS (Italian Sign Language)</article-title>
, in
<source>Sign Language Corpora: Linguistic Issues Workshop 2009 in London, British Sign Language Corpus Project</source>
. Available online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.bslcorpusproject.org/wp-content/uploads/slides-sl-corpora-london.pdf">http://www.bslcorpusproject.org/wp-content/uploads/slides-sl-corpora-london.pdf</ext-link>
(Accessed October 26, 2013).</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B23">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Braze</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Aspectual inflection, verb raising, and object fronting in American Sign Language</article-title>
.
<source>Lingua</source>
<volume>114</volume>
,
<fpage>29</fpage>
<lpage>58</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0024-3841(03)00034-2</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B24">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Brennan</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<source>Word Formation in British Sign Language</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Stockholm</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Stockholm University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B25">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Brennan</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>Word order: introducing the issues</article-title>
, in
<source>Word-order Issues in Sign Language</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Brennan</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Turner</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Durham</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>International Sign Linguistics Association</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>9</fpage>
<lpage>45</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B26">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Brew</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schulte im Walde</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Spectral clustering for German verbs</article-title>
, in
<source>Proceedings of the Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing ‘02</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Hagiè</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Matsumoto</surname>
<given-names>Y.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Stroudsburg, PA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Association for Computational Linguistics</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>117</fpage>
<lpage>124</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B27">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Canale</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1978</year>
).
<source>Word Order Change in OE: Base Reanalysis in Generative Grammar</source>
. Doctoral dissertation, University of Toronto.</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B28">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Carnie</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Guilfoyle</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(eds.). (
<year>2000</year>
).
<source>The Syntax of Verb Initial Languages</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B29">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Carston</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<source>Thoughts and Utterances: The Pragmatics of Explicit Communication</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Blackwell Publishers</publisher-name>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1002/9780470754603</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B30">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cecchetto</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Geraci</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zucchi</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Strategies of relativization in Italian Sign Language</article-title>
.
<source>Nat. Lang. Linguist. Theory</source>
<volume>24</volume>
,
<fpage>945</fpage>
<lpage>975</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s11049-006-9001-x</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B31">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Chen Pichler</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Using early ASL word order to shed light on word order variability in sign language</article-title>
, in
<source>Variation in the Input: Studies in The Acquisition of Word Order (Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics</source>
, Vol. 39), eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Anderssen</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bentzen</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Westergaard</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>The Netherlands</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Springer</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>157</fpage>
<lpage>177</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/978-90-481-9207-6_7</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B32">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Chen Pichler</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schalber</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hochgesang</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Milkovic</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wilbur</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Vulje</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Possession and existence in three sign languages</article-title>
, in
<source>Sign Languages: spinning and Unraveling the Past, Present and Future, TISLR9, Florianopolis, Brazil, December 2006</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Müller de Quadros</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Petrópolis; Rio de Janeiro</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Editora Arara Azul</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>440</fpage>
<lpage>458</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B33">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Choe</surname>
<given-names>H. S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1987</year>
).
<article-title>An SVO analysis of VSO languages and parameterization: a study of berber</article-title>
, in
<source>Studies in Berber Syntax. Lexicon Project Working Papers #14</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Guerssel</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hale</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Cambridge, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Center for Cognitive Science, MIT</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>121</fpage>
<lpage>158</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B34">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Chomsky</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Problems of projection</article-title>
.
<source>Lingua</source>
<volume>130</volume>
,
<fpage>33</fpage>
<lpage>49</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.lingua.2012.12.003</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B35">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Christensen</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tylén</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Gestural representation of event structure in dyadic interaction</article-title>
.
<source>Strings</source>
<volume>1140</volume>
,
<fpage>1241</fpage>
<lpage>1244</lpage>
Available online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.academia.edu/3613166/Gestural_representation_of_event_structure_in_dyadic_interaction">http://www.academia.edu/3613166/Gestural_representation_of_event_structure_in_dyadic_interaction</ext-link>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B36">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Clark</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science</article-title>
.
<source>Behav. Brain Sci</source>
.
<volume>36</volume>
,
<fpage>181</fpage>
<lpage>204</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1017/S0140525X12000477</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23663408</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B37">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Clark</surname>
<given-names>E. V.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1978</year>
).
<article-title>Locationals: existential, locative and possessive constructions</article-title>
, in
<source>Universals of Human Language</source>
, Vol. 4
<italic>Syntax</italic>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Greenberg</surname>
<given-names>J. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Stanford, CA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Stanford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>85</fpage>
<lpage>126</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B38">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Coerts</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>Constituent order in Sign Language of the Netherlands</article-title>
, in
<source>Word-order Issues in Sign Language</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Brennan</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Turner</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Durham</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>International Sign Linguistics Association</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>47</fpage>
<lpage>70</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B39">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Corrazza</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Radutzky</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Santarelli</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Verdirosi</surname>
<given-names>M. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Volterra</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zingarini</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1985</year>
).
<article-title>Italian Sign Language: general summary of research</article-title>
, in
<source>SLR'83, Sign Language Research</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Stokoe</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Volterra</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Silver Spring, MD</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Linstok Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>289</fpage>
<lpage>298</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B40">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Craig</surname>
<given-names>C. G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1977</year>
).
<source>The Structure of Jacaltec</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Austin, TX</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Texas Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B41">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Crasborn</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>How to recognize a clause when you see one</article-title>
.
<source>Sign. Lang. Linguist</source>
.
<volume>10</volume>
,
<fpage>103</fpage>
<lpage>111</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1075/sll.10.2.03cra</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B42">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Crasborn</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>van der Kooija</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Waters</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Woll</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mesch</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Frequency distribution and spreading behavior of different types of mouth actions in three sign languages</article-title>
.
<source>Sign Lang. Linguist</source>
.
<volume>11</volume>
,
<fpage>45</fpage>
<lpage>67</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1075/sll.11.1.04cra</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B43">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Crasborn</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zwitserlood</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>The Corpus NGT: an online corpus for professionals and laymen</article-title>
, in
<source>Construction and Exploitation of Sign Language Corpora. 3rd Workshop on the Representation and Processing of Sign Languages</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Crasborn</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hanke</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Efthimiou</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zwitserlood</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Thoutenhoofd</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Parijs</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>ELDA</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>44</fpage>
<lpage>49</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B44">
<mixed-citation publication-type="webpage">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cowie</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Innateness and language</article-title>
, in
<source>Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy</source>
. Available online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.science.uva.nl/~seop/archives/sum2010/entries/innateness-language">http://www.science.uva.nl/~seop/archives/sum2010/entries/innateness-language</ext-link>
(Accessed April 4, 2014).</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B45">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Crowley</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1992</year>
).
<source>An Introduction to Historical Linguistics</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B46">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Curtiss</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1977</year>
).
<source>Genie: A Psycholinguistic Study of a Modern-Day “Wild Child</source>
.”
<publisher-loc>Boston, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Academic Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B47">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Curtiss</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Krashen</surname>
<given-names>S. D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fromkin</surname>
<given-names>V. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rigler</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rigler</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1973</year>
).
<article-title>Language acquisition after the critical period: Genie as of April, 1973</article-title>
, in
<source>Papers from the Ninth Regional Meeting Chicago Linguistic Society</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Binnick</surname>
<given-names>R. I.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Chicago</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Chicago, Department of Linguistics</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>98</fpage>
<lpage>103</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B48">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cuxac</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<source>La Langue des Signes Française (LSF): les voies de l'iconicité</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Paris</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Ophrys</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B49">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Cuxac</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sallandre</surname>
<given-names>M-A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Iconicity and arbitrariness in French Sign Language: highly iconic structures, degenerated iconicity and grammatic iconicity</article-title>
, in
<source>Verbal and Signed languages: Comparing Structure, Constructs and Methodologies</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Pizzuto</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pietrandrea</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Simone</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Berlin</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>de Gruyter</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>13</fpage>
<lpage>33</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B50">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>DeGraff</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Against creole exceptionalism</article-title>
.
<source>Language</source>
<volume>79</volume>
,
<fpage>391</fpage>
<lpage>410</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1353/lan.2003.0114</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B51">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>DeGraff</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Morphology and word order in ‘creolization’and beyond</article-title>
, in
<source>The Oxford Handbook of Comparative Syntax</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Cinque</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kayne</surname>
<given-names>R. S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>293</fpage>
<lpage>372</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B52">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>De Langhe</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Guitteny</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Portine</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Retoré</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>A propos des structures OSV en langue des signes française</article-title>
.
<source>Silexicales</source>
<volume>4</volume>
,
<fpage>115</fpage>
<lpage>130</lpage>
Available online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.labri.fr/perso/retore/ARTICLES/OSV.rtf.pdf">http://www.labri.fr/perso/retore/ARTICLES/OSV.rtf.pdf</ext-link>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B53">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Delgado-Martins</surname>
<given-names>M. R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Coutinho</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Amaral</surname>
<given-names>M. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<source>Para uma Gramática da Língua Gestual Portuguesa</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Lisbon</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Editorial Caminho</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B54">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Delsing</surname>
<given-names>L.-O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>From OV to VO in Swedish</article-title>
, in
<source>Diachronic Syntax: Models and Mechanisms</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Pintzuk</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tsoulas</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Warner</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>255</fpage>
<lpage>274</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B55">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Desimone</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Duncan</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention</article-title>
.
<source>Annu. Rev. Neurosci</source>
.
<volume>18</volume>
,
<fpage>193</fpage>
<lpage>222</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">7605061</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B56">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>de Swart</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Negation in early L2: a window on language genesis</article-title>
, in
<source>Language Evolution: The View from Restricted Linguistic Systems</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Botha</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>de Swart</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Utrecht</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>LOT Publications</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>59</fpage>
<lpage>100</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B57">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Deuchar</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1983</year>
).
<article-title>Is BSL an SOV language?</article-title>
in
<source>Language in Sign: An International Perspective on Sign Language</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Kyle</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Woll</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Croom Helm</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>59</fpage>
<lpage>76</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B58">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Dryer</surname>
<given-names>M. S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
)
<article-title>Word order in Sino-Tibetan languages from a typological and geographical perspective</article-title>
, in
<source>The Sino-Tibetan Languages</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Thurgood</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>LaPolla</surname>
<given-names>R. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Routledge</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>43</fpage>
<lpage>55</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B59">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Dryer</surname>
<given-names>M. S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Order of subject, object and verb</article-title>
, in
<source>The World Atlas of Language Structures</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Haspelmath</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dryer</surname>
<given-names>M. S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gil</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Comrie</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>330</fpage>
<lpage>333</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B60">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Efthimiou</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fotinea</surname>
<given-names>S-E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>GSLC: creation and annotation of a Greek Sign Language corpus for HCI</article-title>
, in
<source>Proceedings of the 4th International Conference on Universal Access in Human Computer Interaction: Coping with diversity</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Stephanidis</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Berlin</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Springer Verlag</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>657</fpage>
<lpage>666</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B61">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Emmorey</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(ed.). (
<year>2003</year>
).
<source>Perspectives on Classifier Constructions in Sign Languages</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Mahwah, NJ</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Lawrence Erlbaum Associates</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B62">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Engberg-Pedersen</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>Some simultaneous constructions in Danish Sign Language</article-title>
, in
<source>Word-Order Issues in Sign Language</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Brennan</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Turner</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Durham</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>International Sign Linguistics Association</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>73</fpage>
<lpage>88</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B63">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Farris</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>Sign language research and Polish Sign Language</article-title>
.
<source>Lingua Posnaniensis</source>
<volume>36</volume>
,
<fpage>13</fpage>
<lpage>36</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16192405</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B64">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fedorenko</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Duncan</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kanwisher</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Language-selective and domain-general regions lie side by side within Broca's area</article-title>
.
<source>Curr. Biol</source>
.
<volume>22</volume>
,
<fpage>2059</fpage>
<lpage>2062</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.011</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23063434</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B65">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fischer</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Word-order Change as a Source of Grammaticalisation</article-title>
.
<publisher-loc>Amsterdam</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>John Benjamins</publisher-name>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1075/la.157</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B66">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fischer</surname>
<given-names>S. D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1975</year>
).
<article-title>Influences on word order change in American Sign Language</article-title>
, in
<source>Word Order and Word Order Change</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Li</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Austin, TX</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Texas Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>3</fpage>
<lpage>25</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B67">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fischer</surname>
<given-names>S. D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<article-title>The Head Parameter in ASL</article-title>
, in
<source>SLR'87: Papers from the Fourth International Symposium on Sign Language Research</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Edmondson</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Karlsson</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Hamburg</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Signum Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>75</fpage>
<lpage>85</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B68">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fischer</surname>
<given-names>S. D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Janis</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<article-title>Verb sandwiches in ASL</article-title>
, in
<source>Current Trends in European Sign Language Research: Proceedings of the 3rd European Congress on Sign Language Research, Hamburg, 1989</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Prillwitz</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Vollhaber</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Hamburg</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Signum Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>279</fpage>
<lpage>294</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B69">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Flack Potts</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Ambiguity avoidance as contrast preservation: case and word order freezing in Japanese</article-title>
, in
<source>UMass Occasional Papers in Linguistics 32: Papers in Optimality Theory III</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Bateman</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>O'Keefe</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Reilly</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Werle</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Amherst, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>BookSurge Publishing</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>57</fpage>
<lpage>89</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B70">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Friedman</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1976</year>
).
<article-title>The manifestation of subject, object, and topic in American Sign Language</article-title>
, in
<source>Subject and Topic</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Li</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Academic Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>125</fpage>
<lpage>148</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B71">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Fromkin</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Krashen</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Curtiss</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rigler</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rigler</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1974</year>
).
<article-title>The development of language in Genie: A case of language acquisition beyond the “critical period”</article-title>
.
<source>Brain Lang</source>
.
<volume>1</volume>
,
<fpage>81</fpage>
<lpage>107</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0093-934X(74)90027-3</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B72">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gallese</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gernsbacher</surname>
<given-names>M. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Heyes</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hickok</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Iacoboni</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Mirror neuron forum</article-title>
.
<source>Perspect. Psychol. Sci</source>
.
<volume>6</volume>
,
<fpage>369</fpage>
<lpage>407</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/1745691611413392</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">24183924</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B73">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gell-Mann</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ruhlen</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>The origin and evolution of word order</article-title>
.
<source>Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A</source>
.
<volume>108</volume>
,
<fpage>17290</fpage>
<lpage>17295</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1073/pnas.1113716108</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21987807</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B74">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gershkoff-Stowe</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Goldin-Meadow</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Is there a natural order for expressing semantic relations?</article-title>
<source>Cogn. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>45</volume>
,
<fpage>375</fpage>
<lpage>412</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0010-0285(02)00502-9</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12480479</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B75">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gibson</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fedorenko</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>The need for quantitative methods in syntax and semantics research</article-title>
.
<source>Lang. Cogn. Process</source>
.
<volume>28</volume>
,
<fpage>88</fpage>
<lpage>124</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/01690965.2010.515080</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20008661</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B76">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Gibson</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Piantadosi</surname>
<given-names>S. T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brink</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bergen</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lim</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Saxe</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>A noisy-channel account of crosslinguistic word order variation</article-title>
.
<source>Psychol. Sci</source>
.
<volume>24</volume>
,
<fpage>1079</fpage>
<lpage>1088</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1177/0956797612463705</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23649563</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B77">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Giles</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>The trouble with replication</article-title>
.
<source>Nat. Lang</source>
.
<volume>442</volume>
,
<fpage>344</fpage>
<lpage>347</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1038/442344a</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">16871184</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B78">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Givón</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1975</year>
).
<article-title>Serial verbs and syntactic change: Niger-Congo</article-title>
, in
<source>Word Order and Word Order Change</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Li</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Austin, TX</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Texas Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>47</fpage>
<lpage>112</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B79">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Givón</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1976</year>
).
<article-title>Topic, pronoun, and grammatical agreement</article-title>
, in
<source>Subject and Topic</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Li</surname>
<given-names>C. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Academic Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>149</fpage>
<lpage>188</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B80">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Givón</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1979</year>
).
<source>On Understanding Grammar</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Academic Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B81">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Glück</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pfau</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1998</year>
).
<article-title>On classifying classification as a class of inflection in German Sign Language</article-title>
, in
<source>Proceedings of ConSole 6</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Cambier-Langeveld</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lipták</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Redford</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Leiden</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>SOLE</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>59</fpage>
<lpage>74</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B82">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Goldberg</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<source>Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Chicago</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Chicago Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B83">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Goldberg</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Constructions: a new theoretical approach to language</article-title>
.
<source>Trends Cogn. Sci</source>
.
<volume>7</volume>
,
<fpage>219</fpage>
<lpage>224</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00080-9</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">12757824</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B84">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Goldin-Meadow</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1978</year>
).
<article-title>A study in human capacities</article-title>
.
<source>Science</source>
<volume>200</volume>
,
<fpage>649</fpage>
<lpage>651</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1126/science.200.4342.649</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17812701</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B85">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Goldin-Meadow</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<source>The Resilience of Language: What Gesture Creation in Deaf Children Can Tell us About How all Children Learn Language</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Psychology Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B86">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Goldin-Meadow</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>So</surname>
<given-names>W. C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Özyürek</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mylander</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>The natural order of events: how speakers of different languages represent events nonverbally</article-title>
.
<source>Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A</source>
.
<volume>105</volume>
,
<fpage>9163</fpage>
<lpage>9168</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1073/pnas.0710060105</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">18599445</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B87">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hale</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1975</year>
).
<article-title>Gaps in Grammar and Culture</article-title>
, in
<source>Linguistics and Anthropology: In Honor of C.F. Voegelin</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Kinkade</surname>
<given-names>M. D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hale</surname>
<given-names>K. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Werner</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Lisse</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>The Peter de Ridder Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>295</fpage>
<lpage>315</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B88">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hall</surname>
<given-names>M. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mayberry</surname>
<given-names>R. I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ferreira</surname>
<given-names>V. S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Cognitive constraints on constituent order: evidence from elicited pantomime</article-title>
.
<source>Cognition</source>
<volume>129</volume>
,
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>17</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.cognition.2013.05.004</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23792806</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B89">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hänel</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>The acquisition of agreement in DGS: early steps into spatially expressed syntax</article-title>
.
<source>Linguist. Ber</source>
.
<volume>13</volume>
,
<fpage>201</fpage>
<lpage>232</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B90">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hanke</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>König</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wagner</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Matthes</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>DGS corpus and dicta-sign: the Hamburg studio setup</article-title>
, in
<source>7th International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation. Workshop Proceedings. W13, 4th Workshop on the Representation and Processing of Sign Languages: Corpora and Sign Language Technologies</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Dreuw</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Paris</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>ELRA</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>106</fpage>
<lpage>109</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B91">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Happ</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Verköper</surname>
<given-names>M. O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<source>Deutsche Gebärdensprache: Ein Lehr-und arbeitsbuch</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Frankfurt</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Fachhochschulverlag</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B92">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Haviland</surname>
<given-names>J. B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Nouns, verbs, and constituents in an emerging ‘Tzotzil’ sign language</article-title>
, in
<source>Representing Language: Essays in Honor of Judith Aissen</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Gutiérrez-Bravo</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mikkelsen</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Potsdam</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Santa Cruz, CA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Linguistics Research Center</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>157</fpage>
<lpage>171</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B93">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Haywood</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pickering</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Branigan</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Do speakers avoid ambiguity during dialogue?</article-title>
<source>Psychol. Sci</source>
.
<volume>16</volume>
,
<fpage>362</fpage>
<lpage>366</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01541.x</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15869694</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B94">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Herring</surname>
<given-names>S. C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Paolillo</surname>
<given-names>J. C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>Focus position in SOV languages</article-title>
, in
<source>Word Order in Discourse</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Downing</surname>
<given-names>P. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Noonan</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Amsterdam</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>John Benjamins</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>163</fpage>
<lpage>198</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1075/tsl.30.08her</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B95">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hoekstra</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Mulder</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<article-title>Unergatives as copular verbs; locational and existential predication</article-title>
.
<source>Linguist. Rev</source>
.
<volume>7</volume>
,
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>80</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1515/tlir.1990.7.1.1</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B96">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hohenberger</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>The possible range of variation between sign languages: Universal Grammar, modality, and typological aspects</article-title>
, in
<source>Visible Variation</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Bisang</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hock</surname>
<given-names>H. H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Winter</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Berlin; New York</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Mouton de Gruyter</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>341</fpage>
<lpage>383</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B97">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Hróarsdóttir</surname>
<given-names>Þ.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<source>Word Order Change in Icelandic: From OV to VO</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Amsterdam</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>John Benjamins</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B98">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Jantunen</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Fixed and free: order of the verbal predicate and its core arguments in declarative transitive clauses in Finnish Sign Language</article-title>
.
<source>SKY J. Linguist</source>
.
<volume>21</volume>
,
<fpage>83</fpage>
<lpage>123</lpage>
Available online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.linguistics.fi/julkaisut/SKY2008/Jantunen_NETTIVERSIO.pdf">http://www.linguistics.fi/julkaisut/SKY2008/Jantunen_NETTIVERSIO.pdf</ext-link>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B99">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Jefwa</surname>
<given-names>G. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>Structural borrowing: the case of Kenyan Sign Language (KSL) and Kiswahili contact signing</article-title>
.
<source>J. Lang. Technol. Entrep. Afr</source>
.
<volume>1</volume>
,
<fpage>160</fpage>
<lpage>174</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.4314/jolte.v1i2.41781</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B100">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Johnson</surname>
<given-names>J. S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Newport</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1989</year>
).
<article-title>Critical period effects in second language learning: the influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>21</volume>
,
<fpage>60</fpage>
<lpage>99</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">2920538</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B101">
<mixed-citation publication-type="webpage">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Johnston</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<source>Corpus of Grammar and Discourse Strategies of Deaf Native Users of Auslan (Australian Sign Language), Endangered Languages Archive</source>
. London: SOAS, University of London. Available online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://elar.soas.ac.uk/node/3">http://elar.soas.ac.uk/node/3</ext-link>
(Accessed 2012).</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B102">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Johnston</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>From archive to corpus: transcription and annotation in the creation of signed language corpora</article-title>
.
<source>Int. J. Corpus Linguist</source>
.
<volume>15</volume>
,
<fpage>106</fpage>
<lpage>131</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1075/ijcl.15.1.05joh</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B103">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Johnston</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Formational and functional characteristics of pointing signs in a corpus of Auslan (Australian sign language): are the data sufficient to posit a grammatical class of ‘pronouns’ in Auslan?</article-title>
<source>Corpus Linguist. Linguist. Theory</source>
<volume>9</volume>
,
<fpage>109</fpage>
<lpage>159</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1515/cllt-2013-0012</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B104">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Johnston</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schembri</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007a</year>
).
<source>Australian Sign Language (Auslan): An Introduction to Sign Language Linguistics</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B105">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Johnston</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schembri</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007b</year>
).
<article-title>Testing language description through language documentation, archiving and corpus creation: the case of indicating verbs in the Auslan Archive Corpus</article-title>
, in
<source>Proceedings of Conference on Language Documentation and Linguistic Theory</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Austin</surname>
<given-names>P. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bond</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nathan</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>SOAS, University of London</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>145</fpage>
<lpage>154</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B106">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Johnston</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Vermeerbergen</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schembri</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Leeson</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>'Real data are messy': considering cross-linguistic analysis of constituent ordering in Auslan, VGT, and ISL</article-title>
, in
<source>Visible Variation: Comparative Studies on Sign Language Structure</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Perniss</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pfau</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Steinbach</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Berlin</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Mouton</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>163</fpage>
<lpage>206</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B107">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Julien</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<source>Syntactic Heads and Word Formation</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B108">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kegl</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<article-title>Predicate argument structure and verb-class organization in the ASL lexicon</article-title>
, in
<source>Sign Language Research: Theoretical Issues</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Lucas</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Washington, DC</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Gallaudet University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>149</fpage>
<lpage>175</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B109">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kemmerer</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>The cross-linguistic prevalence of SOV and SVO word orders reflects the sequential and hierarchical representation of action in Broca's Area</article-title>
.
<source>Lang. Linguist. Compass</source>
<volume>6</volume>
,
<fpage>50</fpage>
<lpage>66</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1002/lnc3.322</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B110">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Khanittanan</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
<collab>(a.k.a. Kanittanan)</collab>
</person-group>
(
<year>1986</year>
).
<article-title>Kamti Tai: from an SVO to an SOV language</article-title>
,
<source>South Asian Languages: Structure, Convergence and Diglossia</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Krishnamurti</surname>
<given-names>B. H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Delhi</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Motilal Banarsidass</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>174</fpage>
<lpage>178</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B111">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kimmelman</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Word order in Russian Sign Language</article-title>
.
<source>Sign Lang. Stud</source>
.
<volume>12</volume>
,
<fpage>414</fpage>
<lpage>445</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1353/sls.2012.0001</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B112">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Klima</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bellugi</surname>
<given-names>U.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1979</year>
).
<source>The Signs of Language</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Harvard University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B113">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Konrad</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Die lexikalische Struktur der Deutschen Gebärdensprache im Spiegel empirischer Fachgebärdenlexikographie</article-title>
.
<publisher-loc>Tübingen</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Narr Verlag</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B114">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Koutsoudas</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1969</year>
).
<source>Workbook in Syntax</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>McGraw Hill</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B115">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kristoffersen</surname>
<given-names>J. H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Existence, location and possession and the order of constituents in Danish Sign Language</article-title>
, in
<source>Cross-linguistic Perspectives in Sign language Research: Selected Papers from TISLF 2000</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Baker</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>van den Bogaerde</surname>
<given-names>B. E. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Crasborn</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Hamburg</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Signum</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>131</fpage>
<lpage>139</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B116">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Kuhn</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<source>Formal and Computational Aspects of Optimality-Theoretic Syntax</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Stuttgart</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Universität Stuttgart, Institut für maschinelle Sprachverarbeitung</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B117">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Langus</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nespor</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Cognitive systems struggling for word order</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>60</volume>
,
<fpage>291</fpage>
<lpage>318</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.01.004</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">20189553</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B118">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lee</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Markedness and word order freezing</article-title>
, in
<source>Formal and Empirical Issues in Optimality Theoretic Syntax</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Sells</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Stanford, CA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>CSLI</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>63</fpage>
<lpage>128</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B119">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Leeson</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Saeed</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Word order</article-title>
, in
<source>
<italic>Sign Language. An International Handbook (HSK—</italic>
Handbooks of
<italic>Linguistics and Communication Science</italic>
)</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Pfau</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Steinbach</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Woll</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Berlin</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Mouton de Gruyter</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>245</fpage>
<lpage>264</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B120">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Leibowitz</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1965</year>
).
<source>Visual Perception</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Macmillan</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B121">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Leuninger</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Mit den Augen lernen: Gebärdenspracherwerb</article-title>
, in
<source>Enzyklopädie der Psychologie</source>
, Vol. 3.
<italic>Sprachentwicklung</italic>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Grimm</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Göttingen</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Hogrefe</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>229</fpage>
<lpage>270</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B122">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Levy</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>A noisy-channel model of rational human sentence comprehension under uncertain input</article-title>
, in
<source>Proceedings of the Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing</source>
(
<publisher-loc>Stroudsburg, PA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Association for Computational Linguistics</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>234</fpage>
<lpage>243</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.3115/1613715.1613749</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B123">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Levy</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Jaeger</surname>
<given-names>T. F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Speakers optimize information density through syntactic reduction</article-title>
, in
<source>Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 19</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Schölkopf</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Platt</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hoffman</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Cambridge, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>MIT Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>849</fpage>
<lpage>856</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B124">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Liddell</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<article-title>Four functions of a locus: reexamining the structure of space in ASL</article-title>
, in
<source>Sign Language Research: Theoretical Issues</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Lucas</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Washington, DC</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Gallaudet University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>176</fpage>
<lpage>198</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B125">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Liddell</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Johnson</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1986</year>
).
<article-title>American Sign Language compound formation processes, lexicalization, and phonological remnants</article-title>
.
<source>Nat. Lang. Linguist. Theory</source>
<volume>4</volume>
,
<fpage>445</fpage>
<lpage>513</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/BF00134470</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B126">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lillo-Martin</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1986</year>
).
<article-title>Two kinds of null arguments in American Sign Language</article-title>
.
<source>Nat. Lang. Linguist. Theory</source>
<volume>4</volume>
,
<fpage>415</fpage>
<lpage>444</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/BF00134469</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B127">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lillo-Martin</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1991</year>
).
<source>Universal Grammar and American Sign Language: Setting the Null Argument Parameters</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Dordrecht</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Kluwer</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B128">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Lillo-Martin</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Berk</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Acquisition of constituent order under delayed language exposure</article-title>
, in
<source>Proceedings of the 27th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development</source>
, Vol. 2 (
<publisher-loc>Somerville, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cascadilla Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>484</fpage>
<lpage>495</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B129">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Massone</surname>
<given-names>M. I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Curiel</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Sign order in Argentine Sign Language</article-title>
.
<source>Sign Lang. Stud</source>
.
<volume>5</volume>
,
<fpage>63</fpage>
<lpage>93</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1353/sls.2004.0023</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B130">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Matsuoka</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1997</year>
).
<article-title>Verb raising in American Sign Language</article-title>
.
<source>Lingua</source>
<volume>103</volume>
,
<fpage>127</fpage>
<lpage>149</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/S0024-3841(97)00015-6</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B131">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>McDonald</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1985</year>
).
<article-title>Productive
<italic>on Sign</italic>
and frozen lexicon in ASL: an old problem revisited</article-title>
, in
<source>SLR'83: Proceedings of the Third International Symposium Language Research</source>
,
<italic>Rome, June 22–26, 1983</italic>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Stokoe</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Volterra</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Silver Spring, MD</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Linstok</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>254</fpage>
<lpage>259</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B132">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>McEnery</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wilson</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1996</year>
).
<source>Corpus Linguistics</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Edinburgh</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Edinburgh University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B133">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>McIntire</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1980</year>
).
<source>Locatives in American Sign Language</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Los Angeles, CA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of California</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B134">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>McKee</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kennedy</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>New Zealand Sign Language</article-title>
, in
<source>Languages of New Zealand</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Bell</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Harlow</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Starks</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Wellington, NZ</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Victoria University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>271</fpage>
<lpage>297</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B135">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>McWhorter</surname>
<given-names>J. H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(ed.). (
<year>2000</year>
).
<source>Language Change and Language Contact in Pidgins and Creoles</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Amsterdam</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>John Benjamins</publisher-name>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1075/cll.21</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B136">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Meier</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Cormier</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Quinto-Pozos</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<source>Modality and Structure in Signed and Spoken Languages</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B137">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Meir</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>Explaining backwards verbs in ISL: syntactic-semantic interaction</article-title>
, in
<source>Proceedings of the 4th European Congress on Sign Language Research</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Bos</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schermer</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Hamburg</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Signum</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>105</fpage>
<lpage>119</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B138">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Meir</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>A cross-modality perspective on verb agreement</article-title>
.
<source>Nat. Lang. Linguist. Theory</source>
<volume>20</volume>
,
<fpage>413</fpage>
<lpage>450</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1023/A:1015041113514</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B139">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Meir</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lifshitz</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ilkbasaran</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Padden</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010a</year>
).
<article-title>The interaction of animacy and word order in human languages: a study of strategies in a novel communication task</article-title>
, in
<source>Proceedings of the Eighth Evolution of Language Conference</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>A. D. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schouwstra</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>de Boer</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Singapore</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>World Scientific Publishing Co.</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>455</fpage>
<lpage>456</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B140">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Meir</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Padden</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Aronoff</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sandler</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Body as subject</article-title>
.
<source>J. Linguist</source>
.
<volume>43</volume>
,
<fpage>531</fpage>
<lpage>563</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1017/S0022226707004768</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">23066169</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B141">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Meir</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sandler</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<source>A Language in Space: The story of Israeli Sign Language</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Lawrence Erlbaum</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B142">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Meir</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sandler</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Padden</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Aronoff</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010b</year>
)
<article-title>Emerging sign languages</article-title>
, in
<source>Oxford Handbook of Deaf Studies, Language, and Education</source>
, Vol. 2, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Marschark</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Spencer</surname>
<given-names>P. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>267</fpage>
<lpage>280</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B143">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Meurant</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sinte</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Toward a corpus of French Belgian Sign Language (LSFB) discourses</article-title>
, in
<source>Across the Line of Speech and Writing Variation, Corpora and Language in Use –Proceedings 2</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Bolly</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Degand</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Louvain-la-Neuve</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Presses Universitaires de Louvain</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>199</fpage>
<lpage>212</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B144">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Miljan</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<source>The Noun Phrase in Estonian Sign Language from the Typological Perspective</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Tallinn</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Estonian Institute of Humanities</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B145">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Milkovic</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bradaric-Joncic</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wilbur</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Word order in Croatian Sign Language</article-title>
.
<source>Sign Lang. Linguist</source>
.
<volume>9</volume>
,
<fpage>169</fpage>
<lpage>206</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1075/sll.9.1-2.10mil</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17972196</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B146">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Milkovic</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bradaric-Joncic</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Wilbur</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Information status and word order in Croatian Sign Language</article-title>
.
<source>Clin. Linguist. Phon</source>
.
<volume>21</volume>
,
<fpage>1007</fpage>
<lpage>1017</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1080/02699200701659227</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">17972196</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B147">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Minoura</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Word order in Malagasy Sign Language</article-title>
.
<inline-graphic xlink:href="fpsyg-05-00376-i0001.jpg"></inline-graphic>
<source>(Journal of Tokyo University of Foreign Studies)</source>
<volume>77</volume>
,
<fpage>47</fpage>
<lpage>69</lpage>
Available online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://repository.tufs.ac.jp/bitstream/10108/70849/2/acs084009_ful.pdf">http://repository.tufs.ac.jp/bitstream/10108/70849/2/acs084009_ful.pdf</ext-link>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B148">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Morales-López</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Boldú-Menasanch</surname>
<given-names>R. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Alonso-Rodríguez</surname>
<given-names>J. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gras-Ferrer</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Rodríguez-González</surname>
<given-names>M. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>The verbal system of Catalan Sign Language</article-title>
.
<source>Sign Lang. Stud</source>
.
<volume>5</volume>
,
<fpage>441</fpage>
<lpage>496</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1353/sls.2005.0018</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B149">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Morales-López</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Reigosa-Varela</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bobillo-García</surname>
<given-names>N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<article-title>Word order and informative functions (topic and focus) in Spanish Signed Language utterances</article-title>
.
<source>J. Pragmat</source>
.
<volume>44</volume>
,
<fpage>474</fpage>
<lpage>489</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1016/j.pragma.2011.12.010</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B150">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Moravcsik</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1988</year>
).
<article-title>Agreement and markedness</article-title>
, in
<source>Agreement in Natural Language: Approaches, Theories, Descriptions</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Barlow</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Ferguson</surname>
<given-names>C. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Stanford, CA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>CSLI</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>89</fpage>
<lpage>106</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B151">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Nadeau</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Desouvrey</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>Word order in sentences with directional verbs in Quebec Sign Language</article-title>
, in
<source>Perspectives on Sign Language Structure. Papers from the Fifth International Symposium on Sign Language Research</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Ahlgren</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bergman</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Brennan</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Durham</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>International Sign Linguistics Association</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>149</fpage>
<lpage>158</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B152">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Nakanashi</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>The influence of Japanese word order on Japanese Sign Language</article-title>
, in
<source>Word-order Issues in Sign Language</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Brennan</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Turner</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Durham</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>International Sign Linguistics Association</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>171</fpage>
<lpage>192</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B153">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Napoli</surname>
<given-names>D. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sutton-Spence</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Limitations on simultaneity in sign language</article-title>
.
<source>Language</source>
<volume>86</volume>
,
<fpage>647</fpage>
<lpage>662</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1353/lan.2010.0018</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B154">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Napoli</surname>
<given-names>D. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sutton-Spence</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Sign language humor, human singularities, and the origins of language</article-title>
, in
<source>Deaf Around the World: The Impact of Language</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Mathur</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Napoli</surname>
<given-names>D. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>231</fpage>
<lpage>250</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B155">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Neidle</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kegl</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>MacLaughlin</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bahan</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lee</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<source>The Syntax of American Sign Language: Functional Categories and Hierarchical Structure</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>MIT Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B156">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Newmeyer</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000a</year>
).
<source>Language Form and Language Function</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>MIT Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B157">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Newmeyer</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000b</year>
).
<article-title>O”n the reconstruction of ‘proto-world’ word order</article-title>
, in
<source>The Evolutionary Emergence of Language: Social Function and the Origins of Linguistic Form</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Knight</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Studdert-Kennedy</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hurford</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>372</fpage>
<lpage>388</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1017/CBO9780511606441.022</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B158">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Newport</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<article-title>Maturational constraints on language learning</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Sci</source>
.
<volume>14</volume>
,
<fpage>11</fpage>
<lpage>28</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1207/s15516709cog1401_2</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B159">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Newport</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1991</year>
).
<article-title>Contrasting concepts of the critical period for language</article-title>
, in
<source>The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Carey</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gelman</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Lawrence Erlbaum</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>111</fpage>
<lpage>132</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B160">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Newport</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Bavelier</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Neville</surname>
<given-names>H. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Critical thinking about critical periods: perspectives on a critical period for language acquisition</article-title>
, in
<source>Language, Brain, and Cognitive Development: Essays in Honor of Jacques Mehler</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Dupoux</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Cambridge, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>MIT Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>481</fpage>
<lpage>502</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B161">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Nyst</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<source>A Descriptive Analysis of Adamorobe Sign Language</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Amsterdam</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Amsterdam</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B162">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ochs</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1979</year>
).
<article-title>Transcription as theory</article-title>
, in
<source>Developmental Pragmatics</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Ochs</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Schieffelin</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Academic Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>43</fpage>
<lpage>72</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B163">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Oviedo</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<source>Apuntes Para una Gramática de la Lengua de Señas Colombiana</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Santa Fe de Bogotá</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Instituto Nacional para Sordos and the Universidad del Valle</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B164">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Padden</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1988</year>
).
<source>Interaction of Morphology and Syntax in American Sign Language</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Garland Publishing</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B165">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Padden</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1990</year>
).
<article-title>The relation between space and grammar in ASL verb morphology</article-title>
, in
<source>Sign Language Research: Theoretical Issues</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Lucas</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Washington, DC</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Gallaudet University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>118</fpage>
<lpage>132</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B166">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Padden</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Meir</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sandler</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Aronoff</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Against all expectations: encoding subjects and objects in a new language</article-title>
, in
<source>Hypothesis A/Hypothesis B: Linguistic Explorations in Honor of David M. Perlmutter</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Gerdts</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Moore</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Polinsky</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Cambridge, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>MIT Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>383</fpage>
<lpage>400</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B167">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Pakhomova</surname>
<given-names>A. S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Interhemisphere asymmetry and the constancy of visual perception of small and large sizes</article-title>
.
<source>Hum. Physiol</source>
.
<volume>26</volume>
,
<fpage>275</fpage>
<lpage>280</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/BF02760187</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B168">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Parodi</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1995</year>
).
<article-title>Participle agreement and object shift in Old Spanish: a minimalist theory</article-title>
, in
<source>Evolution and Revolution in Linguistic Theory</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Campos</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Kempchinsky</surname>
<given-names>P. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Washington, DC</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Georgetown University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>276</fpage>
<lpage>301</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B169">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Petitto</surname>
<given-names>L. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zatorre</surname>
<given-names>R. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gauna</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Nikelski</surname>
<given-names>E. J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dostie</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Evans</surname>
<given-names>A. C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2000</year>
).
<article-title>Speech-like cerebral activity in profoundly deaf people processing signed languages: implications for the neural basis of human language</article-title>
.
<source>Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci</source>
.
<volume>97</volume>
,
<fpage>13961</fpage>
<lpage>13966</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1073/pnas.97.25.13961</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11106400</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B170">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Petronio</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1993</year>
).
<source>Clause Structure in American Sign Language</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Seattle, WA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Washington Ph.D. dissertation</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B171">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Plaza-Pust</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Why variation matters: on language contact in the development of L2 written German</article-title>
, in
<source>Sign Bilingualism: Language Development, Interaction, and Maintenance in Sign Language Contact Situations</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Plaza-Pust</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Morales-López</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Amsterdam</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>John Benjamins</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>73</fpage>
<lpage>135</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1075/sibil.38.07pla</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B172">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Plaza-Pust</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Weinmeister</surname>
<given-names>K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Bilingual acquisition of German Sign Language and written German: developmental asynchronies and language contact</article-title>
, in
<source>Sign Languages: Spinning and Unraveling the Past, Present and Future, TISLR9, Florianopolis, Brazil, December 2006</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>de Quadros</surname>
<given-names>R. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Petrópolis/Rio de Janeiro</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Editora Arara Azul</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>497</fpage>
<lpage>529</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B173">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Quadros</surname>
<given-names>R. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>de</surname>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Phrase structure of Brazilian Sign Language</article-title>
, in
<source>Cross-linguistic Perspectives in Sign Language Research: Selected Papers from TISLF 2000</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Baker</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>van den Bogaerde</surname>
<given-names>B. E. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Crasborn</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Hamburg</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Signum</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>141</fpage>
<lpage>161</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B174">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Quadros</surname>
<given-names>R. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>de</surname>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lillo-Martin</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Clause structure</article-title>
, in
<source>Sign Languages</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Brentari</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>225</fpage>
<lpage>251</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B175">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Quadros</surname>
<given-names>R. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>de</surname>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Quer</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>Back to back(wards) and moving on: on agreement, auxiliaries and verb classes in sign language</article-title>
, in
<source>Sign Languages: Spinning and Unraveling the Past, Present and Future, TISLR9, Florianopolis, Brazil, December 2006</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>de Quadros</surname>
<given-names>R. M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Petrópolis/Rio de Janeiro</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Editora Arara Azul</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>530</fpage>
<lpage>551</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B176">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Quinto</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<source>Word Order in Mexican Sign Language</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Austin, TX</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>University of Texas at Austin. Available by writing to the author</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B177">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Raney</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1984</year>
).
<article-title>VSOand SVO Order in Welsh and Breton</article-title>
in
<source>Papers and Studies in Contrastive Linguistics</source>
, Vol. 18, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Torbus</surname>
<given-names>A. K.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Okolice</surname>
<given-names>W. Z.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Poznan</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Adam Mickiewicz Univ</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>47</fpage>
<lpage>54</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B178">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Roberge</surname>
<given-names>P. T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>The creation of pidgins as a possible window on language evolution</article-title>
, in
<source>Language Evolution: The View from Restricted Linguistic Systems</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Botha</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>de Swart</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Utrecht</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>LOT Publications</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>101</fpage>
<lpage>138</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B179">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rosenbaum</surname>
<given-names>D. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<source>Human Motor Control</source>
.
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Academic Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B180">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rosenbaum</surname>
<given-names>D. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Carlson</surname>
<given-names>R. A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gilmore</surname>
<given-names>R. O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Acquisition of intellectual and perceptual-motor skills</article-title>
.
<source>Annu. Rev. Psychol</source>
.
<volume>52</volume>
,
<fpage>453</fpage>
<lpage>470</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.453</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">11148313</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B181">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Rosenstein</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2004</year>
).
<article-title>Topics in ISL (Israeli Sign Language) are not topicalized</article-title>
, in
<source>Poster Presentation at the 8th International Conference on Theoretical Issues in Sign Language Research (TISLR)</source>
(
<publisher-loc>Barcelona</publisher-loc>
). Available online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.ub.edu/ling/tislr8/Rosenstein.doc">http://www.ub.edu/ling/tislr8/Rosenstein.doc</ext-link>
(Accessed October 27, 2013).</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B182">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Ross</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<article-title>Contact-induced change in Oceanic languages in North-West Melanesia</article-title>
, in
<source>Areal Diffusion and Genetic Inheritance: Problems in Comparative Linguistics</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Aikhenvald</surname>
<given-names>A. Y.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Dixon</surname>
<given-names>R. M. W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>134</fpage>
<lpage>66</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B183">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Russo</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>A crosslinguistic, cross-cultural analysis of metaphors in two Italian Sign Language (LIS) registers</article-title>
.
<source>Sign Lang. Stud</source>
.
<volume>5</volume>
,
<fpage>333</fpage>
<lpage>359</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1353/sls.2005.0009</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B184">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sallandre</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Simultaneity in French Sign Language discourse</article-title>
, in
<source>Simultaneity in Signed Languages: Form and Function</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Vermeerbergen</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Leeson</surname>
<given-names>L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Crasborn</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Sandler</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lillo-Martin</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Amsterdam</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>John Benjamins</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>103</fpage>
<lpage>126</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B185">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sandler</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Meir</surname>
<given-names>I.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Padden</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Aronoff</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>The emergence of grammar: systematic structure in a new language</article-title>
.
<source>Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A</source>
.
<volume>102</volume>
,
<fpage>2661</fpage>
<lpage>2665</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1073/pnas.0405448102</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15699343</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B186">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schembri</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>British Sign Language corpus project: open access archives and the observer's paradox</article-title>
, in Sixth
<source>International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation, May 26-June 1, 2008</source>
(
<publisher-loc>Marrakech</publisher-loc>
). Available online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.lrec-conf.org/proceedings/lrec2008/">http://www.lrec-conf.org/proceedings/lrec2008/</ext-link>
(Accessed October 26, 2013).</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B187">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schouwstra</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2012</year>
).
<source>Semantic Structures, Communicative Strategies, and the Emergence of Language</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Utrecht</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>LOT</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B188">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Schuit</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Baker</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pfau</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>Inuit Sign Language: a contribution to sign language typology</article-title>
.
<source>Linguist. Amst</source>
.
<volume>4</volume>
,
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>31</lpage>
Available online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.linguisticsinamsterdam.nl/04/nr01/a01">http://www.linguisticsinamsterdam.nl/04/nr01/a01</ext-link>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B189">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Senghas</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Coppola</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Newport</surname>
<given-names>E. L.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Supalla</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1997</year>
).
<article-title>Argument structure in Nicaraguan Sign Language: the emergence of grammatical devices</article-title>
, in
<source>Proceedings of the Boston University Conference on Language Development</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Hughes</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Hughes</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Greenhill</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Boston, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cascadilla Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>550</fpage>
<lpage>561</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B190">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Shannon</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1948</year>
).
<article-title>A mathematical theory of communication</article-title>
.
<source>Bell Syst. Tech. J</source>
.
<volume>27</volume>
,
<fpage>623</fpage>
<lpage>656</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1002/j.1538-7305.1948.tb00917.x</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B191">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Slobin</surname>
<given-names>D. I.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Issues of linguistic typology in the study of sign language development of deaf children</article-title>
, in
<source>Advances in the Sign Language Development of Deaf Children</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Schick</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Marschark</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Spencer</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>20</fpage>
<lpage>45</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B192">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Smith</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Taiwan Sign Language research: an historical overview</article-title>
.
<source>Lang. Linguist</source>
.
<volume>6</volume>
,
<fpage>187</fpage>
<lpage>215</lpage>
Available online at:
<ext-link ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="http://www.ling.sinica.edu.tw/files/publication/j2005_2_02_5817.pdf">http://www.ling.sinica.edu.tw/files/publication/j2005_2_02_5817.pdf</ext-link>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B193">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>So</surname>
<given-names>W. C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Coppola</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Licciardello</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Goldin-Meadow</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>The seeds of spatial grammar in the manual modality</article-title>
.
<source>Cogn. Sci</source>
.
<volume>29</volume>
,
<fpage>1029</fpage>
<lpage>1043</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1207/s15516709cog0000_38</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21702801</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B194">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sprouse</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>A test of the cognitive assumptions of magnitude estimation: commutativity does not hold for acceptability judgments</article-title>
.
<source>Language</source>
<volume>87</volume>
,
<fpage>274</fpage>
<lpage>288</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1353/lan.2011.0028</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B195">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sundquist</surname>
<given-names>J. D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Syntactic variation in the history of Norwegian and the decline of XV word order</article-title>
.
<source>Diachronica</source>
<volume>23</volume>
,
<fpage>105</fpage>
<lpage>141</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1075/dia.23.1.06sun</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B196">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sutton-Spence</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Woll</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1999</year>
).
<source>The Linguistics of British Sign Language: An Introduction</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B197">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Sze</surname>
<given-names>Y. B. F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Word order of Hong Kong Sign Language</article-title>
, in
<source>Cross-linguistic Perspectives in Sign Language Research: Selected Papers from TISLF 2000</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Baker</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>van den Bogaerde</surname>
<given-names>B. E. M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Crasborn</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Hamburg</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Signum</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>163</fpage>
<lpage>191</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B198">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tallerman</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1998</year>
).
<article-title>Word order in Celtic</article-title>
, in
<source>Constituent Order in the Languages of Europe</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Siewierska</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>The Hague</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Mouton de Gruyter</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>21</fpage>
<lpage>46</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B199">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Taub</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2001</year>
).
<source>Language from the Body: Iconicity and Metaphor in American Sign Language</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1017/CBO9780511509629</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B200">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Taylor</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1994</year>
).
<article-title>The change from SOV to SVO in Ancient Greek</article-title>
.
<source>Lang. Var. Change</source>
<volume>6</volume>
,
<fpage>1</fpage>
<lpage>37</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1017/S0954394500001563</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">487727</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B201">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tettamanti</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Buccino</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Saccuman</surname>
<given-names>M. C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Gallese</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Danna</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Scifo</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<etal></etal>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits</article-title>
.
<source>J. Cogn. Neurosci</source>
.
<volume>17</volume>
,
<fpage>273</fpage>
<lpage>281</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1162/0898929053124965</pub-id>
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">15811239</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B202">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Timm</surname>
<given-names>L. A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1989</year>
).
<article-title>Word order in 20th century Breton</article-title>
.
<source>Nat. Lang. Linguist. Theory</source>
<volume>7</volume>
,
<fpage>361</fpage>
<lpage>378</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/BF00208101</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B203">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tomlin</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1986</year>
).
<source>Basic Word Order. Functional Principles</source>
.
<publisher-loc>London</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Croom Helm</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B204">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tsay</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Myers</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2009</year>
).
<article-title>The morphology and phonology of Taiwan Sign Language</article-title>
, in
<source>Taiwan Sign Language and Beyond</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Tai</surname>
<given-names>J. H.-Y.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Tsay</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Taipei</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Taiwan Institute for the Humanities, National Chung Cheng University</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>83</fpage>
<lpage>130</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B205">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Tomasello</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<source>Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Cambridge, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Harvard University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B206">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Van Gelderen</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<source>The Linguistic Cycle: Language Change and the Language Faculty</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B207">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Varin</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1979</year>
).
<article-title>VSO and SVO order in Breton</article-title>
.
<source>Arch. Linguist. Leeds</source>
<volume>10</volume>
,
<fpage>83</fpage>
<lpage>101</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B208">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Vermeerbergen</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1996</year>
).
<article-title>ROOD KOOL TIEN PERSOON IN. Morfo-syntactische Aspecten van Gebarentaal. [RED CABBAGE TEN PERSON IN. Morpho-syntactic Aspects of Sign Language, Where that Flemish Sign Sentence Means ‘That (big) Red Cabbage Can feed 10 People’.]</article-title>
<publisher-loc>Brussels</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Vrije Universiteit Brussel Dissertation</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B209">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Vermeerbergen</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Van Herreweghe</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Akach</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007a</year>
).
<article-title>Word order issues in South African Sign Language and Flemish Sign Language</article-title>
, in
<source>Visible Variation: Comparative Studies in Sign Language Structure</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Perniss</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Pfau</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Steinbach</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Berlin</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Mouton de Gruyter</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>163</fpage>
<lpage>205</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B210">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Vermeerbergen</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Van Herreweghe</surname>
<given-names>M.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Akach</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Matabane</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007b</year>
).
<article-title>Constituent order in Flemish Sign Language (VGT) and South African Sign Language (SASL): a cross-linguistic study</article-title>
.
<source>Sign Lang. Linguist</source>
.
<volume>10</volume>
,
<fpage>25</fpage>
<lpage>54</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1075/sll.10.1.04ver</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B211">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Volterra</surname>
<given-names>V.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Laudanna</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Corazza</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Radutzky</surname>
<given-names>E.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Natale</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1984</year>
).
<article-title>Italian Sign Language: the order of elements in the declarative sentence</article-title>
, in
<source>Recent Research on European Sign Language</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Loncke</surname>
<given-names>F.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Boyes Braem</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Lebrun</surname>
<given-names>Y.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Lisse</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Swets and Zeitlinger BV</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>19</fpage>
<lpage>46</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B212">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Vulanoviæ</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2005</year>
).
<article-title>The combinatorics of cases and word order</article-title>
.
<source>Res. Lang. Comput</source>
.
<volume>3</volume>
,
<fpage>107</fpage>
<lpage>129</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1007/s11168-005-1289-x</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B213">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Washabaugh</surname>
<given-names>W.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Woodward</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>De Santis</surname>
<given-names>S.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1978</year>
).
<article-title>Providence Island Sign Language: a context-dependent language</article-title>
.
<source>Anthropol. Linguist</source>
.
<volume>20</volume>
,
<fpage>95</fpage>
<lpage>109</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B214">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Weskott</surname>
<given-names>T.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Fanselow</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2011</year>
).
<article-title>On the informativity of different measures of linguistic acceptability</article-title>
.
<source>Language</source>
<volume>87</volume>
,
<fpage>249</fpage>
<lpage>273</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1353/lan.2011.0041</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B215">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wilbur</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1987</year>
).
<source>American Sign Language: Linguistic and Applied Dimensions, 2nd Edn</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Boston, MA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Little Brown</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B216">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wilbur</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2002</year>
).
<article-title>Phrase structure in ASL and ÖGS</article-title>
, in
<source>Progress in Sign Language Research in Honor of Sigmund Prillwitz</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Schulmeister</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Reinitzer</surname>
<given-names>H.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Hamburg</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Signum</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>235</fpage>
<lpage>247</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B217">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wilbur</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Representations of telicity in ASL</article-title>
.
<source>Chic. Linguist. Soc</source>
.
<volume>39</volume>
,
<fpage>354</fpage>
<lpage>368</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B218">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Willis</surname>
<given-names>D. W. E.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1998</year>
).
<source>Syntactic Change in Welsh: A Study of the Loss of the Verb-Second</source>
.
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B219">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wojda</surname>
<given-names>P.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Transmission of Polish sign systems</article-title>
, in
<source>Sign Languages</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Brentari</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Cambridge</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Cambridge University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>131</fpage>
<lpage>148</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B220">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Woll</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Modality, universality and the similarities among sign languages: an historical perspective</article-title>
, in
<source>Cross-linguistic Perspectives in Sign Language Research: Selected Papers from TISLR 2000</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Baker</surname>
<given-names>A.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>van den Bogaerde</surname>
<given-names>B.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Crasborn</surname>
<given-names>O.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Hamburg</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Signum</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>17</fpage>
<lpage>27</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B221">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wood</surname>
<given-names>S. K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Degrees of resiliency in acquisition of language</article-title>
.
<source>Nanzan Linguistics; Special Issue</source>
<volume>3</volume>
,
<fpage>1</fpage>
, 315-330.
<pub-id pub-id-type="pmid">21058578</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B222">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Wood</surname>
<given-names>S. K.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2010</year>
).
<article-title>Acquisition of topicalization in very late learners of Libras</article-title>
, in
<source>Deaf Around the World: The Impact of Language</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Mathur</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Napoli</surname>
<given-names>D. J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Oxford</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Oxford University Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>164</fpage>
<lpage>183</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B223">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Woodward</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1980</year>
).
<article-title>Sociolinguistic research on ASL: an historical perspective</article-title>
, in
<source>Sign Language and the Deaf Community: Essays in Honor of William Stokoe</source>
, eds
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Baker</surname>
<given-names>C.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Battison</surname>
<given-names>R.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Silver Spring, MD</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Linstok Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>117</fpage>
<lpage>134</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B224">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Woodward</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>1987</year>
).
<article-title>Sign languages—providence Island</article-title>
,
<source>Gallaudet Encyclopedia of Deaf People and Deafness</source>
, Vol. 3, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Van Cleve</surname>
<given-names>J. V.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>New York, NY</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>McGraw-Hill</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>103</fpage>
<lpage>104</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B225">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Yau</surname>
<given-names>S. C.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2008</year>
).
<article-title>The role of visual space in sign language development</article-title>
, in
<source>Space in Languages of China</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Xu</surname>
<given-names>D.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Dordrecht</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Springer Verlag</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>143</fpage>
<lpage>174</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B226">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Zeshan</surname>
<given-names>U.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2003</year>
).
<article-title>Indo-Pakistani Sign Language grammar: a typological outline</article-title>
.
<source>Sign Lang. Stud</source>
.
<volume>3</volume>
,
<fpage>157</fpage>
<lpage>212</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1353/sls.2003.0005</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B227">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Zeshan</surname>
<given-names>U.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2006</year>
).
<article-title>Negative and interrogative structures in Turkish Sign Language (TID)</article-title>
, in
<source>Interrogative and Negative Constructions in Sign Languages</source>
, ed
<person-group person-group-type="editor">
<name>
<surname>Zeshan</surname>
<given-names>U.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<publisher-loc>Nijmegen</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>Ishara Press</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>128</fpage>
<lpage>164</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B228">
<mixed-citation publication-type="journal">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Zhang</surname>
<given-names>N. N.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2007</year>
).
<article-title>Universal 20 and Taiwan Sign Language</article-title>
.
<source>Sign Lang. Linguist</source>
.
<volume>10</volume>
,
<fpage>55</fpage>
<lpage>81</lpage>
<pub-id pub-id-type="doi">10.1075/sll.10.1.05zha</pub-id>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
<ref id="B229">
<mixed-citation publication-type="book">
<person-group person-group-type="author">
<name>
<surname>Zhang</surname>
<given-names>X.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Han</surname>
<given-names>J.</given-names>
</name>
<name>
<surname>Zhao</surname>
<given-names>G.</given-names>
</name>
</person-group>
(
<year>2013</year>
).
<article-title>Research on the construction of dynamic chinese natural sign language corpus</article-title>
, in
<source>Mobile Ad-hoc and Sensor Networks (MSN), 2013 IEEE Ninth International Conference on</source>
(
<publisher-loc>Piscataway, NJ; Los Alamitos, CA</publisher-loc>
:
<publisher-name>The Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers, Inc.</publisher-name>
),
<fpage>502</fpage>
<lpage>505</lpage>
</mixed-citation>
</ref>
</ref-list>
</back>
</pmc>
</record>

Pour manipuler ce document sous Unix (Dilib)

EXPLOR_STEP=$WICRI_ROOT/Wicri/Linguistique/explor/TamazightV2/Data/Pmc/Corpus
HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_STEP/biblio.hfd -nk 000186  | SxmlIndent | more

Ou

HfdSelect -h $EXPLOR_AREA/Data/Pmc/Corpus/biblio.hfd -nk 000186  | SxmlIndent | more

Pour mettre un lien sur cette page dans le réseau Wicri

{{Explor lien
   |wiki=    Wicri/Linguistique
   |area=    TamazightV2
   |flux=    Pmc
   |étape=   Corpus
   |type=    RBID
   |clé=     
   |texte=   
}}

Wicri

This area was generated with Dilib version V0.6.33.
Data generation: Wed Nov 15 18:28:35 2017. Site generation: Sat Feb 10 16:46:27 2024